Microsoft Word - Issue 3.DOCX


 

96  

The Influence of Employee Attractiveness on Service Recovery Paradox: Facial  

Recognition and Eye Tracker Analyses  
  

Erdoğan Koc  

Bahcesehir University 

Faculty of Economics, Administrative and Social Sciences 

Yeni Mahalle Şehit Astsubay Mustafa Soner Varlık Caddesi No:77, 10200 Bandırma/Balıkesir, Turkey 

Phone: +90 266 717 01 17 erdogankoc@yahoo.com  

  

Hakan Boz  

Usak University  

İzmir Yolu 8. Km. Atatürk Bulvarı Merkez/Uşak Merkez, 64000 Uşak Merkez, Turkey  

Phone: +90 276 221 21 21  
onlyhakanboz@hotmail.com  

  

Burcin Boz  

Bilecik Seyh Edebali University  

Adapazarı Bilecik Yolu Bilecik Şeyh Edebali Üniversitesi Rektörlüğü, 11230 Gülümbe/Bilecik 

Merkez/Bilecik, Turkey  

Phone: +90 228 214 11 11  

burcinsonmez86@gmail.com  

  

Abstract  

This study aims to explore the influence of employee attractiveness on service recovery 

paradox by using Eye Tracker and Facial Recognition Software.  Based on two service recovery 

scenarios an experiment was carried out with the participation of 20 subjects in Turkey. In the 

experiment participants’ pre-and post-satisfaction levels before the service failure and after the service 

recovery attempt have been measured. The participants were also asked to respond to Likert type 

questions to explain to explain how they felt about the service encounter they were exposed to in the 

scenarios.  The analysis of data showed that although attractiveness reduced the negative of the 

responses, increased interest in the attractive service employee.  However, the results showed that in 

both scenarios (with attractive and less attractive service employee photos) the service recovery 

paradox did not occur.  

  

Keywords: Service Failure; Service Recovery Paradox; Artificial Intelligence; Hospitality; 

Facial Recognition; Eye Tracker.  

  

1. Introduction  

A service failure can be defined as any type of error, mistake, deficiency or problem that 

occurs during the provision of a service, causing a delay or hindrance in the satisfaction of customer 

needs (Koc, 2017; Koc, 2019a).  Due to the intense and frequent interactions taking place tourism and 

hospitality services are highly prone to service failures (Koc, 2013). Coupled with the general service 

characteristics of inseparability and heterogeneity nature, service failures in tourism and hospitality 

are almost inevitable.    

Although service failures are almost inevitable, unsuccessful service failure recovery attempts 

result in negative emotions and negative behavioural outcomes and threaten the survival and growth 

of tourism and hospitality businesses (Ha and Jang, 2009; Wen and Chi, 2013 Koc, 2017).  The 

negative emotions and the behavioural intentions after a service failure may range from customer 

dissatisfaction (Kelley et al., 1993; Koc, 2017) to  negative word-of-mouth (Mattila, 2001), customer  



E. Koc, H. Boz, B. Boz - The Influence of Employee Attractiveness on Service Recovery Paradox: Facial Recognition 

and Eye Tracker Analyses  

97  

 

switching (Keaveney, 1995; Pranić and Roehl, 2013), increased costs (Armistead et al., 1995), and 

lower employee performance and morale (Lee et al., 2013) for the service business.  

Service recovery is a process which comprises of all the actions designed and implemented to 

resolve problems and change customers' negative attitudes to prevent negative word-of-mouth 

communications and to avoid the switching of customers (Miller et al., 2000, Baker, 2017). First 

coined by McCollough and Bharadwaj (1992) refers to service recovery situations in which a 

customer's post-failure satisfaction exceeds pre-failure satisfaction after a service failure has been 

recovered.  

The service recovery paradox relates to a secondary satisfaction after a service failure in which 

customers compare their expectations for recovery to their perceptions of the service recovery 

performance.  Service recoveries ensure that feeling of justice is reinstated. In a recovery situation, 

and for a service recovery paradox to take place, customers usually look for three types of justice.  

These are distributional justice, procedural justice and interactional justice (Sparks and McColl-

Kennedy, 2001). Distributional justice is about the compensation of the losses incurred by the 

customer as a result of a service failure (Patterson et al., 2006; Koc, 2019b). Distributional justice 

may be reinstated by refunding, or part of, the bill, or providing free products or services.  

Procedural justice is to do with the perceived fairness of a process (Sparks and 

McCollKennedy, 2001), i.e. whether the procedures or the criteria used in the recovery were perceived 

as being fair. The procedural justice may be established by the use of appropriate policies and 

procedures, the convenience of the process and the timeliness of the service recovery attempt (Tax et 

al., 1998, Smith et al., 1999; Baker, 2017). According to Blodgett et al., (1997) the speed of the 

recovery action taken is one of the major determinants of procedural justice.  

Interactional justice relates to the interaction quality between the service employee and the 

customer and, depends on friendliness shown by service staff during the recovery process (Tax et al, 

1998; Smith et al., 1999) or the interaction quality between a service employee and management 

(Baker, 2017).  

Several research findings show that after an efficient and effective recovery of a service 

failure, i.e. when the feeling of justice has been reinstated, customers' repurchase intentions, the 

likelihood of making positive word-of-mouth communications, and their image of the brand may 

increase significantly (Baker, 2017). Based on the above background this study explores the influence 

of the attractiveness of service employees on service failure paradox by attempting to reinstate 

distributional justice (Sparks and McColl-Kennedy, 2001).  

  

2. Employee Attractiveness  

Attractiveness forms a bias causing favour in higher physical attractiveness and disfavour in 

lower physical attractiveness (Berscheid and Walster, 1972; Frevert and Walker, 2014). In fact, 

attractiveness is a heuristic device, a type of conditioning, people use in responding to external stimuli 

(Villi and Koc, 2018).  

Facial attractiveness which primarily determines physical attractiveness (Olson and 

Marshuetz, 2005) may have a significant influence on other people's various responses towards the 

person who is attractive. For example research shows that physically attractive people may getter 

better grades in school (Langlois et al., 2000; Elovitz et al., 2009); may be more likely to be recruited 

following (Johnston, 2010; Roberts, 2012; Toledano, 2013; Verhulst et. al., 2010) and may even tend 

to get higher salaries for the same job done (Bennett, 2010; Salter et al., 2012; Toledano, 2013; Frevert 

and Walker, 2014). The influence of attractiveness in tourism and hospitality has been researched by 

various scholars.  For instance, Magnini et al.'s (2013) study found that clean-shaven male employees 

and employees who smiled produced more positive feelings among hotel customers. Kim et al.'s 

(2017) study found that while employee attractiveness enhanced the service encounter, attractiveness 

was not a substitute for friendly, courteous service. Although appearance was one of the salient  



BRAIN – Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience  

Volume 10, Issue 3 (September, 2019), ISSN 2067-3957  

98  

 

attributes of an employee, when building rapport, customers paid more attention to the nonverbal and 

behavioural cues of employees. In another study, Boz and Yilmaz's (2017) study showed that tourism 

and hospitality managers were more likely to recruit attractive candidates even for back-stage jobs. 

Villi and Koc's (2018) study found that customers gave milder responses to service failures when they 

interacted with attractive service failure.  However, the influence of employee attractiveness on 

service recoveries and particularly on service recovery paradox has not been researched by 

researchers.    

  

3. The Method and the Procedure  

The study aims to explore the influence of employee attractiveness on service recovery 

paradox. In the study, data were collected from 10 female and 10 male participants who eat at a 

restaurant at least once a week. The participants were asked to read the following two scenarios:  

  

Service Failure and Recovery Scenarios  

Scenario 1  

Imagine that you have had an exhausting day.  It was so busy 

that you did not have time to have lunch during the day. You decided 

to eat dinner in a restaurant. You ordered your food to the waitress on 

the right. You felt that you were really hungry. The meal you ordered 

arrived without any delay. You started to eat your meal.  The food 

tasted really good.  But just before having your second fork of the meal 

you saw a long hair in the dish. You immediately called the waitress 

and informed her about what happened. The waitress first apologised 

and brought a new plate of food and told you that it was on the house, 

i.e. there was going to be no bill to pay.  

  

Scenario 2  

Imagine that you had your dinner in a restaurant.  After 

finishing your main course, you saw an appetizing dessert eaten by 

another guest in the restaurant. You ordered the same dessert to the 

waitress on the right. The dessert arrived and you started eating.  But 

before having your second fork you saw a long hair in the dessert. You 

immediately called the waitress and informed her about what 

happened. The waitress first apologised and brought a new dish of a 

dessert and told you that it was on the house, i.e. there was going to be 

no bill to pay.  

  

The scenarios were basically the same except in the first 

scenario an attractive and in the second scenario not so attractive  

waitress photos were used.  The attractiveness of the persons (waitresses) in the photos was 

determined with a pilot test with thirty participants two weeks before the experiment. The attractive 

female photo was provided courtesy of Dr. Martin Gruendl at Beautycheck (Braun et al., 2001). The 

not so attractive female photo was taken from The Chicago Face Database (Ma, Correll, & 

Wittenbrink. 2015).  

The participants' emotions were measured before, during and after the reading of the scenarios.  

The participants were also asked to respond to four short questions appearing on the screen by using 

the mouse of the computer. The purpose of asking these questions was, though to a limited extent, to 

triangulate data (Koc and Boz, 2014) and support the data collected by the psychophysiological tools. 

These questions were particularly aimed at measuring participants’ emotions regarding the regarding  



E. Koc, H. Boz, B. Boz - The Influence of Employee Attractiveness on Service Recovery Paradox: Facial Recognition 

and Eye Tracker Analyses  

99  

 

the service failure itself, the recovery attempt and their future intentions whether to at the same 

restaurant again and whether they would recommend this particular restaurant to other people.     

  

4. The Task and Experimental Procedure  

The experiment procedure was designed by using the experiment flows used in the studies of 

(Boz, 2015; Boz and Yılmaz, 2016; Taşkın, Koç and Boz, 2017; Koc and Boz, 2018).  The task and 

the procedure were as follows (See Figure 1):  

  

 

  

1. In order to eliminate the influence of the order effect the visual materials were shown randomly 

to the participants.  

2. The duration of the experiment was 120 seconds for each participant.  

3. After the fixing of the facial recognition software subjects were asked to take a comfortable 

position and relax. Participants ran the experiment individually using a mouse to answer 

assessment questionnaires.  

4. First visual marketing material was shown ten seconds after the experiment started. During these 

ten seconds, the participants were shown a black screen.  

5. After having being exposed to marketing stimuli the participants were asked to make an evaluation 

of the scenarios for a duration of twenty seconds - by indicating their responses to the questions 

shown on the screen with the use of a Mouse.   

6. The participants indicated their responses on a Likert scale a 1 (one) score meant “Extremely 

negative attitude towards exposed stimuli” and a score of 10 meant “Extremely positive attitude 

towards exposed stimuli” on the screen with the mouse.  

7. The experiment was carried out in a quiet room.  

8. Prior to the start of the experiment, the participants were given information about the experiment 

procedure for 4-5 minutes in order to relax them.  

9. The participants were instructed to avoid any excess movements during the experiment.  

10. Two service recovery paradox scenarios (Figure 1) were shown to the participants. Each scenario 

exposure lasted 35 seconds. Then, the participants were asked to respond to four questions 

appearing on the screen for each scenario for a period of 20 seconds.  

11. Before each service recovery scenario, the participants were shown a black screen for ten seconds.  

  

  
 

 

 

 

 

 



BRAIN – Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience  

Volume 10, Issue 3 (September, 2019), ISSN 2067-3957  

100  

 

The data collected by the psychophysiological tools of Eye Tracker and Facial Expression 

recognition were calculated and analysed by using the software developed by Boz and Kose (2018). 

Facial Expression recognition software was developed by using machine learning and artificial 

intelligence methods and the accuracy of the algorithm is %87.  

 

5. Findings and Analysis  

The participants’ responses (Likert Type - 1 to 10) regarding the service failure itself, the 

recovery attempt and their future intentions whether to visit the same restaurant again and whether 

they would recommend this particular restaurant to other people.  By looking at the total means in 

Table1 it can be seen that when the waitress was more attractive the participants tended to evaluate 

the service failure less important (4,70 to 5, 40 – the difference being 25,38%). This may mean that 

attractiveness of the service employee (the waitress) reduced the severity of the service failure by 

more than 25%.  

  

Table 1. Overall Evaluation of the Service Encounter  

  Attractive Waitress Stimuli  Less Attractive Waitress Stimuli  The  
Difference  
(Between 

the  
Totals) 

(Mean)  

Female  
Respondents  

(Mean)  

Male  
Respondents  

(Mean)  

Total  

  
(Mean)  

Female  
Respondents  

(Mean)  

Male  
Respondents  

(Mean)  

Total   

  
(Mean)  

Importance of 
service failure.  
1=  Not 

important  
10=Important   

4,50  4,90  4,70  5,80  5,00  5,40  25,38%  

Overall 

evaluation 

 of 

service 

experience   
1= Negative  
10= Positive  

3,70  2,80  3,25  3,30  3,00  3,15  3,25%  

Tolerability 

 of the 

failure  
1= Intolerable  
10= Tolerable  

  

3,80  2,50  3,15  3,00  3,10  3,05  3,15%  

Recommending 

the restaurant to 

others.  
  
1= would not 

recommend  
10=  would 

recommend  

5,30  2,80  4,05  3,40  2,20  2,80  30,86%  

  

Additionally, when the service employee was more attractive the participants tended to 

evaluate the Service experience relatively more positively (3,25 to 3,15) as a higher score meant that 

the experience was more positive. Moreover, when the service employee was more attractive, the 

participants found the failure relatively more tolerable (3,15 to 3,05). These three above findings are  



E. Koc, H. Boz, B. Boz - The Influence of Employee Attractiveness on Service Recovery Paradox: Facial Recognition 

and Eye Tracker Analyses  

101  

 

in line with Villi and Koc's (2018) findings in which the customers gave milder responses to service 

failures when the service employee was more attractive. Regarding the recommendation of the 

restaurant to others the participants stated that they were more likely to recommend the restaurant to 

others (4,05 to 2,80 - the difference being 30,86%).    

These findings are line with the studies mentioned above (e.g. Langlois et al., 2000; Olson 

and Marshuetz, 2005; Elovitz et al., 2009; Verhulst et. al., 2010; Johnston, 2010; Bennett, 2010; 

Roberts, 2012; Toledano, 2013; Frevert & Walker, 2014; Boz and Yilmaz, 2017; Villi and Koc, 2018) 

which found favorable outcomes  relating to attractiveness of people.  The findings show that the 

distributional justice used in the scenarios, to some extent, has been instrumental in reinstating 

distributional justice in the participants.   

Overall, it can be seen that the attractiveness of the service employee (the waitress) had a 

positive effect both in terms of the service failure, as it was seen as a less of a problem and increased 

the satisfaction with the recovery attempt. However, it should be kept in mind that, in highly 

competitive markets, facial attractiveness cannot solve all of the problems.  Moreover, as found in 

Kim et al.’s study the attractiveness may not be a substitute for delivering a high quality service. 

Additionally, the idea of recruiting more attractive candidates for jobs has significant ethical and legal 

implications.   

The findings of this study also show that although attractiveness may lessen the negativity of 

the responses towards service failures, it may not solve all of the problems.  For instance, having in 

mind the fact that, the mean scores were out of 10, the highest figure of 5,40 (Table 1) may not be 

considered sufficient to establish a competitive advantage. Business firms with higher scores on these 

may be more likely to survive and prosper in the competitive markets.  

  

The Eye Tracker and Facial Recognition data were analyzed whether the service recovery 

paradox took place after the recovery of the service. The eye tracker data showed where the 

participants concentrated/focused on the materials they were shown.  

  

  
Figure 2: Attractive Waitress Scenario Eye Tracker Analysis Results  

  

            Figure 2 shows the eye tracker analysis results for the first scenario, i.e. service failure scenario 
with an attractive service employee. The results show that while the duration of the focus of the male 
participants on the verbal explanations in the scenario was 268 ms, the duration of the focus of the female 

participants on the verbal explanations in the scenario was 251 ms. On the other hand, while the duration 
of the focus of the male participants on the attractive female employee photo 459 ms, the duration of the  



BRAIN – Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience  

Volume 10, Issue 3 (September, 2019), ISSN 2067-3957  

102  

 

gaze of the female participants on the on the attractive female employee photo was 231 ms. This means 
that male participants spent more time focusing on the photo of the attractive service employee than they 
focused on the scenario itself, while the female participants spent more time focusing on the scenario 
itself.  

  

  
Figure 3. Less Attractive Waitress Scenario Eye Tracker Analysis Results  

  

Figure 2 shows the eye tracker analysis results for the second scenario, i.e. service failure scenario 
with a less attractive service employee. The results show that while the duration of the focus of the male 
participants on the verbal explanations in the scenario was 307 ms, the duration of the focus of the female 
participants on the verbal explanations in the scenario was 231 ms. On the other hand, while the male 

participants did not focus on the less attractive service employee photo at all, female participants focused 
on the less attractive service employee photo was 352 ms.  Interestingly, the female participants focused 
on the less attractive service employee photo more than they focused on the attractive service employee 

photo.  
  

Table 2. Facial Expression Scores of the Participants  

Feelings  Attractive Service Employee  Less Attractive Service Employee  

Before the 

Service Failure  
After the Service 

Recovery Attempt  
Before the 

Service Failure  
After the Service 

Recovery Attempt  

Surprise  0,250  0,340  0,246  0,260  

Anger  0,004  0,047  0,004  0,005  

Sadness  0,0720  0,0746  0,0720  0,0750  

Disgust  0,3800  0,3711  0,0234  0,0370  

Fear  0,057  0,006  0,006  0,007  

Happiness  0,038  0,027  0,002  0,002  



E. Koc, H. Boz, B. Boz - The Influence of Employee Attractiveness on Service Recovery Paradox: Facial Recognition 

and Eye Tracker Analyses  

103  

Neutral  0,792  0,790  0,854  0,838  

             

Table 2 shows the facial expression analysis results. The results mean that the service recovery 

paradox did not take place in both scenarios. In other words, the attractiveness of service employee 

had no effect on service recovery paradox. For instance, the scores for negative feelings (anger and 

sadness) did not decrease in both scenarios after they have been introduced to the service failure and 

service recovery.  Likewise, the positive feeling scores of happiness did not increase after the 

participants were introduced to the service failure and ensuing service recovery attempt.  

Research shows that service recovery paradox may not take place in all service recovery 

situations.  For instance, studies carried out by McCollough et al. (2000) and Michel and Meuter 

(2008) showed that service recovery paradox did not take place after the recovery of the service 

failure.  

There may be several reasons to explain why the service recovery may not occur due to factors, 

ranging from the severity of the failure to the cultural factors and from personality characteristics to 

various other factors such as the service sector the study conducted in (Magnini et al., 2007; Du et al., 

2011).  For instance, Koc et al.’s (2018) that study showed in restraint cultures, as opposed to 

indulgence cultures, service recovery may not take place due to the fact that people from restraint 

cultures were more likely to remember negative events such as a service failure.  

  

6. Limitations of the Study and Recommendations for Future Research  

As one of the limitations of the study, the number of people (20) can be stated. Future studies 

may investigate the concept of service recovery paradox with larger numbers and perhaps supported 

with focus group studies. A larger group of participants in the experiment supported by focus group 

studies may not only increase the validity and reliability of the study but may also enable gaining a 

deeper insight into the service recovery paradox phenomenon.  

In the study service recovery paradox was mainly concerned with reinstating distributional 

justice, i.e. compensating the losses incurred by the customer.  Future studies may investigate the 

influence of attractiveness in relation to other types of justice, i.e. procedural justice and interactional 

justice. Additionally, this study investigated the occurrence of service recovery paradox in restraint 

cultures.  Future studies may be carried out in indulgence cultures or both cultures where and which 

the findings may be compared.  

  

7. Conclusions  

The study has found that the attractiveness of hospitality employees can play a role in terms 

of influencing customers’ perceptions of service failures.  Although the attractiveness of the service 

employees may mitigate the responses of customers service failures, service recovery paradox may 

not occur no mater service employees are attractive or not. In other words, customers may not easily 

forget what happened during the service failure and remember those negative feelings even after the 

service has been recovered. Based on the findings of this study, hospitality managers can be 

recommended to establish better quality assurance systems in their businesses.  It should also be borne 

in mind that the attractiveness of service employees may not be a substitute for providing a high-

quality service.  

  

References   

Armistead, C. G., Clarke, G. and Stanley, P. (1995). Managing service recovery, Cranfield, England: 

Cranfield School of Management.  

Baker, M. A. (2017). Service failures and recovery: theories and models. In Service Failures and 

Recovery in Tourism and Hospitality: A Practical Manual, 24-41.  

Bennett, J. (2010, July 26). The beauty advantage. Newsweek, 156(4), 46–48.   



BRAIN – Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience  

Volume 10, Issue 3 (September, 2019), ISSN 2067-3957  

104  

 

Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). Beauty and the Best. Psychology Today, 5, 42–46.  

Blodgett, J. G., Hill, D. J., and Tax, S. S. (1997) The effects of distributive, procedural, and 

interactional justice on post complaint behavior. Journal of Retailing, 73(2), 185-210.  

Boz, H. (2015). Turistik Ürün Satın Alma Karar Sürecinde İtkiselliğin Rolü:  

Psikonörobiyokimyasal Analiz, Unpublished Doctoral Thesis. Balıkesir, Turkey: Balikesir 

University, Institute of Social Sciences.  

Boz, H. and Yilmaz, O. (2017). An eye tracker analysis of the influence of applicant attractiveness 

on employee recruitment process: A neuromarketing study. Ecoforum Journal, 6(1).  

Boz, H., & Kose, U. (2018). Emotion Extraction from Facial Expressions by Using Artificial 

Intelligence Techniques. BRAIN. Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience, 

9(1), 5-16.  

Braun, C., Gruendl, M., Marberger, C. & Scherber, C. (2001). Beautycheck - Ursachen und Folgen 

von Attraktivitaet.  

Du, J., Fan, X., & Feng, T. (2011). Multiple emotional contagions in service encounters. Journal of 

the Academy of Marketing Science, 39(3), 449-466.  

Elovitz, G. P., & Salvia, J. (1982). Attractiveness as a biasing factor in the judgments of 

schoolpsychologists. Journal of School Psychology, 20, 339–345.   

Frevert, T. K., & Walker, L. S. (2014). Physical attractiveness and social status. Sociology Compass, 

8(3), 313–323.  

Ha, J. and Jang, S., (2009), Perceived justice in service recovery and behavioral intentions: The role 

of relationship quality, International Journal of Hospitality Management, 28(3), 319-327.   

Johnston, D. W. (2010). Physical appearance and wages: Do blondes have more fun? Economics 

Letters, 108(1), 10–12.  

Keaveney, S. M. (1995), Customer Switching Behavior in Service Industries: An Exploratory Study, 

Journal of Marketing, (59)2, 71-82.  

Kelley, S. W., Hoffman, K. D. and Davis, M. A. (1993). A typology of retail failures and recoveries, 

Journal of Retailing, 69(4), 429-452.  

Koc, Erdogan; Hakan Boz (2018). How Can Consumer Science Be Used for Gaining Information 

About Consumers and the Market?: The role of psychophysiological and neuromarketing 

research. In Case Studies in the Traditional Food Sector, 129-152.  

Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). 

Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 

126, 390–423.   

Lee, Y. L., Sparks, B., & Butcher, K. (2013). Service encounters and face loss: Issues of failures, 

fairness, and context. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 34, 384-393.  

Kim, K., & Baker, M. A. (2017). How the Employee Looks and Looks at You: Building Customer– 

Employee Rapport. Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research.  

Koc, E. (2013) Power distance and its implications for upward communication and empowerment:  

crisis management and recovery in hospitality services. The International Journal of Human 

Resource Management, 24, 3681–3696  

Koc, E., & Boz, H. (2014). Triangulation in tourism research: A bibliometric study of top three 

tourism journals. Tourism Management Perspectives, 12, 9-14.  

Koc, E. (2017). Service failures and recovery in tourism and hospitality: A practical manual. 

Wallingford.  

Koc, E., Aydin, G., Ar, A. A. and Boz, H. (2017). Emotions and Emotional Abilities in Service 

Failures and Recovery In Koc (ed.) Service Failures and Recovery in Tourism and Hospitality: 

A Practical Manual, 42-55.  

Koc, E., Yilmaz, O., and Boz, H. (2018). Dizginleyici Kültürlerde Hizmet Telafisi Paradoksu: Turizm 

ve Konaklama Sektörü, 23. Ulusal Pazarlama Kongresi.  



E. Koc, H. Boz, B. Boz - The Influence of Employee Attractiveness on Service Recovery Paradox: Facial Recognition 

and Eye Tracker Analyses  

105  

 

Koc, E. (2019). Service failures and recovery in hospitality and tourism: a review of literature and 

recommendations for future research. Journal of Hospitality Marketing & Management, 28(5), 

513-537.  

Koç, E. (2019b). Hizmet pazarlaması ve yönetimi. Baskı 3, Seçkin Yayınları: Ankara.  

Ma, Correll, & Wittenbrink (2015). The Chicago Face Database: A Free Stimulus Set of Faces and 

Norming Data. Behavior Research Methods, 47, 1122-1135.  

Magnini, V. P., Ford, J. B., Markowski, E. P., & Honeycutt Jr, E. D. (2007). The service recovery 

paradox: justifiable theory or smoldering myth?. Journal of Services Marketing, 21(3), 

213225.  

Magnini, V. P., Baker, M., & Karande, K. (2013). The frontline provider’s appearance: A driver of 

guest perceptions. Cornell Hospitality Quarterly, 54(4), 396-405.  

Mattila, A. S. (2001), The effectiveness of service recovery in a multi-industry setting, Journal of 

Services Marketing, 15(7), 583-596.  

McCollough, M.A. and Bharadwaj, S.G. (1992), The recovery paradox: an examination of customer 

satisfaction in relation to disconfirmation, service quality, and attribution based theories', in 

Allen, C.T. (Ed.), Marketing Theory and Applications, American Marketing Association, 

Chicago, IL.  

Miller, J., Craighead, C. & Karwan, K. (2000). Service recovery: A framework and empirical 

investigation. Journal of Operations Management, 18(4), 387-400.  

McCollough, M., Berry, L. and Yadav, M. (2000). An Empirical Investigation of Customer 

Satisfaction after Service Failure and Recovery. Journal of Service Research, 3(2), 121-137.  

Olson, I. R., & Marshuetz, C. (2005). Facial attractiveness is appraised in a glance. Emotion, 5(4), 

498–502.   

Patterson, P. F., Cowley, E. and Prasongsukarn, K. (2006), Service failure recovery: The moderating 

impact of individual-level cultural value orientation on perceptions of justice International 

Journal of Research in Marketing, 23(3), 263-277.  

Pranić, L., & Roehl, W. S. (2013). Development and validation of the customer empowerment scale 

in hotel service recovery. Current Issues in Tourism, 16(4), 369-387.  

Roberts, S. C. (2012). Evolution, appearance, and occupational success. Evolutionary Psychology, 

10, 782–801.  

Salter, S. P., Mixon, J. F. G., & King, E. W. (2012). Broker beauty and boon: A study of physical 

attractiveness and its effect on real estate brokers’ income and productivity. Applied Financial 

Economics, 22(10), 811–825.   

Smith, A. K., Bolton, R. N., & Wagner, J. (1999). A model of customer satisfaction with service 

encounters involving failure and recovery. Journal of marketing research, 356-372.  

Sparks, B. A., & McColl-Kennedy, J. R. (2001). Justice strategy options for increased customer 

satisfaction in a services recovery setting. Journal of Business Research, 54(3), 209-218.  

Taşkın, Ç., Koç, E., & Boz, H. (2017). Perceptual Image of Conflict-Ridden Destinations: An EEG 

and Eye Tracker Analysis 1. Business and Economics Research Journal, 8(3), 533.  

Tax, S. S., Brown, S. W., and Chandrashekaran, M. (1998) Customer evaluations of service complaint 

experiences: Implications for relationship marketing. Journal of Marketing, 62, 60-76.  

Toledano, E. (2013). May the best (looking) man win: The unconscious role of attractiveness in 

employment decisions. Cornell HR Review, Retrieved from Cornell University, ILR School 

site.  

Verhulst, B., Lodge, M., & Lavine, H. (2010). The attractiveness halo: Why some candidates are 

perceived more favorably than others. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34(2), 111–117.  

Villi, B., & Koc, E. (2018). Employee Attractiveness and Customers’ Service Failure Perceptions. 

Journal of Hospitality Marketing & Management, 27(1), 41-60.  

 



BRAIN – Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience  

Volume 10, Issue 3 (September, 2019), ISSN 2067-3957  

106  

 

Wen, B., & Chi, C. G. Q. (2013), Examine the cognitive and affective antecedents to service recovery 

satisfaction: A field study of delayed airline passengers”, International Journal of 

Contemporary Hospitality Management, 25(30), 306-327.