[1]415-2569-2-CE


Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).    ascilite 
 

 762 

Bricks or clicks? Predicting student intentions in a blended 
learning buffet 
 
Michelle Hood 
Griffith Health Institute and School of Applied Psychology, Griffith University 
 

This study examined predictors of students' intentions to access face-to-face (f2f) or online 
options for lectures and tutorials in a buffet-style blended learning 2nd-year psychology 
statistics course (N = 113; 84% female). Students were aged 18 to 51 years (M = 23.16; SD 
= 6.80). Practical and technological predictors, along with attitudinal and motivational 
factors drawn from the expectancy value model, were tested. Higher work commitments, 
greater reliance on rehearsal, higher self-regulation, and higher critical thinking were the 
most important predictors of intentions to use online lectures. Almost 40% of the variance 
in those intentions was explained. Having the required computer software was the only 
independent predictor of intentions to attend synchronous online tutorials. Overall, 10% of 
the variance in those intentions was explained. Intentions to access asynchronous (archived) 
online tutorials were uniquely predicted by lower ability and higher extrinsic motivation. 
Overall, 26% of the variance in those intentions was explained. The predictors did not 
explain significant variance in intentions to attend f2f lectures or tutorials. These findings 
contribute to understanding how students go about making choices when faced with buffet 
style blended learning courses. Motivational and practical factors both influence the choices 
students make.  
 

Introduction 
 

When blended learning is well understood and implemented, higher education will be 
transformed in a way not seen since the expansion of higher education in the late 1940s 
(Garrison & Vaughan, 2008, p. 10) 

 
Blended learning refers to integrating technology or media with traditional face-to-face (f2f) learning 
activities in a planned and pedagogically sound manner (Picciano, 2009). Bleed (2001) called this a "mix 
of bricks and clicks" (p. 24). Blended learning produces greater student engagement, achievement, and 
satisfaction than traditional or purely online approaches, although there are mixed results (Chen, Lambert, 
& Guidry, 2010; Means, Toyama, Murphy, Bakia, & Jones, 2009). The mixed results may arise from the 
wide range of approaches to blended learning currently employed and the lack of understanding about 
how student characteristics influence what works best for a given individual.  
 
Twigg (2003) described five broad approaches to blended learning. These range from the supplemental 
approach, in which online activities supplement the largely unchanged traditional f2f teaching, to fully 
online approaches, in which online activities replace all f2f activities. The buffet approach offers students 
a choice of options customised to their background, learning preferences, and goals. This approach does 
not treat students as if they were all the same and would all prefer and benefit from the one pre-
determined way of learning. The current study examined how individual differences influence student 
intentions to access online versus f2f options in a buffet-style blended learning statistics course.  
 
Most of the published research on what format students choose in buffet-style courses has focused on 
practical and technological predictors. Williams and Fardon (2007) reported that Australian university 
students chose recorded over f2f lectures because of timetable clashes (47%) and outside commitments 
(43%). Matheos, Daniel, and McCalla (2005) found that having internet connectivity at home was 
associated with higher access of online resources in a buffet-style computer science course. Learning style 
was also important. Independent learners preferred technology-based support, whereas collaborative 
learners preferred f2f interaction. Huang, Lin, and Huang (2011) also found that learning style affected 
online participation in a blended learning software usage course. Having a higher sensory learning style 
was associated with greater online participation. Sensory learners are more practical and prefer learning 
facts and solving problems via well-established methods. Students' study skills and learning styles are 
initially assessed to inform their choices from the buffet offered in Ohio State University's introductory 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 763 

statistics courses (Program in Course Redesign, 2005); however, published details on the relationships 
between these factors and the choices made are not available. 
 
Studies that have examined student choices between fully online or fully f2f formats have also identified 
predictors that might apply to choices made in a buffet-style blended course. Rose, Frisby, Hamlin, and 
Jones (2000) found that practical considerations (distance to campus and competing time commitments) 
and attitudinal/motivational factors (desire for a new challenge and lower self-efficacy for the course) 
predicted choice of the online option in a graduate nursing epidemiology course. By comparison, not 
owning a computer, low computer self-efficacy, and preference for f2f discussion and interaction 
predicted the f2f choice. Artino (2010) found that higher self-efficacy for learning online and lower value 
for the task predicted stronger preference for fully online delivery of an aviation course. Thus, student 
motivational beliefs are important in determining their choices.  
 
Several studies have used the technology acceptance model (TAM, Davis, Bagozzi &Warshaw, 1989) to 
understand how perceptions and attitudes influence students' or workers' behavioural intentions to engage 
in online learning (e.g., Chang & Tung, 2008; Cheng, 2011; Tselios, Daskalakis, & Papadopoulou, 2011). 
Perceptions of usefulness, ease of use of the online activities, and system quality, along with computer 
self-efficacy, predict behavioural intentions, although results are mixed (see Tselios et al., 2011). Cheng 
(2011) extended this model to show that intrinsic motivation predicted perceived enjoyment of online 
learning, which, in turn, predicted intentions to engage with it. Behavioural intentions strongly predict 
actual usage, which, in turn, strongly predicts perceived performance (Cheng, 2011).  
 
TAM developed from the expectancy value model (EVM, Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). According to the 
EVM, behaviour and behavioural intentions are functions of expectancies (beliefs) that particular 
outcomes will result and values (affective evaluations) associated with those outcomes (Palmgreen, 
1984). When confronted with a choice of behaviours, the option that provides the greatest outcomes in 
terms of combined expectancies and values is chosen. Many studies focus on behavioural intentions, 
because intentions are the strongest predictors of behaviours (Ajzen, 2002). The current study used 
Eccles's broader EVM of achievement (e.g., Eccles, 2005; Eccles et al., 1983), which incorporates 
demographic factors, past experiences, affective reactions, and goals, as well as values and expectancies, 
as predictors of achievement-related behaviours. The aim was to explain student intentions to use online 
or f2f options in a blended learning buffet that was offered in a 2nd-year compulsory research methods 
and statistics course in an Australian undergraduate psychology degree.  
 
The existing research on blended learning approaches in statistics teaching has generally described the 
approaches used and/or reported their effectiveness on student satisfaction, engagement, or performance 
(e.g., Baharun & Porter, 2009; Neumann & Hood, 2009; Neumann, Neumann, & Hood, 2011; Schober, 
Wagner, Reimann, Atria, & Spiel, 2006; Tudor, 2006; Utts, Sommer, Acredolo, Maher, & Matthews, 
2003; Ward, 2004; Yablon & Katz, 2001). However, Utts et al. (2003) examined predictors of student 
choice of a blended learning or f2f version of the same introductory statistics course. They found similar 
practical (timetabling) and technological (desire to improve computer skills) predictors to those identified 
by Rose et al. (2000) in graduate nursing students. There was some evidence that lower value (course not 
a part of their major) and a deeper approach to learning (belief that it would expand their knowledge and 
critical/analytic skills) predicted the blended learning choice. However, to date no studies have examined 
student choices in a buffet-style blended learning course.  
 
Statement of the problem 
 
To date, little research has been conducted on factors that influence whether students intend to choose f2f 
versus online options in a buffet-style blended learning environment, especially in statistics courses. This 
knowledge will enable educators to better tailor blends to student characteristics. In the current study, 
students were offered access to lectures f2f or asynchronously via recordings available to download or 
stream. Similarly, tutorials could be attended f2f, virtually in real-time (synchronous), or via archived 
recordings (asynchronous) available to download or stream. The study extended on previous research 
which has mainly examined practical and technological predictors by also examining attitudinal and 
motivational factors drawn from the EVM. Practical and technological predictors were outside 
commitments (work, family), Internet and software resources, and distance to campus. Attitudinal and 
motivational predictors based on the EVM were values (e.g., attitudes toward statistics), affective 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 764 

reactions (statistics anxiety), expectancies (statistics and learning self-efficacy), extrinsic and intrinsic 
motivation, learning strategies, and ability (past statistics performance). The main research questions were 
whether these practical, technological, attitudinal, and motivational factors would predict student 
intentions to use either f2f or online access, and, if so, what were the most important predictors of each 
mode of delivery.  
 
Method 
 
The author's university ethics committee approved this study. Participation in completing the pen-and-
paper questionnaire in the first lecture of the course was voluntary. Selection of attendance method was 
also voluntary.  
 
Description of the course 
 
This is a compulsory course for students enrolled in the 2nd year of psychology degrees and is the third 
compulsory research methods and statistics course in their degree. This course teaches experimental and 
correlational research design and analysis. Conceptual and theoretical content is covered in lectures. In 
tutorials, students gain practical experience in designing studies, conducting and interpreting analyses 
using Statistics Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS ©), and report writing skills (American 
Psychological Association [APA], 2009). Assessment consists of mid-semester and final exams and an 
APA style (2009) research report based on original data collected in the course.  
 
Description of the buffet-style blended learning approach 
 
Students had choices of both online and f2f options for the weekly lectures and tutorials. Face-to-face 
lectures (delivered by the author) were captured (audio of the instructor with visual of the PowerPoint 
presentation) using Lectopia (http://www.lectopia.com.au/). Captured lectures were accessed via the 
Blackboard (http://www.blackboard.com/) learning and teaching system used for online course support. 
Students could stream audio only or audio-visual using Flash or QuickTime or could download audio 
only as an MP3 file or audio-visual for PC/laptop/iPod.  
 
For tutorials, students could attend the f2f version or synchronous or asynchronous virtual versions. The 
author used Wimba Classroom (http://www.wimba.com/products/wimba_classroom) to deliver a virtual 
tutorial. Wimba Classroom enables live audio (via microphone) and visual (of participants via webcams 
and of the presenter's desktop) communication in real-time. All activities and facilities possible in a f2f 
classroom (e.g., projecting the presenters desktop to demonstrate analyses, using a whiteboard, having a 
student present, interaction, breakout groups) are replicated in the Wimba classroom. These synchronous 
virtual tutorials were also archived (captured and accessed in a similar fashion to the captured lectures), 
providing a third asynchronous online option for tutorials.  
 
Participants 
 
Participants were 113 undergraduate psychology students (15.9% male, 84.1% female) enrolled in the 
participating course at a large urban university in Queensland, Australia. This represented 69.75% of 
students in the course. Mean age was 23.16 years (SD = 6.80; range = 18 - 51; 2 participants did not 
report age). Most were domestic students (96.5%). Therefore, English was the main language for 92.9% 
of the participants. Most participants were enrolled in a single psychology degree (92.9%), with the 
remainder enrolled in double degrees with psychology and law (2.7%), exercise science (0.9%), human 
resource management (2.7%), and criminology (0.9%). The majority were full-time students, completing 
three (22.1%), four (65.5%), or five courses (4.5%). The remainder were part-time, completing one 
(4.5%) or two courses (2.7%). A few (7.1%) were repeating the course because they had previously failed 
it (3.55%) or to improve their grade (3.55%).  
 
Only six participants reported not having internet connectivity at home. Of those with internet access, 
only three participants reported dial-up access, with the remainder having broadband or satellite access 
(10 participants did not answer this). The SPSS software was available at home for 34.5% of the 
participants (one did not answer this).  
 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 765 

Measures 
 
Participants reported age, gender, student status (domestic/international), main language, degree program, 
enrolment load, whether they were repeating the course, distance to campus, and outside commitments 
(work/volunteer or caring). They also reported their grades (from fail = 2 to high distinction = 7) in the 
two prerequisite research methods and statistics courses. Questions also asked about their internet 
connectivity at home (and, if so, what sort) and availability of SPSS (essential in the tutorials).  
 
Participants indicated their intentions to access the online and f2f options available in the course using a 
visual analogue scale, anchored by never (1) and weekly (10) with the midpoint indicated as about half 
the semester (5). They also completed measures of statistics attitudes, anxiety, and self-efficacy, 
motivation, and learning strategies. 
 
The Statistics Anxiety Rating scale (STARS; Cruise & Wilkins, 1980) is a 51-item scale that assesses 
statistics anxiety and attitudes. The wording of some items was modified to reflect the Australian context 
(e.g., "professor" was changed to "lecturer"). On the first 23 items, students rate how anxious they feel in 
statistical situations, using a 5-point Likert scale from 1 = no anxiety to 5 = considerable anxiety. This 
yields three composite subscale scores; test and class anxiety (8 items; e.g., "Studying for an examination 
in a statistics course"), interpretation anxiety (11 items; e.g., "Trying to understand the odds in a lottery"), 
and fear of asking for help (4 items; e.g., "Asking someone in the computer lab for help in understanding 
statistical output on the computer"). On the remaining 28 items, students rate attitudes toward statistics 
using a 5-point scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. This also yields three composite 
subscale scores: worth of statistics (16 items; e.g., "I feel statistics is a waste"), fear of statistics teachers 
(5 items; "Statistics teachers are so abstract they seem inhuman"), and computational self-concept (7 
items; "I am too slow in my thinking to get through statistics"). Total subscale scores were formed by 
summing the relevant items and averaging (maximum score = 5). Higher scores indicate higher anxiety or 
attitudes that are more negative (i.e., statistics as being worthless, high fear of statistics teachers, and poor 
computational self-concept). Hanna, Shevlin, & Dempster (2008) confirmed the 6-factor structure. There 
is adequate validity and subscale internal reliabilities ranging from .64 to .94 (Baloglu, 2002; Hanna et al., 
2008). The current sample yielded high internal reliabilities (Cronbach's αs); test/class anxiety (.86), 
interpretation anxiety (.88), fear of asking for help (.91), worth (.94), fear of statistics teachers (.76), and 
computational self-concept (.88).  
 
The Statistics Self-Efficacy scale (Bandalos, Finney, & Geske, 2003) is a 10-item scale that assesses self-
perceptions of competence in statistics using a 7-point Likert response scale with anchors of 1 = strongly 
disagree and 7 = strongly agree. The wording of some items was modified for the current context (e.g., "I 
think I am naturally good at statistics" reworded as "I think I am naturally good at research methods and 
statistics"). A total statistics self-efficacy score was computed by adding scores on all 10 items and 
calculating an average (maximum score = 7). Higher scores indicate higher statistics self-efficacy. 
Bandalos et al. (2003) reported high internal reliability (Cronbach's α = .95). The current data yielded an 
α of .94.  
 
The 81-item Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 
1991) assesses motivation (goals & values, beliefs about skill to succeed, test anxiety) and learning 
strategies (cognitive & metacognitive strategies, management of resources). Responses are made on a 7-
point scale from 1 = not at all true of me to 7 = very true of me. In total, there are 15 subscales. For each, 
items were summed and averaged (maximum score = 7). There are six motivation subscales. Intrinsic 
goal orientation (4 items) assesses motivations of challenge, mastery, or curiosity. Extrinsic goal 
orientation (4 items) assesses motivations to gain grades or other rewards and to perform better than 
others do. Task value (6 items) assesses interest, importance, or usefulness attached to the course. Control 
of learning beliefs (4 items) assesses beliefs about outcome expectancies based on effort. Self-efficacy for 
learning and performance (8 items) assesses two aspects of expectancy: expectancy for success and self-
efficacy for task performance. Test anxiety (5 items) assesses cognitive and affective components of 
worry related to the course. Pintrich et al. (1991) reported evidence for this 6-factor structure and 
Cronbach's αs that ranged from .62 to .93. The Cronbach's αs for the current sample were .59 (intrinsic 
motivation), .63 (extrinsic motivation), .83 (task value), .76 (control of learning beliefs), .95 (self-efficacy 
for learning and performance), and .77 (test anxiety). 
 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 766 

There are nine learning strategies subscales. Rehearsal (4 items), elaboration (6 items), and organization 
(4 items) assess memory and learning strategies that reflect increasingly deeper processing. Critical 
thinking (5 items) assesses critical application of previous knowledge to current learning tasks. 
Metacognitive self-regulation (12 items) assesses planning (goal setting), monitoring (tracking), and 
regulating (adjusting) learning. Time and study environment (8 items) assesses time management and 
suitability of study environment. Effort regulation (4 items) assesses ability to self-manage study goals in 
the face of difficulties or distractions. Peer learning (3 items) assesses collaboration and communication 
with peers to gain clarification and understanding not likely on one's own. Help-seeking (4 items) 
assesses support-seeking strategies when assistance is needed. Pintrich et al. (1991) reported evidence for 
this factor structure and Cronbach's αs that ranging from .52 to .80. The Cronbach's αs for the current 
sample were .69 (rehearsal), .76 (elaboration), .65 (organization), .79 (critical thinking), .81 
(metacognitive self-regulation), .82 (time & study environment), .74 (effort regulation), .68 (peer 
learning), and .61 (help seeking).  
 
Results 
 
Data were initially screened for missing values and assumptions of the analyses. Six cases had missing 
values, which represented 0.85% missing values. Little's Missing Completely at Random Test indicated 
this was missing completely at random, χ2 (2480, N = 113) = 127.10, p = 1.00. As there was very little 
missing data, it was not replaced.  
 
Descriptive statistics are in Table 1. The assumption of normality was violated for several variables. 
There was negative skew in self-efficacy for learning and performance (std. statistic = -2.90), learning 
beliefs (std. statistic = -3.43), extrinsic motivation (std. statistic = -3.88), number of courses enrolled in 
(std. statistic = -7.28), intended attendance at f2f tutorials (std. statistic = -13.95), and intended attendance 
at f2f lectures (std. statistic = -14.64). There was some positive skew in intended attendance at virtual 
tutorials (std. statistic = 4.34), age (std. statistic = 10.57), and distance from campus (std. statistic = 
43.51). The skew in distance was largely due to one student who lived almost 900 km from campus and 
who flew in and out each week for class. This score was replaced by the next highest distance + 1 (101 
km), which reduced the skew substantially (std. statistic = 8.35) while retaining the order of scores. 
Transformations were applied to normalise these skewed distributions, with reflections first applied to 
negatively skewed distributions. Inverse transformation improved but did not entirely normalise the 
distribution of age (std. statistic = -5.82), but normalised intended attendance at f2f tutorials and lectures. 
Log transformations normalised the distributions of distance from campus, number of courses enrolled in, 
extrinsic motivation, self-efficacy for learning and performance, and learning beliefs. Square root 
transformation normalised the distribution of intended attendance at virtual tutorials. All other variables 
were normally distributed. Analyses were conducted using these transformed variables; however, for ease 
of interpretation, descriptive statistics are reported using untransformed values. 
 
Initial univariate analyses were used to determine which of the practical, technological, demographic, and 
attitudinal/motivation variables were associated with intentions to access the different learning options. 
Those predictors that demonstrated significant univariate associations were then entered into a multiple 
regression analysis to determine how much variance in total they explained in the outcome (intention) and 
which variables made significant unique contributions.  
 
Bivariate correlations are given in Table 2. There were significant correlations between the intended 
modes of accessing content. Greater intention to attend f2f tutorials was strongly associated with greater 
intention to attend f2f lectures, indicating a consistent preference for f2f learning. Similarly, there was a 
strong positive correlation between intention to access tutorial and lecture material online. Greater 
intention to access f2f tutorials was associated with lower intention to attend virtual tutorials, indicating 
that there was a tendency to choose one form of access over the other.  
 
Age, main language, student status, enrolment load, repeating the course, program of study, family or 
carer responsibilities, and distance from campus were not significantly associated with any intentions so 
were not included in subsequent regression analyses. There were no effects of internet connectivity or 
type of connectivity at home. This was most likely due to only six students not having internet access at 
home and only three reporting dial-up rather than broadband access. No variables were significantly 
associated with intentions to attend f2f tutorials, so no further results are presented for that outcome.  



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 767 

 
Table 1 
Descriptive statistics for demographic details, STARS, statistics self-efficacy, MSLQ, and intentions  

Variable M (95% CI) Range 

Age (years) 23.16 (21.88; 24.45) 18 - 51 
Grade 1st year a 5.10 (4.93; 5.27) 4 - 7 
Grade 2nd year a 5.43 (5.22; 5.64) 4 - 7 
Enrolment Load (courses) 3.63 (3.48; 3.78) 1- 5 
Hours work/volunteer per week 13.35 (11.48; 15.22) 0 - 40 
Distance from campus (kms)  20.05 (15.96; 24.14) 0.5 - 101 b 

STARS (max = 5)   
   Test/class Anxiety 2.93 (2.78; 3.08) 1.13 - 4.63 
   Interpretation Anxiety 2.40 (2.27; 2.53) 1.09 - 3.91 
   Help-seeking Anxiety 2.33 (2.14; 2.52) 1.00 - 4.75 
   Worth 2.08 (1.93; 2.22) 1.00 - 4.25 
   Fear Statistics Teachers 1.81 (1.69; 1.94) 1.00 - 3.60 
   Computational Self-Concept 2.07 (1.92; 2.23) 1.00 - 4.43 
Statistics Self-Efficacy (max = 7) 4.45 (4.26; 4.65) 1.30 - 6.90 
MSLQ (max = 7)   
   Intrinsic Motivation 4.77 (4.60; 4.94) 2.25 - 7.00 
   Extrinsic Motivation 5.52 (5.33; 5.71) 1.50 - 7.00 
   Task Value 5.09 (4.92; 5.27) 2.50 - 7.00 
   Control Learning Beliefs 5.84 (5.68; 6.00) 2.75 - 7.00 
   SE for learning and performance 4.90 (4.71; 5.10) 2.25 - 7.00 
   Test Anxiety 4.01 (3.77; 4.25) 1.00 - 6.60 
   Rehearsal 4.87 (4.66; 5.08) 1.75 - 7.00 
   Elaboration 5.07 (4.89; 5.26) 3.00 - 7.00 
   Organization  5.08 (4.89; 5.28) 2.25 - 7.00 
   Critical Thinking 4.04 (3.82; 4.26) 1.00 - 7.00 
   Self-regulation metacognition 4.56 (4.40; 4.72) 1.83 - 6.58 
   Self-regulation time and study environment 5.04 (4.84; 5.24) 2.25 - 7.00 
   Self-regulation Effort 5.01 (4.80; 5.21) 2.25 - 7.00 
   Peer Learning  4.63 (4.38; 4.89) 1.00 - 7.00 
   Help-Seeking 4.28 (4.05; 4.51) 1.00 - 6.75 
Intentions (max = 10)   
   F2F lecture 8.95 (8.67; 9.24) 1.2 - 10 
   Lecture Capture 5.75 (5.19; 6.31) 0.2 - 10 
   F2F Tutorial 8.87 (8.56; 9.18) 1 - 10 
   Virtual Tutorial 3.06 (2.55; 3.57) 0 - 10 
   Archived Tutorial 4.89 (4.36; 5.41) 0 - 10 

a Pass = 4, High Distinction = 7 (students required pass in order to proceed into this course) 
b Extreme high score of 887 replaced with 101 (= next highest score + 1) 
   



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).    ascilite 
 

 768 

Table 2 Bivariate correlations  

Note. Only those predictors significantly correlated with intentions were included

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 
1 Grade 1st yr -                       
2 Work -22* -                      
3 Test/class Anx -47* 10 -                     
4 Interpret Anx -34* -01 68* -                    
5 Worth -43* 18 48* 35* -                   
6  Fear Teach -29* 08 40* 29* 72* -                  
7  Comput SC -40* 16 58* 42* 74* 60* -                 
8 Statistics SE 47* 22* -61* -48* -60* -43* -64* -                
9 Extrin Motiv -02 01 -15 -04 17 11 13 -20* -               
10 Task Value 37* -11 -20* -19* -71* -47* -52* 49* -32* -              
11 Rehearsal -02 -07 13 09 -15 -09 -07 20* -13 23* -             
12 Elaboration 23* -07 02 -04 41* -34* -21* 32* -35* 55* 55* -            
13 Organization  06 -06 19* -02 -12 -21* -01 10 -39* 29* 49* 66* -           
14 Critical Think 03 00 07 -07 -28* -15 -10 19* -12 44* 28* 59* 38* -          
15 Metacognition 25* -14 -10 -11 -45* -34 -23 42* -29* 50* 57* 79* 54* 55* -         
16 Environ 22* -22* -10 -10 -37 -35* -12 28* -22* 38* 39* 60* 49* 29* 70* -        
17 Self-reg Effort 22* -20* -21* -14 -44* -34* -25* 37* -15 43* 30* 52* 34* 23* 63* 79* -       
18 Peer Learning  37* -09 09 -01 -16 -12 -21* 18 -14 25* 42* 44* 36* 32* 29* 17 15 -      
19 F2F Lecture 19* -10 -09 -13 -28* -22* -21* 21* -13 17 10 11 05 04 03 10 08 14 -     
20 Lect Capture -20* 24* 20* 08 -01 04 09 -03 -30* 17 28* 33* 35* 25* 20* 29* 23* 15 -03 -    
21 F2F Tute 16 00 -01 -01 -18 -17 -12 15 -18 13 -03 02 -01 -08 00 11 03 -04 72* -02 -   
22 Virtual Tute -08 01 05 06 03 14 12 -12 -07 06 15 14 15 21* 05 -02 -09 23* -16 29* -30* -  
23 Archive Tute -24* 06 15 22* -01 08 17 -06 -33* 10 25* 19* 22* 16 13 21* 15 10 01 66* -01 39* - 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).    ascilite 
 

 769 

Predictors of intention to attend f2f lectures 
 
Lower grades in previous statistics courses, lower statistics self-efficacy, lower computational self-
competence, lower worth of statistics, and greater fear of statistics teachers were associated with lower 
intentions to attend f2f lectures. Thus, poorer perceptions of statistics ability along with evidence of 
poorer past performance and more negative attitudes about statistics and statistics teachers were 
associated with intentions to avoid attending lectures f2f.  
  
To determine the unique contributions of these predictors to explaining variance in intentions to attend f2f 
lectures, they were entered into a standard multiple regression analysis (see Table 3a). They just failed to 
account for a significant percentage of variance (R2 = .09, R2adj = .05), p = .08. When entered together, no 
individual predictor made a significant unique contribution.  
 
Table 3 
Predicting intention to access lectures face-to-face (f2f) or online (captured lecture) 

Variables B  SE(B) β p 
a) Criterion = f2f Lecture (reflected and inverse transformed) 

Grade 1st Year 0.04 0.03 .15 .210 
Statistics Self Efficacy 0.00 0.03 -.01 .938 
Worth -0.08 0.06 -.27 .144 
Computational SC 0.01 0.05 .04 .777 
Fear Statistics Teachers 0.01 0.05 .02 .903 

R2 = .09, F(5, 93) = 2.04, p =.08 
b) Criterion = Captured Lecture 

Work Hrs 0.08 0.03 .27 .002* 
Grade 1st Year -0.48 0.33 -.14 .158 
Class/test Anxiety 0.08 0.35 .02 .821 
Extrinsic Motivation (reflect, log transformed) -2.83 1.48 -.18 .058 
Rehearsal 0.96 0.31 .36 .002* 
Elaboration 0.11 0.48 .04 .819 
Organisation 0.21 0.34 .07 .542 
Self-regulation Time and Study Environment 0.95 0.44 .35 .031* 
Self-regulation Metacognition -1.57 0.57 -.46 .007* 
Self-regulation Effort 0.05 0.40 .02 .894 
Critical Thinking  0.63 0.27 .25 .022* 

R2 = .39, F(11, 91) = 5.37, p < .001 
 
Predictors of intention to access captured lectures 
 
Number of hours of work/volunteering per week was positively correlated with intention to use lecture 
capture. Thus, the more hours worked, the greater the intention to access lecture material online. Lower 
past performance in statistics; higher statistics test/class anxiety; greater extrinsic motivation; greater use 
of rehearsal, organisation, and elaboration strategies; greater self-regulation of metacognition, time and 
the study environment, and effort; and higher critical thinking were also associated with greater intentions 
to use the lecture capture. Thus, students who had lower ability, higher statistics anxiety, and who showed 
performance-oriented motivation reported stronger intentions to use the online lecture material. However, 
so did those students who were more self-regulated, had higher critical thinking, and engaged in more 
memorisation strategies to learn material.  
  
Those significant univariate predictors were entered into a multiple regression analysis (see Table 3b). 
Together, these 11 predictors explained 39.3% (32% adjusted) of the variance in intentions to use lecture 
capture, F (11, 91) = 5.37, p < .001. Several made significant independent contributions: hours of work 
(6.66%), rehearsal (6.5%), self-regulation of metacognitive strategies (5.06%), critical thinking (3.65%), 
and self-regulation of time and the study environment (3.20%). Thus, work commitments and reliance on 
rehearsal were the strongest predictors of intentions to use lecture capture. The remaining variables no 
longer made significant independent contributions.  
 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 770 

Predictors of intention to attend synchronous virtual tutorials 
 
Students who had SPSS on their home computer (n = 73) intended to attend more virtual tutorials (M = 
3.92, SEM = 0.50) than those who did not (n = 38; M = 2.61, SEM = 0.28), t (109) = -2.23, p = .028, d = 
0.47. This represents a small effect size according to Cohen (1988). Higher engagement in critical 
thinking and use of peer learning were also associated with greater intentions to attend the virtual 
tutorials. Thus, students who engaged in deeper processing of material and who recognised the 
importance of peer discussion and interaction to their learning, as well as those who had the technology 
required to do this at home, had stronger intentions to attend this tutorial option.  
 
These three predictors (SPSS at home, critical thinking, and peer learning) were entered into a standard 
multiple regression analysis (see Table 4a). Together, they explained a significant 10.4% (7.9% adjusted) 
of the variance in intention to attend the virtual tutorials, F (3, 106) = 4.11, p = .008. Only SPSS made a 
significant unique contribution, explaining 3.96% of the variance in intentions to attend the virtual 
tutorials. 
 
Table 4 
Predicting intention to access synchronous (virtual) or asynchronous (archived) tutorials 
Variables B  SE(B) β p 

a) Criterion = Virtual Tutorial (square root transformed) 

SPSS a 0.34 0.16 .20 .033* 
Critical Thinking 0.09 0.07 .13 .179 
Peer Learning 0.09 0.06 .15 .134 

R2 = .10, F(3, 106) = 4.11, p = .008 

b) Criterion = Archived Tutorials 

Gender b 0.95 0.74 .13 .202 
Grade 1st Year -0.63 0.32 -.20 .048* 
Interpretation Anxiety 0.33 0.42 .08 .433 
Extrinsic Motivation (reflect, log transformed) -3.60 1.50 -.24 .019* 
Rehearsal 0.48 0.27 .20 .077 
Elaboration -0.03 0.40 -.01 .949 
Organisation 0.00 0.34 .00 .993 
Time and Study Environment 0.25 0.30 .10 .415 

R2 = .26, F(8, 94) = 4.12, p < .001 
a No = 0, Yes = 1 b Male = 0, Female = 1  
 
Predictors of intention to access asynchronous virtual tutorials 
 
Female students intended to use the archived virtual tutorials more (M = 5.19, SEM = 0.28) than did male 
students (M = 3.32, SEM = 0.67), t (108) = -2.70, p = .008, d = 0.68. This represents a medium effect size 
according to Cohen (1988). Lower past performance in statistics; greater interpretation anxiety; higher 
extrinsic motivation; higher reliance on rehearsal, organisation, and elaboration strategies; and higher 
self-regulation of time and the study environment were associated with greater intentions to use the 
archived virtual tutorials. Thus, intentions to access the archived tutorials online were related to lower 
ability and anxiety over having to interpret statistical information, which is a large part of the tutorial 
activities, as well as with performance-oriented motivation. However, intentions to access these 
asynchronous online tutorials were also associated with greater use of memorisation learning strategies 
and self-regulation.  
 
Together these eight predictors explained a significant 25.9% (19.6% adjusted) of the variance in 
intentions to access the archived virtual tutorials, F (8, 94) = 4.12, p < .001 (Table 4b). There were 
significant independent contributions by 1st year statistics grade, which explained 3.17% of the variance, 
and extrinsic motivation, which explained 4.54% of the variance. The remaining variance overlapped 
between all predictors.  
 
 
 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 771 

Discussion 
 
Understanding how motivation and attitudes as well as practical and technological considerations affect 
student intentions to access different components in a blended learning buffet can help lecturers tailor the 
blend to students' needs. It is likely that a more tailored approach will lead, in turn, to better student 
engagement, achievement, and satisfaction. While blended learning courses offer integrated f2f and 
online components, only buffet-style courses give students control over what mode of delivery they 
choose for a particular learning activity. Ideally, those choices are based on student background, learning 
styles, and goals (Twigg, 2003). However, to date, no studies had examined the factors associated with 
students' choices. 
 
The current results demonstrate that motivation and attitudes are important predictors of student choices 
in a buffet-style blended learning course. While no predictors of intentions to attend f2f tutorials were 
identified, a cluster of negative attitudes, low motivation, and high anxiety predicted intentions to avoid 
f2f lectures. Poor past performance, low self-efficacy, low task value, and high anxiety are also related to 
disorganised study strategies, low effort, and poor achievement (Bandalos et al., 2003). In traditional f2f 
courses, it is possible that these poor outcomes arise from avoidance of classes. Indeed, Billings-Gagliardi 
and Mazor (2007) reported that only 17% of first year medical students reported routinely attending all 
f2f lectures. The other 83% made case-by-case costs-benefits decisions regarding attendance. Reasons 
given for not attending included the lecturer's approach, predicted outcomes of attending, the particular 
topic, and learning needs, as well as personal considerations such as the time of the lecture and other 
competing demands. Many of these factors are similar to those that predict choice of online over f2f 
access (Copley, 2007). Providing asynchronous online access to lecture content via podcasts or lecture 
capture allows more convenient and flexible access to learning (Frydenberg, 2006; Nathan & Chan, 
2007), which addresses some of the practical reasons for not attending f2f lectures.  
 
Asynchronous online access may also offer a less threatening means for some students to access the 
content in statistics courses. Social science students report high levels of anxiety about statistics and this 
is associated with avoidance and, subsequently, poor performance (Onwuegbuzie, 2003, 2004). In the 
current study, it was specifically fear of statistics teachers that predicted intentions to avoid f2f lectures 
and high class/test anxiety for statistics that predicted intentions to use lecture capture. Therefore, this 
suggests that students who are anxious about being in a statistics class and, in particular, facing statistics 
teachers, find the asynchronous online access less threatening. Similarly, interpretation anxiety predicted 
intentions to use the archived tutorials. Thus, those students who were particularly anxious about 
interpreting statistical equations and output, which is a major component of the work in tutorials, were 
more likely to prefer the asynchronous tutorial. General test anxiety and anxiety over seeking help in 
statistics were not significantly related to any of the intentions. Thus, this shows that it is specifically 
anxiety associated with being in a statistics class that is important rather than general anxiety about taking 
tests (the format of which were identical regardless of what format students accessed the lectures and 
tutorials in) or about seeking help with statistics. Therefore, it is important to use discipline specific 
measures when examining students' intentions.  
 
In addition, a heavier reliance on rehearsal as a learning strategy was independently related to stronger 
intention to use lecture capture (and this predictor approached significance with intentions to use the 
archived tutorials as well). This is consistent with Huang et al. (2011) who found that students who 
preferred learning facts and using well-established methods for problem solving preferred online options. 
The fact that higher extrinsic motivation was also related to stronger intentions to use those asynchronous 
modes suggests that students driven to achieve and be seen to achieve perceive the accessibility and 
control afforded by asynchronous media as an important means to that end. Certainly ability to revise 
content is an advantage often reported for these media (e.g., Williams & Fardon, 2007), and extrinsically 
motivated students may respond to that. However, intrinsic motivation, or the desire to learn and master 
content without considering external rewards, was unrelated to intentions.  
 
There did appear to be two underlying reasons for students intending to use the online options more. One, 
as discussed, was negative attitudes, high anxiety, a preference for learning using rehearsal, and extrinsic 
motivation. However, positive approaches to learning were also related to intentions to use these online 
options. Higher use of critical thinking was significantly related to intentions to use the online options. 
Thus, students who wish to think more critically and deeply about the material recognize the benefits of 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 772 

these media for increasing control over the manner in which one can process the material. In addition, 
three aspects of self-regulation (metacognitive, time and environment, and effort) were related to 
intentions to use different online modes. The lack of a set timetable for accessing this online material 
demands a higher level of self-regulation in order to be successful in learning this way. This is consistent 
with Matheos et al.'s (2005) finding that independent learners preferred online options.  
 
Contrary to expectations, there was little evidence that practical or technological considerations affected 
student's intended choices. However, engaging in more work outside of university was related to stronger 
intentions to use lecture capture, but not a concomitant intention to avoid f2f lectures. This suggests that 
those students with more outside commitments do not specifically intend to miss f2f classes, but perhaps 
know it is more likely they will due to competing demands and that they will use lecture capture more for 
that reason. In contrast, family and caring commitments did not play a role. Most of this sample did not 
have caring responsibilities so that may be one reason this was not an important factor. However, most 
did have part-time work commitments. Distance to the campus was not a factor in choices in the current 
study, although Rose et al. (2000) had found that it predicted the decision to enrol in an online over a f2f 
course. The degrees in which the current sample was enrolled are only offered in on-campus mode and so 
students all lived within a reasonable distance of the campus (100 km or less, except for the one student 
who flew in). Where degrees are offered in online or on-campus modes, distance may be a more 
important factor in the choices made. The only technological consideration was that having the SPSS 
software predicted intentions to attend the virtual tutorials. This software was essential to the tutorial 
activities. Research (Matheos et al., 2005; Rose et al., 2000) that has previously found that technological 
factors relating to having computers and the Internet predict choices may be outdated. Most students in 
this study had computers and the Internet at home. Many probably also had the internet on mobile phones, 
iPads, and laptops.  
 
One question that this study does not answer is whether intentions to use the online options represent 
avoidance strategies that are likely to result in poorer achievement or whether the online options assist 
engagement, processing, and subsequent achievement by reducing the threat felt in attending f2f classes 
and increasing the control over processing material. Asynchronous online options allow learners more 
control over the pace of delivery of material (Copley, 2007; Robinson & Hullinger, 2008; Williams & 
Fardon, 2007). They can pause, rewind, or slow down delivery speed. This provides time to think 
critically and more deeply about the material. The additional control that learners have with asynchronous 
online learning is particularly likely to benefit those students with high anxiety and concomitant low self-
efficacy. Anxiety interferes with learning because cognitive processing capacity is directed from the task 
(learning statistics) to the cognitive components of worry (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001; Hopko, Ashcraft, Gute, 
Ruggerio, & Lewis, 1998). This affects focus and students attribute this to poor ability (Ashcraft & Kirk, 
2001), which subsequently reduces self-efficacy (Pajares, 2002). This, in turn, results in high anxiety in 
the future when faced with similar tasks (Zohar, 1998). Therefore, allowing students' choice in how they 
access course content may be especially useful to the teaching of statistics and other "scary" courses. The 
ability of students to control the pace of delivery with asynchronously delivered online material can also 
have benefits for instructors. It means that they can deliver f2f material at a pace that better suits them in 
terms of the time available to cover necessary content, rather than having to slow the pace or repeat 
material several times to meet the processing needs of some of the students (Gedik, Kiraz, & Ozden, 
2013). Students who require a slower delivery or the opportunity to hear it several times can access the 
asynchronous online option. Obviously, instructors still need to provide means for students to ask 
questions, but this can be achieved for those accessing the online material via online forums or discussion 
groups (Gedik et al., 2013). 
 
Increasingly, higher education is using online or blended learning approaches. The Bradley Review of 
Australian Higher Education (2008) identified "An accessible and sophisticated online learning 
environment" (p. 79) as a key aspect in the provision of a quality student experience. However, the 
current results, along with those of related studies (e.g., Tselios et al., 2011), indicate that understanding 
the cognitive strategies employed by individual students along with the values and expectancies they have 
for learning is essential to designing a blended learning structure that will engage them and enhance their 
learning experience. Providing a well-designed blended learning buffet from which students can choose 
their preferred means of access to different learning activities (e.g., lectures versus tutorials) has the 
potential to better engage students by providing a better match for their particular approach to learning. 
The results of this study can inform the development of a pre-course screening battery to measure student 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 773 

attitudes and motivations, which would provide students with guidance on what means of delivery is 
likely to better suit them (similar to the approach taken in Ohio State University's Program in Course 
Redesign, 2005). 
 
The provision of a blended learning buffet need not be expensive or time consuming for institutions or 
staff. It can be as simple as in the current study of providing asynchronous or synchronous access online 
to lectures and tutorials that are provided f2f. The technology to deliver synchronous virtual classes (e.g., 
via Wimba, or Blackboard Collaborate [https://www.blackboard.com/Platforms/Collaborate/Products/ 
Blackboard-Collaborate.aspx]) as well as to capture f2f classes for asynchronous online access (e.g., via 
Lectopia or Echo360 [http://echo360.com/]) is readily available in many universities. With the increasing 
development of associated apps for smart phones (e.g., Blackboard Collaborate has released an app for 
Android phones), accessibility and flexibility for students is increasing. However, Gedik et al. (2013) 
identified that it is important to provide students with sufficient orientation and instructions in utilising 
the online delivery. Ensuring all students adequate access to staff and interaction with other students are 
important considerations as well; however, these same tools can be used for virtual consultation and 
discussion groups overcoming that problem. Where courses require access to course-specific software, as 
in the current study, provisions need to be made for student access to this at home if they were to choose 
the online delivery modes. A student-version of the SPSS software required in the current statistics course 
is available packaged with a number of relevant textbooks. Having this software was a factor in intentions 
to choose the online tutorials so this is an important consideration. 
 
Future research needs to extend this work to examine these individual characteristics in relation to actual 
behaviour. Given the strong relationship between intentions and behaviours (Ajzen, 2002; Cheng, 2011), 
it is expected that the relationships would be similar. However, it would be useful to also examine 
whether the behavioural choices made regarding f2f versus online access and individual differences 
interact in their effects on outcomes. For example, do students with high anxiety who choose the online 
option perform better because they feel more control over the content compared to those with high 
anxiety who choose to attend f2f? Alternatively, does this result in poorer performance because it 
represents an avoidance or procrastination strategy? Until more is understood about how student 
characteristics influence the choices they make and how they then interact with those choices to affect 
achievement, blended learning cannot be well implemented.  
 
References 
 
Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned 

behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 665-683. Retrieved from 
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb00236.x 

 
American Psychological Association. (2009). Publication manual of the American Psychological 

Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. 
 
Artino, A. R. (2010). Online or face-to-face learning? Exploring the personal factors that predict students' 

choice of instructional format. Internet and Higher Education, 13, 272-276. Retrieved from 
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.iheduc.2010.07.005  

 
Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety and 

performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 224-237. Retrieved from 
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.130.2.224  

 
Baharun, N., & Porter, A. (2009). Teaching statistics using a blended approach: Integrating technology-

based resources. In Same places, different spaces. Proceedings ascilite Auckland 2009. Retrieved 
from http://www.ascilite.org.au/conferences/auckland09/procs/baharun.pdf  

 
Baloglu, M. (2002). Psychometric properties of the statistics anxiety rating scale. Psychological Reports, 

90, 315-325. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2002.90.1.315 
 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 774 

Bandalos, D. L., Finney, S. J., & Geske, J. A. (2003). A model of statistics performance based on 
achievement goal theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 604-616. Retrieved from 
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.604  

 
Billings-Gagliardi, S., & Mazor, K. M., (2007). Student decisions about lecture attendance: Do electronic 

course materials matter? Academic Medicine, 82(10 Suppl.), S73-S76. Retrieved from 
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ACM.0b013e31813e651e  

 
Bleed, R. (2001). A hybrid campus for the new millennium. Educause Review, 36, 16-24. Retrieved from 

http://net.educause.edu/ir/library/pdf/ERM0110.pdf  
 
Bradley, D. (2008). Review of Australian higher education: Final report. Retrieved from 

http://www.innovation.gov.au/HigherEducation/Documents/Review/PDF/Higher%20Education%20
Review_one%20document_02.pdf 

 
Chang, S., & Tung, F. (2008). An empirical investigation of students' behavioural intentions to use the 

online learning course websites. British Journal of Educational Technology, 39, 71-83. Retrieved 
from http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8535.2007.00742.x  

 
Chen, P. D., Lambert, A. D., & Guidry, K. R. (2008). Engaging online learners: The impact of Web-based 

learning technology on college student engagement. Computers & Education, 54, 1222-1232. 
Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2009.11.008  

 
Cheng, Y. (2011). Antecedents and consequences of e-learning acceptance. Information Systems Journal, 

21, 269-299. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2575.2010.00356.x  
 
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciencies. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence 

Erlbaum Associates. 
 
Copley, J. (2007). Audio and video podcasts of lectures for campus-based students: Production and 

evaluation of student use. Innovations in Education and Teaching International, 44, 387-399. 
Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14703290701602805  

 
Cruise, R. J., & Wilkins, E. M. (1980). STARS: Statistical anxiety rating scale. Berrien Springs, MI: 

Andrews University. 
 
Davis, F. D., Bagozzi, R. P., & Warshaw, P. R. (1989). User acceptance of computer technology: A 

comparison of two theoretical models. Management Science, 35, 982-1002. Retrieved from 
http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.35.8.982 

 
Eccles, J. (2005). Subjective task value and the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. In A. 

J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 105-121). New York, 
NY: Guilford Press. 

 
Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L., & Midgley, C. (1983). 

Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement 
motivation (pp. 75-146). San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman. 

 
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and 

research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Retrieved from 
http://people.umass.edu/aizen/f&a1975.html  

 
Frydenberg, M. (2006). Principles and pedagogy: The two P's of podcasting in the information 

technology classroom. In Proceedings of the Information Systems Education Conference 2006, 23, 
10pp. Retrieved from http://proc.isecon.org/2006/3354/ISECON.2006.Frydenberg.pdf 

 
Garrison, R., & Vaughan, H. (2008). Blended learning in higher education: Framework, principles and 

guidelines. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 775 

 
Gedik, N., Kiraz, E., & Ozden, Y. (2013). Design of a blended learning environment: Considerations and 

implementation issues. Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 29(1), 1-19. Retrieved from 
http://ascilite.org.au/ajet/submission/index.php/AJET/article/view/6/21  

 
Hanna, D., Shevlin, M., & Dempster, M. (2008). The structure of the statistics anxiety rating scale: A 

confirmatory factor analysis using UK psychology students. Personality and Individual Differences, 
45, 68-74. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.02.021  

 
Hopko, D. R., Ashcraft, M. H., Gute, J., Ruggerio, K. J., & Lewis, C. (1998). Mathematics anxiety and 

working memory: Support for the existence of a deficient inhibition mechanism. Journal of Anxiety 
Disorders, 12, 343-355. 

 
Huang, E. Y., Lin, S. W., & Huang, T. K. (2011). What type of learning style leads to online participation 

in the mixed-mode e-learning environment? A study of software usage instruction. Computers & 
Education, 58, 338-349. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2011.08.003  

 
Matheos, K., Daniel, K., McCalla, G. I. (2005). Dimensions for blended learning technology: Learners' 

perspectives. Journal of Learning Design, 1, 56-76. Retrieved from 
https://www.jld.edu.au/article/view/9/6 

 
Means, B., Toyama, Y., Murphy, R., Bakia, M., & Jones, K. (2010). Evaluation of evidence-based 

practices in online learning: A meta-analysis and review of online learning studies. Retrieved from 
http://www2.ed.gov/rschstat/eval/tech/evidence-based-practices/finalreport.pdf  

 
Nathan, P., & Chan, A. (2007). Engaging undergraduates with podcasting in a business subject. In ICT: 

Providing choices for learners and learning. Proceedings ascilite Singapore 2007. Retrieved from 
http://www.ascilite.org.au/conferences/singapore07/procs/nathan.pdf 

 
Neumann, D. L., & Hood, M. (2009). The effects of using a wiki on student engagement and learning of 

report writing skills in a university statistics course. Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 
25(3), 382-398. Retrieved from http://www.ascilite.org.au/ajet/ajet25/neumann.html  

 
Neumann, D. L., Neumann, M. M., & Hood, M. (2011). Evaluating computer-based simulations, 

multimedia, and animations that help integrate blended learning with lectures in first year statistics. 
Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 27(2), 274-289.Retrieved from 
http://www.ascilite.org.au/ajet/ajet27/neumann.html  

 
Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2003). Modeling statistics achievement among graduate students. Educational and 

Psychological Measurement, 63, 1020-1038. Retrieved from 
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164402250989  

 
Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2004). Academic procrastination and statistics anxiety. Assessment and Evaluation 

in Higher Education, 29, 1-19. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0260293042000160384  
 
Pajares, F. (2002). Overview of social cognitive theory and of self-efficacy. Retrieved from 

http://p20motivationlab.org/social-cognitive-theory  
 
Palmgreen, P. (1984). Uses and gratifications: A theoretical perspective. In R. N. Bostrom (Ed.), 

Communication Yearbook 8, (pp. 61-72). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. 
 
Picciano, A. G. (2009). Blending with purpose: The multimodal model. Journal of the Research Center 

for Educational Technology, 5, 4-14. Retrieved from 
http://www.rcetj.org/index.php/rcetj/article/download/11/14  

 



Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 2013, 29(6).  
 

 776 

Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1991). A manual for the use of the 
motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ). Ann Arbour, MI: National Institute for 
Research to Improve Postsecondary Teaching and Learning. Retrieved from 
http://www.scribd.com/doc/93744041/Motivated-Strategies-for-Learning-Questionnaire-Manual  

 
Program in Course Redesign (2005). The Ohio State University. Retrieved from 

http://www.thencat.org/PCR/R3/OSU/OSU_Overview.htm 
 
Robinson, C. C., & Hullinger, H. (2008). New benchmarks in higher education: Student engagement in 

online learning. Journal of Education for Business, 84, 101-108. Retrieved from 
http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/JOEB.84.2.101-109 

 
Rose, M., Frisby, A., Hamlin, M., & Jones, S. (2000). Evaluation of the effectiveness of a web-based 

graduate epidemiology course. Computers in Nursing, 18, 162-167. 
 
Schober, B., Wagner, P., Reimann, R., Atria, M., & Spiel, C. (2006). Teaching research methods in an 

internet-based blended-learning setting: Vienna e-lecturing (VEL). Methodology, 2, 73-82. Retrieved 
from http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1614-1881.2.2.73  

 
Tselios, N., Daskalakis, S., & Papadopoulou, M. (2011). Assessing the acceptance of a blended learning 

university course. Educational Technology & Society, 14, 224-235. Retrieved from 
http://www.ifets.info/journals/14_2/19.pdf  

 
Tudor, G. E. (2006). Teaching introductory statistics online - Satisfying the students. Journal of Statistics 

Education, 14(3). Retrieved from http://www.amstat.org/publications/jse/v14n3/tudor.html 
 
Twigg, C. A. (2003). Improving learning and reducing costs: Models for online learning. Educause 

Review, 38, 28-38. Retrieved from http://net.educause.edu/ir/library/pdf/erm0352.pdf  
 
Utts, J., Sommer, B., Acredolo, C., Maher, M. W., & Matthews, H. R. (2003). A study comparing 

traditional and hybrid internet-based instruction in introductory statistics classes. Journal of Statistics 
Education, 11(3). Retrieved from http://www.amstat.org/publications/jse/v11n3/utts.html  

 
Ward, B. (2004). The best of both worlds: A hybrid statistics course. Journal of Statistics Education, 

12(3). Retrieved from http://www.amstat.org/publications/jse/v12n3/ward.html 
 
Williams, J., & Fardon, M. (2007). Perpetual connectivity: Lecture recordings and portable media 

players. In ICT: Providing choices for learners and learning. Proceedings ascilite Singapore 2007. 
Retrieved from http://www.ascilite.org.au/conferences/singapore07/procs/williams-jo.pdf  

 
Yablon, Y. B., & Katz, Y. J. (2001). Statistics through the medium of internet: What students think and 

achieve. Academic Exchange Quarterly, 5, 17-22. 
 
Zohar, D. (1998). An additive model of test anxiety. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 330-340. 

Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.90.2.330 

 

Corresponding author: Michelle Hood, michelle.hood@griffith.edu.au 

Australasian Journal of Educational Technology © 2013. 

Please cite as: Hood, M. (2013). Bricks or clicks? Predicting student intentions in a blended learning 
buffet. Australasian Journal of Educational Technology, 29(6), 762-776.