Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021 Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 https://doi.org/10.15826/csp.2021.5.3.147 Received 29 March 2021 © Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, Accepted 1 October 2021 Bolanle Tawakalitu Olatunji, Published online 11 October 2021 Grace Oluwatobi Ogunwale, Olawale Julius Aluko fausatibrahim@gmail.com bensonosikabor@yahoo.com bolanletawakalitu7@gmail.com tobi.ayodele@yahoo.com juliwal2002@yahoo.com ARTICLE Forest in the Context of Social Change: Traditional Orientation and Forest Mystification in a Nigerian Forest-Reserve Setting Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, Bolanle Tawakalitu Olatunji, Grace Oluwatobi Ogunwale, Olawale Julius Aluko Forestry Research Institute of Nigeria, Ibadan, Oyo State, Nigeria ABSTRACT This article exposits the mystification of forests among people residing in proximity to a forest reserve in southwestern Nigeria. The theory of material engagement and the ecology of human development support the position that the forest is a classical motivator of traditional culture. Still, socio-cultural change is prevalent. As an element of this change, forest-based social cognition warrants systematic examination in the interest of environmental sustainability. This is because the concurrent conveyance of sustainability-promoting immaterial culture across generations is a component of the pathway to a sustainable future. Moreover, systems theory posits that social events affect each other. Since social change is not solitary but encompassing, forest mystification was examined along with other indicators of traditional orientation including attitudes towards religion, ageing and gender, as well as cultural enthusiasm. The results indicate that forest mystification is still huge and connected with orientations towards ageing and cultural enthusiasm. This exemplifies the Yorùbá social context’s manifestation of continuity as opposed to change in forest culture; and stands in solidarity with traditional African mentality. KEYWORDS social change, forest mystification, Africa, Yorùbá, traditional orientation, age, culture, religion, gender https://changing-sp.com/ Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 497 Introduction From time immemorial, human’s natural environment is the base of his subsistence and cultural progress, showcasing the interconnectivity and interdependence of humans and their persisting and evolving ecosystem. This presumption underlies the focal argument of the Material Engagement Theory (MET), which places the material world at the heart of human cognitive development and change. MET is a platform expounding the active function of material culture or things in the composition of the human mind (Roberts, 2016). It is a denunciation of the Cartesian dichotomic separation of the mind, which is regarded as active; and the material world, which is construed to be passive (Graham, 2019; Roberts, 2016). It promotes the ideology that there is a dynamic relationship between the material world and the cognitive world, with the former promoting the development of the latter. Material culture is therefore a tool of analysis for cognitive science (Malafouris, 2004) and of course, human social thought. It is an active determinant of culture and who we are. Material things ecologize and educate the mind of the individual and the collective (Graham, 2019). Further, MET is synonymous with Bronfenbrenner’s (1977, 1979) ecology of human development, which canvasses principles and analytical traditions that underscore the interconnectedness of human lives/outcomes in changing ecological contexts. Social cognition is said to be ecological-context-dependent, making social thoughts develop pari-passu with human ecology (Kilanowski, 2017; Lyons et al., 2019). The theoretical supposition of MET and the ecological perspective explicates the logic of the Yorùbá socio-cultural reality. The forest is a classical locale of traditional culture among many peoples including the Yorùbá of southwestern Nigeria. The Yorùbá people are typically referred to as ọmọ káàárọ̀-oòjíire, ọmọ Oòduà (literally, the one who greets themselves in the morning by asking how the other is faring, descendants of Odùduwà). The people are one of the three dominant ethnic groups in Nigeria (the others being the Igbo of the southeast and the Hausa/Fulani people of northern Nigeria). Being one of Nigeria’s six geopolitical divisions, the southwestern region and its Yorùbá people constitute about 18% (Okolie et al., 2018) of Nigeria’s population, which is over 200 million (Population Division, 2019). The term Yorùbá evolved as a nomenclature to describe the people in the early 19th century (Ajala, 2009). The need for such a nomenclature was borne out of the “internationalisation of political and economic forces, which favours generalization” (Ibrahim, 2020, p. 750). Prior to the 19th century, Yorùbá people were autonomous groupings and they recognized themselves according to different Yorùbá dialects such as the Ijebu, Ifẹ, and Ọyọ (Ibrahim, 2020, p. 750). Despite the dialectic divisions, the Yorùbá share tremendous cultural features including myths regarding the origin of human being and the recognition of Odùduwà the prime ancestral figure (Ibrahim & Jegede, 2017a). This underscores the homogeneity of the cohorts that make up the Yorùbá people. The Yorùbá see human being as being on a journey between ayé (the world) and ọrun (heaven). The world is perceived as a market place whereas heaven is human being’s real home. At the same time, ọrun is projected as a very mystical place comprising supernatural bodies. Yet, the world is also believed to be a https://changing-sp.com/ 498 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko strong manifestation of the supernatural bodies that ideally belong to ọrun. Hence, the world is perceived as being manoeuvred by supernatural forces. The Yorùbá strongly believe that there is “an interface of the visible and invisible, the tangible and intangible, the known and unknown[…]; the act of looking and seeing in Yorùbá culture is much more than a perception of objects by use of the eyes” (Lawal, 2001, p. 521). The people believe that “the invisible world is symbolized or manifested by visible and concrete phenomena and objects of nature” (Jegede, 2002, p. 323). For instance, in an interpretative analysis of Yorùbá proverbs, Ibrahim (2016) reported that forests and trees are “humanized” by the people. Yorùbá proverbs convey the idea that trees are seen and referenced like human beings in the culture. Further, the forest and its physical dynamics reverberate through the socially constructed knowledge of the Yorùbá. In this culture, the forest is a mystified cultural creation.Adeduntan asserted as follows: To the Yorùbá, the bush or forest is not just the habitat of flora and fauna, but also of spirits – iwin, ẹbọra, ànjọ̀ọ̀nú, sẹrankosènìyàn, ọ̀rọ̀, and so on. In other words, the bush or forest is a realm of the infinite where the giant rat may tie up the hunter’s dog, the ìrókò (a tree, the Chlorophora excelsa) might tell the hunter in which direction to seek game, and porcupines could perform in a concert. (Adeduntan, 2019, p. 2) As noted earlier, mystification is generously featured in Yorùbá culture, not just the forest-related. Perhaps, the greatest indication of the mythological concentration of Yorùbá thoughts about forests is encapsulated in the writings of Daniel O. Fágúnwà (for instance, 1949, 1954), whose fictional expositions capture a true picture of cultural dynamics among the Yorùbá. Nobel Laureate Wole Soyinka asserted that Fágúnwà’s novels are robust projections of Yorùbá culture, which is supernaturalistic and “densely mythological” (Soyinka, 2010, p. 3). Smith (2001) also concurred that the novels purvey Yorùbá culture. The literal title of some of these novels including “Ògbójú Ọdẹ nínú Igbó Irúnmọlè” (Fágúnwà, 1950, translated as The Forest of a Thousand Daemons by Soyinka, 1982), is a remarkable projection of how Yorùbá culture and mentality is shrouded around their material physical environment which is dominated by rich forestall vegetation. Certainly, the MET and the ecological perspective find vindication in the Yorùbá socio-cultural parlance. At the same time, the omnipresence of socio-cultural change in human communities globally is resounding. Analysing data from four waves of the World Values Survey conducted between 1984 and 2004 with participants from 72 countries, Li and Bond (2010) reported evidence indicating that people are globally becoming more modern or secular in their orientation which is independent of socio-economic development. Social change is prevalent everywhere including among the Yorùbá due to several factors, especially western contact (Lawal, 2001; Olurode & Olusanya, 1994). Notwithstanding, cultural survival is also prominently featured especially in African contexts. For instance, Opefeyitimi (2009) asserted that, despite modernity and change, Yorùbá people subscribe to paranormal explanations, which populate their worldview. Jegede (2002) similarly reported how supernatural entities are Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 499 important aspects of the perception of disease aetiology, making people engage in cultural practices such as charm-use and scarification to earn health. In the report of a qualitative study conducted among smallholder farmers to explore beliefs regarding climate change in northwestern Nigeria, Jellason et al. (2020) expounded how participants dominantly practised socio-cultural adaption to climate change such as making sacrifices and praying to God. Indeed, traditional orientation is still reckoned with. This orientation is premised on being connected with pristine culture. However, this orientation is also a continuum that features modernistic orientation at one of its ends. Determining the connection between traditional orientation (using specific indicators) and the traditionality of orientation towards the forest is the prime goal of this study. This goal is consequential for sustainability especially from socio-ecological and MET’s perspectives. Forest-Based Social Cognition to Track Social Change in the Interest of Sustainability Borrowing from the much-referenced Bruntland Commission’s definition of sustainability, the current generation ought to consider the needs of future generations and decline from defiling future generation’s capacity to fulfil such needs (World Commission, 1987). Invariably, sustainability is a proposal for continuity or change, whichever is in favour of responsible use of environmental resources. From the socio-ecological perspective and MET’s conception of the mind and material synergy, people think amidst rather than a detached environment. Consequently, sustainability ought to necessarily encompass the conveyance of immaterial culture/mindset of sustainability in addition to material resources. Both variables are essential elements of sustainability, which direct any pathway to a sustainable future. Sustainability entails the ecology of the mindset of current and future generations. In other words, people’s persisting and evolving mindset or community psychology, which developed in the context of certain material entities such as forests, deserve to be focused. Examining prevailing forest mystification in the interest of tracking change in social thought is therefore justifiable. Moreover, it is established that social change is the outcome of changing human cognition, which is generated in an ecological context (Alwin & McCammon, 2003; Bandura, 2001, 2004). While mystification manifests itself in individual behaviour, it originates from a forest-ecological context. Examining forest mystification is therefore tantamount to examining the relationship between the individual and society, which is a classical question of all time. This examination is a potential indicator of the social change being created in the Yorùbá social context. Further, social change entails a shift in the pattern of people’s behaviour, a difference in their ways of participating in the world. This behavioural component of social change is preceded by the cognitive and emotional components of change (Graham, 2019). Still, cognitive, emotive and behavioural components exist interconnectedly. Social change is manifested in social practices and multiplied through mindsets. Social cognitions regarding forest, the emotions that forests emit and people’s forest- https://changing-sp.com/ 500 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko related behaviour are therefore intertwined. Forest mystification is the quintessence of cognitive-emotive components, which precedes forest-related behaviour whose examination is therefore warranted. The criticisms against evolutionary theories have shown that social change is not unilinear (de Vries, 2017; Gedeon, 2018). Instead, social change encompasses several aspects of society. The focal contribution of systems theory, which undertones material engagement theory and the ecological perspective is simply that one thing affects the other (Mandavilli, 2018; Strain, 2007). Human social life occurs by the ongoing interaction among forces including physical and cognitive, as well as cognitive versus cognitive phenomena. It is virtually impossible to fully appreciate changing or continuing forest mystification for instance, without taking into account other elements of traditional orientation in the Yorùbá social context. Social phenomena are complex because of their interconnectedness. This complexity must be acknowledged by considering forest mystification in the context of other traditional social thought. Developing a model of traditional orientation to examine its influence on forest culture or mystification is therefore apposite. Hence, the articulation of social change on the premise of forest culture is appealing especially in a forest-shrouded Yorùbá social context. A speculative model of traditional orientation featuring attitude to―religion, ageing, gender; and cultural enthusiasm―to establish influencers of forest culture or mystification was therefore developed. The correlations between each element of this model and forest mystification were examined. Attitude towards Religion, Ageing, Gender and Cultural Enthusiasm as Traditional Orientation Religion is a ubiquitous social phenomenon that permeates virtually all facets of human society (Ives & Kidwell, 2019), and reflects people’s identity construction and decision-making (Kose & Cinar, 2020). About 84% of mankind is associated with a religion (The Changing Global Religious Landscape, 2020). Meanwhile, there is a great diversity of beliefs and practices across and within world religions. In pre-colonial Africa, people were engrossed with the traditional religion, which has deep connections with the culture and practices of the people. Contemporary religions especially the Abrahamic ones are precariously practised by the people. Attitude towards religion is a focal indicator of traditional orientation because religious orientation and supernaturality becomes attenuated with increasing modernity (Durkheim, 1912/1976; Hardy et al., 2020; Weber, 1946). Secularization, science and increasing rationality is associated with modernity (Berger, 1999; Edgell, 2006; Norris & Inglehart 2004; Wuthrow, 2007). In addition, increasing change in human societies characterized by urbanization and westernization is concomitantly associated with increasing negative attitudes towards ageing. Youthfulness is prized in modern contemporary society while ageing is linked with poor health, reliance on others and lonesomeness (Black, 2009; Kalfoss, 2017). Fealy et al. (2012) reported that older people are collectively Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 501 stigmatized with several negative identities like fragility, infirmity, vulnerability, etc. Yet, in more traditional societies such as in precolonial Yorùbá society, age is revered. The anthropological study reported by Schwab (1955) indicated that everyday life in Yorùbá society features the principle of seniority tremendously. Gerontocracy is generously featured in the socio-political life of the Yorùbá (Fadipe, 1970). Hence, the traditional attitude towards ageing is, by all means, an indicator of traditionality. The attitude towards gender is crucial signification of traditional orientation or otherwise. Perhaps the most typical phenomenon characteristic of all human societies in their historical past is the subservient position of women as opposed to men (Ibrahim, 2019). Yet, the egalitarian positioning of women bears positive consequences for national economies (World Bank, 2012). Social values hardly advocate women empowerment, especially in more traditional settings. Egalitarian gender roles underlie societal advancement and such equity becomes vivid with increasing development. No wonder gender inequities are more characteristic of less developing countries (Duflo, 2012) like Nigeria. Such countries tend to demonstrate unequal attitude towards gender, thus marking the commitment towards traditional orientation. Cultural enthusiasm is people’s keenness and excitement regarding their traditional, pristine culture. It is very conceptually close to cultural conservatism, which is about protecting ancient culture (Crowson, 2009). Studies have shown that cultural conservatism/enthusiasm is positively related to self-esteem (Crowson, 2009; van Hiel & Brebels, 2011). Cultural enthusiasm represents traditionalism (van Hiel & Kossowska, 2007) and is, therefore, a prime indicator of traditional orientation. These variables―attitudes towards religion, ageing, gender and cultural enthusiasm―collectively constitute the model of indicators of traditional orientation in this study (see Figure 1). Each of these variables is expected to influence forest mystification in the target population. Invariably, the major objective of this study is to examine the influence of each variable on forest mystification. Forest mystification was highlighted as a variable, while the effects of respondents’ sex, education, age, religion and ethnicity on this mystification were also determined. Figure 1 Traditional Orientation and Its Indicators TRADITIONAL ORIENTATION Attitude towards religion Attitude towards gender Attitude towards ageing Cultural enthusiasm https://changing-sp.com/ 502 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko Materials and Methods The Study Sites The study sites include eight communities within a 1–5 km radius (see Table 1) of the Onigambari forest reserve. The reserve is located in Oluyole local government area (LGA) of Oyo state, southwestern Nigeria. Nigeria’s southwestern region is the homeland of the Yorùbá people. Oyo state is one of the six states constituting the southwestern region while Oluyole LGA is one of the 33 LGAs in the state. The Onigambari forest reserve is located on latitude 7⁰8′N and 7⁰3′N longitude 3⁰49′E and 3⁰22′E. It was declared a reserve in 1899 by Ibadan city council―the British-colonially created unit that included indigenous chiefs in the administration of Ibadan. The reserve occupies a land area of 13932.18 hectares. Table 1 Projected and Sampled Population of Study Sites Study sites 1991 population Projected population (2000) @ 2.5% growth rate Projected population (2010) @ 2.6% growth rate Projected population (2020) @ 2.6% growth rate Proportionally sampled population Olubi 297 372 482 625 22 Onigambari 600 751 974 1,263 44 Oloowa 95 119 154 200 07 Busogboro 532 666 864 1,121 39 Onipe 410 513 665 862 30 Dalli 351 439 569 738 25 Olonde 215 269 349 453 16 Seriki 102 128 166 215 07 Total 2,602 3,257 4,223 5,477 190 Study Design/Sampling Procedure This study is a snap-shot, non-experimental cross-sectional survey targeting members of communities around the Onigambari forest reserve, Nigeria. Eight communities adjoining the reserve were identified. The population of each community as at the year 2000, 2010 and 2020 were projected. These projections were based on the population of the communities as declared at the end of the 1991 national census (National Population Commission, 1998), using the formula: = ×0 rtP P e Where: P = final population, P0 = initial population, r = growth rate (at varying growth rates, see Table 1), t = time interval (varying time interval, see Table 1). The projected population of the communities is 5477 and was taken as total population (N) for the study. The following modified version of the Cochran formula was then used to calculate sample size: = − + 2 2 2( 1) n Npqz l N pqz Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 503 Where n is the sample size, N, the population size = 5477, p, the (estimated) proportion of the population that has the attribute in question at assumption of 50% = 0.5; q is 1–p; z is the z-value obtained from z-table for 95% confidence which is 1.96; and l is the desired level of precision (i.e., the margin of error) which is set to 7% or 0.07. The required sample size was 189.3 ≈ 190. The selection of respondents from the eight sampling units was based on proportional representation: the projected population of each community was divided by the total population of the eight communities (5477) and multiplied by 190. The number of samples drawn from each community is shown in Table 1. Instrument of Data Collection A structured questionnaire was used to collect data. The questionnaire was translated into the Yorùbá language to ease its administration among respondents who do not speak the English language. The questionnaire was administered through structured interviews. The aim of the study was explained to every potential respondent while their anonymity was assured. They were enjoined to offer their consent to participate in the study. Respondents indicated their informed consent by appending their signature in an informed consent form. Definition/Measurement of Variables • Attitude to religion was defined in this context as the degree to which respondents are confident in their religion. It was measured with the four- item turning to religion sub-scale of the multidimensional coping inventory, assessing how people deal with stress (Carver et al., 1989). Samples of items include: “I put my trust in God”, “I try to find comfort in my religion”. Responses were “totally true (2)”, “fairly true (1)” and “not true at all (0)”. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.745. • Attitude to ageing was defined as the respondent’s evaluation of the extent to which pleasurable things and respect are attracted to older age. This was assessed using four of the eight-item psychological growth sub-scale of the attitude to ageing scale (Laidlaw et al., 2007)1. Responses were “totally true (2)”, “fairly true (1)” and “not true at all (0)”. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.734. Examples of items are: “there are many pleasant things about growing older”, “I am more accepting of myself as I have grown older”. • Attitude to gender was operationally defined as the degree of respondent’s acceptance of egalitarianism in the assignment of social roles between men and women. It was measured with the eight-item gender-linked sub-scale of the social role questionnaire (Baber & Tucker, 2006). Responses included “strongly agree (4)”, “agree (3)”, “disagree (2)” and “strongly disagree (1)”. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.676. Examples of items are: “for many important jobs, it is better to choose men instead of women”, “mothers should work only if necessary”. 1 The four selected items are reflective of peoples’ attitude to ageing irrespective of their age, unlike other items. https://changing-sp.com/ 504 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko • Cultural enthusiasm was defined as respondent’s zeal regarding their culture and was assessed with the four-item tradition sub-scale of the personal cultural orientations (Sharma, 2010). Responses were also “totally true (2)”, “fairly true (1)” and “not true at all (0)”. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.736. Samples of items include: “I am proud of my culture”, “traditional values are important for me”. • Forest mystification was defined as the respondent’s evaluation of the forest as a supernatural and paranormal spatial entity. It was assessed with a seven- item author-constructed scale whose response categories were “strongly agree (4)”, “agree (3)”, “disagree (2)” and “strongly disagree (1)”. The scale was reliable (Cronbach’s alpha was 0.885). There was a significant and positive correlation (r = 0.193, p < 0.05) between summary scores of the transcendence conviction sub-scale of the aspects of spirituality questionnaire (Büssing et al., 2014) and the author-constructed scale of forest mystification. This is taken as an indication of the convergent validity of the author-constructed scale (see Table 3 for items of this scale). All items were scored such that the higher the score, the more traditional the attitude to religion, ageing and gender; the more traditional the cultural enthusiasm, the stronger the forest mystification. Respondent’s scores were simply aggregated. Study Hypotheses The general null hypotheses tested are as follows: • H1: There will be no significant difference in the mean score of forest mystification across sub-groups of age, religion, ethnicity, sex and education. • H2: There will be no significant relationship between pairs of attitude to religion, attitude to ageing, attitude to gender, cultural enthusiasm and forest mystification. Statistical Analyses Descriptive analyses of distributions of socio-demographic characteristics were conducted using frequencies and percentages. Means and standard deviations were used for the descriptive analysis of items in the author-constructed scale of forest mystification. The one-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to assess the normalcy of distribution of all interval-measured variables. Homogeneity of variance across sub-groups of sex, education, age, religion and ethnicity was tested using Levene’s test. When homogeneity was confirmed across sub-groups of age, religion and ethnicity, equality of mean scores of forest mystification were tested using one-way ANOVA. When homogeneity was not confirmed across sub-groups of sex and education, the Mann-Whitney U test and the Kruskal-Wallis test with the pairwise multiple-comparison post-hoc test were used to assess the effects of sex and education on forest mystification. Epsilon and epsilon2 were measures of effect sizes. Pearson correlation coefficient (r) was used to examine the nature of the relationships between pairs of attitude to religion, attitude to ageing, attitude to gender, cultural enthusiasm and forest mystification. Data were analyzed using the IBM SPSS (Statistical Package for Social Sciences) version 23.0. Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 505 Results Distribution of Respondents’ Socio-Demographic Characteristics Most respondents were male (64.7%) while females constituted 35.3%. The mean ± SD age of respondents was 37.13 ± 11.21 (minimum = 19, maximum = 64). The distribution of respondents’ age category resembles an inverted U, which reached a peak at the 36–45 age sub-group. Most respondents (53.2%) had secondary education, while a noticeable proportion (14.5%) had no formal education. Another noticeable proportion (10.8%) had post-secondary education while 3 respondents were holders of the first degree. More than half of respondents (52.6%) were Christians while Muslims were also vast (42.6%). Nine respondents (4.7%) identified themselves as practitioners of traditional religion. Yorùbá were expectedly predominant (82.1%). Hausa respondents were noticeable (10.5%) while other ethnic nationalities including Igbo, Igbira, Igede and Idoma were marginally represented in the study. The distributions of respondents’ socio-demographic characteristics are shown in Table 2. Table 2 Distribution of Respondents’ Socio-Demographic Characteristics (N = 190) Socio-demographic characteristic Sub-groups Frequency Percentage Sex Male 123 64.7 Female 67 35.3 Age* 16–25 31 16.3 26–35 57 30.0 36–45 61 32.1 46–55 28 14.7 56–65 13 6.8 Highest education attained No formal education 27 14.5 Primary 37 19.9 Secondary 99 53.2 Post-secondary 20 10.8 First degree 3 1.6 No response 4 2.1 Religion Islam 81 42.6 Christianity 100 52.6 Traditional 9 4.7 Ethnicity Yorùbá 156 82.1 Hausa 20 10.5 Igbo 7 3.7 Others** 7 3.7 Note. * The mean ± SD of age was 37.13 ± 11.21, minimum = 19, maximum = 64. ** Igbira, Igede, Idoma. https://changing-sp.com/ 506 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko Univariate Analysis of Forest Mystification/Items in the Author-Constructed Scale of Forest Mystification The mean score of forest mystification was 23.70 ± 3.62 (range was 18 to 28). This mean is high and indicative of a strong manifestation of forest mystification in the study area. The item statistics on Table 3 shows that respondent’s rating of items on the scale of forest mystification were generally similar and their agreement with items was high because the mean score of these items were all above 3 while the maximum score was 4. The generic item maintaining that forest is not just the habitat of flora and fauna but also of spirits attracted the strongest rating (mean = 3.65), followed by the item which upheld that there are creatures in the forest that cannot be seen with ordinary eyes (mean = 3.55). Interestingly, the item affirming that there are some animals in forests that are spiritual and should not be consumed attracted the weakest agreement (mean = 3.17), suggesting that mystification weakens in the face of consumable things. The idea that there are mysterious trees in forests also attracted weak agreement (mean = 3.31), suggesting weaker mystification of trees when compared with spirits and other invincible creatures. Table 3 Item Statistics, Indicators of Reliability and Validity of the Author-constructed Scale Forest mystification Mean ± SD Minimum Maximum Indicator of reliability (Crobach’s alpha) Indicator of validity (Pearson’s r) Forest is not just the habitat of flora and fauna but also of spirits 3.65±0.57 2 4 0.885 0.193, p = 0.008 Special creatures that are neither humans nor animals are there in forests 3.43±0.73 1 4 There are creatures in the forest that cannot be seen with ordinary eyes 3.55±0.63 2 4 Trees like ìrókò can provide directions to hunters in the forest 3.29±0.76 1 4 There is no deity (orisa) that does not have connection with forest 3.29±0.73 1 4 There are some animals in forest that are spiritual and should not be consumed 3.17±0.66 1 4 There are mysterious trees in forest 3.31±0.64 1 4 Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 507 Effect of Age, Religion, Ethnicity, Sex and Education on Forest Mystification Information contained in Table 4 shows that the oldest age sub-group made up of respondents aged between 56 and 65 years held the strongest forest mystification (mean ± SD = 25.6 ± 2.5). This was followed by the next age sub-group (46–55) whose mean ± SD was 24.2 ± 2.6. The 26–35 age sub-group held stronger forest mystification (mean ± SD = 24.0 ± 3.8) when compared with 36–45 sub-group (mean ± SD = 23.1 ± 3.9) while weakest mystification was exhibited by the youngest age sub-group of 16 to 25 years (mean ± SD = 22.9 ± 3.2). These descriptive statistics generically suggests that forest mystification strengthens with increasing age. There is homogeneity of variance across sub-groups of age regarding forest mystification (p > 0.05) while the result of one-way ANOVA shows that these means are not significantly different (p > 0.05). Hence, age had no significant effect on forest mystification. Forest was most strongly mystified among Muslims (mean ± SD = 24.0 ± 3.1). The extent to which Christians (mean ± SD = 23.4 ± 4.0) and practitioners of traditional religion (mean ± SD = 23.3 ± 3.2) mystified forest was very comparable. However, there was no significant difference in means across sub- groups of religion with regard to forest mystification (p > 0.05). Hence, respondents’ religion also had no significant effect on forest mystification. Respondents who belonged to the Yorùbá ethnic group held the strongest extent of forest mystification (mean ± SD = 24.0 ± 3.6) while Hausa respondents were stronger in terms of this mystification (mean ± SD = 23.1 ± 2.7). Igbo respondents held the weakest extent of forest mystification (mean ± SD = 22.9 ± 4.0). However, there was no significant difference in these means (p > 0.05). The results in Table 5 indicate that male respondents exhibited a stronger extent of forest mystification (mean rank = 102.73) Table 4 Effects of Age, Religion and Ethnicity on Forest Mystification Socio- demographic variable Sub-groups Mean±SD Levene’s test for homogeneity of variances ANOVA Levene’s statistic p value F statistic p value Age 16–25 22.9 ± 3.2 1.665 0.160 1.848 .122 26–35 24.0 ± 3.8 36–45 23.1 ± 3.9 46–55 24.2 ± 2.6 56–65 25.6 ± 2.5 Religion Islam 24.0 ± 3.1 2.691 0.070 0.695 0.500Christianity 23.4 ± 4.0 Traditional 23.3 ± 3.2 Ethnicity Yorùbá 24.0 ± 3.6 1.454 0.218 1.090 0.367Igbo 22.9 ± 4.0 Hausa 23.1 ± 2.7 https://changing-sp.com/ 508 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko when compared with female respondents (mean rank = 80.93). The result of the Mann-Whitney U test shows that these mean ranks are significantly different (p < 0.05). Hence, sex has an effect on forest mystification. Epsilon was 0.188 while epsilon2 was 0.035. Therefore, 3.5% of the variance in forest mystification is explained by sex. Forest mystification was very similar between, and most strongly held by the holders of primary (mean rank = 98.42) and secondary education (mean rank = 98.70). Forest mystification became weakened a little among respondents having no formal education (mean rank = 89.44) and was even weaker among those having post-secondary education (mean rank = 51.98) while it was weakest among first-degree holders (mean rank = 12.00). These mean ranks generically suggest weakening forest mystification with increasing formal education. Kruskal Wallis test shows that the means are significantly different (p < 0.05). Hence, education has an effect on forest mystification. Epsilon was 0.259 while epsilon2 was 0.067. As a result, 6.7% of the variation in forest mystification is explained by education. Relationship between Forest Mystification and Indicators of Traditional Orientation The results on Table 6 indicate that attitude to religion is positively but insignificantly related to forest mystification (r = 0.085, p = 0.244). There is a positive and significant relationship between attitude to ageing and forest mystification (r = 0.335, p = 0.000). There is a negative and insignificant relationship between attitude to gender and forest mystification (r = –0.033, p = 0.663). Cultural enthusiasm is positively and significantly related to forest mystification (r = 0.213, p = 0.003). Table 5 Influence of Sex and Education on Forest Mystification Socio- demographic variable Sub-groups Mean rank Mann-Whitney U test Kruskal Wallis test Epsilon Epsilon2 Statistic p value Chi-Square p value Sex Male 102.73 3144.50 .008 – – 0.188 0.035 Female 80.93 Education No formal Education 89.44 – – 11.729 0.019* 0.259 0.067 Primary 98.42 Secondary 98.70 Post- secondary 51.98 First degree 12.00 Note. *The B.Sc./HND sub-group is significantly difference from the no formal education (p = 0.017), the primary (p = 0.007) and the secondary (p = 0.005) sub-groups. However, the B.Sc./HND sub-group is not significantly different from the NCE/OND/HSC/A Level sub-group (p = 0.068). Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 509 Table 6 Relationship between Forest Mystification and Indicators of Traditional Orientation Forest mystification Forest mystification Pearson’s r 1 p value .000 Attitude to religion Pearson’s r .085 p value .244 Attitude to ageing Pearson’s r .335* p value .000 Attitude to gender Pearson’s r –.033 p value .663 Cultural enthusiasm Pearson’s r .213* p value .003 Note. * Significant correlations. Discussion The univariate analysis of items in the author-constructed scale of forest mystification shows that the respondents’ agreements with items were notably high, signifying the embossment of continuity as opposed to change in forest culture of mystification in the target population. This is very instructive about people’s sense of solidarity with their culture, whether they are conscious about same or otherwise. As propounded by Crow (2002), social events and manifestations are typically in pursuance of solidarity. The strong exhibition of forest mystification among respondents strongly suggests that people are rather motivated and are psychically attuned with their cultural dictates of forest mystification. Indeed, this finding upholds the popularity of the African environmental theory, “eco-humanism” (Eze, 2017, p. 626) which upholds the sacredness and humanity of nature. In this African perspective, sacred nature “means that everything on Earth—rivers, mountains, trees, plants, seas, the sun, moon, stars, et cetera—has embedded force and spirit. The environment is not just inconsequential, it is part of life and constitutive of humanity” (Eze, 2017, p. 627). Indeed, this stands in favour of respectful attitude and behaviour towards the environment and its elements. In the light of these descriptive findings, proclaiming secularity as a global identity constitutes global injustice (Wilson, 2017). Descriptive findings indicate increasing forest mystification with increasing age. This is quite expected considering that older persons have experienced and assimilated the culture for much longer. However, age was not a significant determinant of forest mystification. This suggests the strength of the continuity of forest culture: with no significant difference in younger and older people’s extent of forest mystification, the survival of this culture is staunch. This is in favour of cultural survival across generations. Quite similarly, Azong (2020) reported that belief in witchcraft was prevalent among farmers of Bamenda Highlands, Cameroon of which the age https://changing-sp.com/ 510 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko of participants expressing this belief was diverse (39 to 63 years). Respondents’ religion and ethnicity have no effect on the extent to which they mystify forests. These are indications of the voidness of contemporary religions and ethnic affiliations on the traditionality of people’s orientation towards the forest. These suggest that in matters of supernaturality, Africans tend to be united in orientation without reference to religion and ethnicity. Finding that men hold a significantly stronger extent of forest mystification than women is definitely an interesting piece. Although women’s subordinate positioning across all facets of life in developing and developed social contexts are generously acknowledged, substantial evidence showcases the power- holding status of women in African traditional contexts. For instance, in her much- referenced book titled “The Invention of Women: Making an African Sense of Western Gender Discourse”, Oyeronke Oyewumi (1997) argued passionately that gender was largely egalitarian among the Yorùbá. The stratification of gender, she argued, was colonially entrenched. Oyewumi cited the fact that Yorùbá language is not gendered to drive home her thesis. Ibrahim and Jegede (2017b) asserted that custodians of Ifá, the most central of Yorùbá people’s frame of reference, include Babaláwo (male) and Iyanifa (female). In African contexts, women served as priestesses and diviners (Amaechi & Amaechi, 2019). Nevertheless, there are tremendous bases to the claim that women experience constraints and domination, which are informed by African social structures and norms including in the Yorùbá context. Among the Yorùbá, some taboos are instituted to exclude women from participating in traditional rites. For instance, menstruating women do not get involved in sacrificing to Obàtálá, a Yorùbá god. Women are also excluded from orò, a popular Yorùbá festival (Familusi, 2012). The report of Azong (2020) shows that women are often accused of being witches and using the same for destructive purposes including causing poor rainfall and harvests in Bamenda Highlands, Cameroon. Further, there is an abundance of language and social thoughts that dehumanizes women in African culture (Oduyoye, 2001). It is probable, therefore, that negative human-social dynamics are propelling women to be more daring to cultural dictates and indifferent rather than reverential towards cultural knowledge. As it is, the current finding asserts men as opposed to women as significantly better champions of traditional forest culture. This is an interesting focus for further research. The significant effect of education on forest mystification such that increasing formal education accompanies weaker forest mystification is consonant with expectation considering that the degree of western education correlates very strongly with secular orientation irrespective of material security (Braun, 2012; Zuckerman, 2009). This buttresses the position that formally educated people are better champions of social change. People’s orientation about ageing and having respect for culture are positively and significantly related to the extent to which they mystify the forest. Hence, the more people regard older age as attracting pleasurable things and respect, the more they exhibit zeal towards their culture, the more they perceive the forest as a supernatural and paranormal spatial entity. These findings are consonant with expectation. They indicate that people are more traditional about ageing and they are zealous about their culture. Among the Yorùbá people who are dominant in the target population, age Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 511 is superfluously revered. This society is a gerontocratic society by every standard. Everyday attitude and behaviour in the Yorùbá context are typically driven by the ideology of seniority (Schwab, 1955). Older age automatically confers authority among the Yorùbá, though this might be limited in exceptional situations where an older person’s character is questionable (Fadipe, 1970). It is also in order, that cultural enthusiasm is related to forest mystification in a significantly positive manner. On the contrary, attitude to religion is not significantly related to the extent to which forest is mystified in the target population. This can be understood in the context that attitude to religion in this study was defined in terms of respondents’ devotion to conventional rather than traditional African religious practices. The insignificance of the relation between the variables under consideration can be seen as reflective of the rather conflictual relationship between them: Swidler (2013, p. 682) contended that “the axial religions [Islam, Christianity and Judaism], and especially the Pentecostal churches, devote their ritual power to a fierce war on demons, witchcraft”. The attitude of the axial religions usually makes adherence to traditional Yorùbá religions to seem “heathen” and “unholy” (Ajuwọn, 1980, p. 66). The insignificance of the relation between attitude to religion and forest mystification in this study is another indication of cultural survival in the target population. African ideologies are at the heart of Africans even as they identify themselves as Christians and Muslims. For instance, Olurode and Olusanya (1994, p. 91–92) asserted that “while the Yorùbá may profess Islam or Christianity because it is the mark of a “civilized” man to do so, at heart he is a traditionalist in the sphere of the supernatural”. In addition, attitude to gender is not significantly related to forest mystification in the target population, implying that people are no longer traditional about gender, at least when compared with their orientation concerning forests. Gender dynamics have been changing from traditional to modern/egalitarian even in developing nations. For instance, Hewett and Lloyd (2005, cited in Psaki et al., 2018) reported that 45% of girls and 66% of boys in twenty-four sub-Saharan African nations ever attended school by 1960. By 1990, these proportions increased to 73% of girls and 78% of boys. This kind of rapid change cannot be said to have been recorded in matters of supernaturality. It is even interesting that attitude to gender is the only of the four indicators of traditional orientation which yielded an inverse relationship with forest mystification, suggesting that attitude to gender and forest mystification traverse opposing directions. It appears that the character of attitude to gender is modernistic while that of forest mystification is traditionalistic in the study area. Hence, modernistic attitude to gender is not deterring of traditional attitude to the forest. Conclusions There is a strong continuity in the forest culture of mystification in the study area. This bares the popularity of supernaturality and eco-humanism thereby standing in favour of solidarity to traditional African orientation as opposed to the modern orientation. Age, religion and ethnicity are not significantly relevant in matters of forest mystification. Forest cultural survival transcends divisions along age, religion and ethnic affinities. Being male as opposed to being female, and having reduced https://changing-sp.com/ 512 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko education rather than otherwise are significantly predisposing to holding a greater extent of forest mystification. The greater the extent to which people are traditional in their outlook to ageing, the greater the extent to which they are zealous about their culture, then the greater is the degree of their forest mystification. In contrast, attitude to religion and attitude to gender are indeterminate of forest mystification. References Adeduntan, A. (2019). What the forest told me: Yoruba hunter, culture and narrative performance. NISC (Pty) Ltd. Ajala, A. S. (2009). Yoruba nationalist movements, ethnic politics and violence: A creation from historical consciousness and socio-political space in South-western Nigeria (Working paper Nr. 105). Institut für Ethnologie und Afrikastudien, Johannes Gutenberg-Universität. https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/197831604.pdf Ajuwọn, B. (1980). The preservation of Yoruba tradition through hunters’ funeral dirges. Africa: Journal of the International African Institute, 50(1), 66–72. https://doi.org/10.2307/1158643 Alwin, D. F., & McCammon, R. J. (2003). Generations, cohorts, and social change. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 23–49). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-306-48247-2_2 Amaechi, C. M., & Amaechi, E. U. (2019). Pre-colonial African gender cosmology and the gender equality nexus: “The road not taken” in Igboland, Nigeria. Asian Women, 35(3), 93–113. https://doi.org/10.14431/aw.2019.09.35.3.93 Azong, M. N. (2020). Impact of cultural beliefs on smallholders’ response to climate change: the case of Bamenda Highlands, Cameroon. International Journal of Environmental Studies, 78(4), 663–678. https://doi.org/10.1080/0020723 3.2020.1824866 Baber, K. M., & Tucker, C. J. (2006). The social roles questionnaire: A new approach to measuring attitudes toward gender. Sex Roles, 54(7–8), 459–467. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9018-y Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual review of psychology, 52(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1 Bandura, A. (2004). Social cognitive theory for personal and social change by enabling media. In A. Singhal, M. J. Cody, E. M. Rogers, & M. Sabido (Eds.), Entertainment-education and social change: History, research, and practice (pp. 75–96). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Berger, P. L. (Ed.). (1999). The desecularization of the world: Resurgent religion and world politics. Wm. B. Eerdmans. Black, H. K. (2009). Pictures of suffering in elders’ narratives. Journal of Aging Studies, 23(2), 82–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaging.2008.12.002 https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/197831604.pdf https://doi.org/10.2307/1158643 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-306-48247-2_2 https://doi.org/10.14431/aw.2019.09.35.3.93 https://doi.org/10.1080/00207233.2020.1824866 https://doi.org/10.1080/00207233.2020.1824866 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9018-y https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaging.2008.12.002 Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 513 Braun, C. M. J. (2012). Explaining global secularity: Existential security or education? Secularism and Nonreligion, 1, 68–93. http://doi.org/10.5334/snr.ae Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32(7), 513–531. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003- 066X.32.7.513 Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). Contexts of child rearing: Problems and prospects. American Psychologist, 34(10), 844–850. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.844 Büssing, A., Pilchowska, I., Baumann, K., & Surzykiewicz, J. (2014). Aspects of spirituality in German and Polish adolescents and young adults—Factorial structure of the ASP students’ questionnaire. Religions, 5(1), 109–125. https://doi.org/10.3390/ rel5010109 Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2), 267–283. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267 Crow, G. (2002). Social solidarities: Theories, identities and social change. Open University Press. Crowson, H. M. (2009). Are all conservatives alike? A study of the psychological correlates of cultural and economic conservatism. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 143(5), 449–463. https://doi.org/10.3200/ JRL.143.5.449-463 de Vries, G. (2017). Evolutionary theory. In B. S. Turner (Ed.), The Wiley- Blackwell encyclopedia of social theory. Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/ 9781118430873.est0115 Duflo, E. (2012). Women empowerment and economic development. Journal of Economic Literature, 50(4), 1051–1079. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.50.4.1051 Durkheim, E. (1976). The elementary forms of religious life (J. W. Swain, Trans.). Allen & Unwin. (Originally published in French 1912) Edgell, P. (2006). Religion and family in a changing society. Princeton University Press. Eze, M. O. (2017). Humanitatis-eco (Eco-humanism): An African environmental theory. In A. Afolayan & T. Falola (Eds.), The Palgrave handbook of African philosophy (pp. 621–632). Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-59291-0_40 Fadipe, N. A. (1970). The sociology of the Yoruba. Ibadan University Press. Fágúnwà, D. O. (1949). Igbó Olódùmarè [The almighty forest]. Thomas Nelson and Sons. Fágúnwà, D. O. (1950). Ògbójú Ọdẹ nínú Igbó Irúnmọlè [Hunter in the jungle]. Thomas Nelson and Sons. https://changing-sp.com/ http://doi.org/10.5334/snr.ae https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.32.7.513 https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.32.7.513 https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.844 https://doi.org/10.3390/rel5010109 https://doi.org/10.3390/rel5010109 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267 https://doi.org/10.3200/JRL.143.5.449-463 https://doi.org/10.3200/JRL.143.5.449-463 https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118430873.est0115 https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118430873.est0115 https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.50.4.1051 https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-59291-0_40 514 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko Fágúnwà, D. O. (1954). Ìrìnkèrindò Nínú Igbó Elégbèje [A journey in the wilderness]. Thomas Nelson and Sons. Familusi, O. O. (2012). African culture and the status of women: the Yoruba example. The Journal of Pan-African Studies, 5(1), 299–313. http://www.jpanafrican. org/docs/vol5no1/5.1AfricanCulture.pdf Fealy, G., McNamara, M., Treacy, M. P., & Lyons, I. (2012). Constructing ageing and age identities: A case study of newspaper discourses. Ageing and Society, 32(1), 85–102. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X11000092 Gedeon, P. (2018). Social change or social evolution? Arguments for and against social progress in the sociological theory of evolution. Corvinus Journal of Sociology and Social Policy, 9(1), 3–33. https://doi.org/10.14267/CJSSP.2018.1.01 Graham, P. (2019). Material engagement theory and sustainability: The materiality of social change. Journal of Strategic Innovation and Sustainability, 14(3), 70–78. https://doi.org/10.33423/jsis.v14i3.2107 Hardy, M., Skirbekk, V., & Stonawski, M. (2020). The religiously unaffiliated in Germany, 1949–2013: Contrasting patterns of social change in East and West. The Sociological Quarterly, 61(2), 254–286. https://doi.org/10.1080/00380253.2019.1593064 Hewett, P. C., & Lloyd, C. B. (2005). Progress towards education for all: Trends and current challenges for sub-Saharan Africa. In C. B. Lloyd, J. R. Behrman, N. P. Stromquist, & B. Cohen (Eds.), The changing transitions to adulthood in developing countries: Selected studies (pp. 84–117). National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/11524 Ibrahim, F. M. (2016). Òwe l’ẹsin ọrọ: Forest and tree related expositions in proverbial analysis of Yorùbá traditional knowledge. Antropologija, 16(2), 75–94. Ibrahim, F. M., & Jegede, A. S. (2017a). Tradition and limits: Polemical construction of body size among the Yoruba of southwestern Nigeria. Journal of African American Studies, 21(2), 236–255. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12111-017-9360-x Ibrahim, F. M., & Jegede, A. S. (2017b). Body size in indigenous oral knowledge among the Yorùbá of southwestern Nigeria. Africology: Journal of Pan African Studies, 10(7), 156–177. Ibrahim, F. M. (2019): Tracking women empowerment: marital communication and participation in family decision-making among married women farmers in Ibadan, Nigeria. Humanities, Management, Arts, Education & the Social Sciences Journal, 7(3), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.22624/AIMS/HUMANITIES/V7N1P1 Ibrahim, F. M. (2020). “Everyone tries to avoid responsibility”: The attenuating role of financial obligations in fertility change among Yorùbá farmers of southwestern Nigeria. Demographic Research, 43, 745–778. https://doi. org/10.4054/DemRes.2020.43.26 http://www.jpanafrican.org/docs/vol5no1/5.1AfricanCulture.pdf http://www.jpanafrican.org/docs/vol5no1/5.1AfricanCulture.pdf https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X11000092 https://doi.org/10.14267/CJSSP.2018.1.01 https://doi.org/10.33423/jsis.v14i3.2107 https://doi.org/10.1080/00380253.2019.1593064 https://doi.org/10.17226/11524 https://doi.org/10.1007/s12111-017-9360-x https://doi.org/10.22624/AIMS/HUMANITIES/V7N1P1 https://doi.org/10.4054/DemRes.2020.43.26 https://doi.org/10.4054/DemRes.2020.43.26 Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 515 Ives, C. D., & Kidwell, J. (2019). Religion and social values for sustainability. Sustainability Science, 14(5), 1355–1362. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-019-00657-0 Jegede, A. S. (2002). The Yoruba cultural construction of health and illness. Nordic Journal of African Studies, 11(3), 322–335. https://www.njas.fi/njas/article/ view/345 Jellason, N. P., Conway, J. S., & Baines, R. N. (2020). Exploring smallholders’ cultural beliefs and their implication for adaptation to climate change in North- Western Nigeria. The Social Science Journal, Advance Online Publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/03623319.2020.1774720 Kalfoss, M. H. (2017). Attitudes to ageing among older Norwegian adults living in the community. British Journal of Community Nursing, 22(5), 238–245. https://doi.org/10.12968/bjcn.2017.22.5.238 Kilanowski, J. F. (2017). Breadth of the socio-ecological model. Journal of Agromedicine, 22(4), 295–297. https://doi.org/10.1080/1059924X.2017.1358971 Kose, T., & Cinar, K. (2020). A global assessment of the relationship between religiosity and financial satisfaction. The Social Science Journal, Advance Online Publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/03623319.2020.1808769 Laidlaw, K., Power, M. J., & Schmidt, S. (2007). The attitudes to ageing questionnaire (AAQ): development and psychometric properties. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(4), 367–379. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1683 Lawal, B. (2001). Àwòrán: representing the self and its metaphysical other in Yoruba art. The Art Bulletin, 83(3), 498–526. https://doi.org/10.2307/3177240 Li, L. M. W., & Bond, M. H. (2010). Value change: Analyzing national change in citizen secularism across four time periods in the World Values Survey. The Social Science Journal, 47(2), 294–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2009.12.004 Lyons, S. T., Schweitzer, L., Urick, M. J. & Kuron, L. (2019). A dynamic social-ecological model of generational identity in the workplace. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 17(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.201 8.1500332 Malafouris, L. (2004). The cognitive basis of material engagement: where brain, body and culture conflate. In E. DeMarrais, C. Gosden, & C. Renfrew (Eds.), Rethinking materiality: The engagement of mind with the material world (pp. 53–62). McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research. https://core.ac.uk/ download/pdf/86443.pdf Mandavilli, S. R. (2018). The role of Symbiotic approaches to socio-cultural change in the Twenty-first Century: The principles and uses of the Symbiotic approach to socio-cultural change. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 5(8), 418–424. https://doi.org/10.14738/assrj.58.4986 https://changing-sp.com/ https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-019-00657-0 https://www.njas.fi/njas/article/view/345 https://www.njas.fi/njas/article/view/345 https://doi.org/10.1080/03623319.2020.1774720 https://doi.org/10.12968/bjcn.2017.22.5.238 https://doi.org/10.1080/1059924X.2017.1358971 https://doi.org/10.1080/03623319.2020.1808769 https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1683 https://doi.org/10.2307/3177240 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2009.12.004 https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2018.1500332 https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2018.1500332 https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/86443.pdf https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/86443.pdf https://doi.org/10.14738/assrj.58.4986 516 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko National Population Commission. (1998). 1991 Population census of the Federal Republic of Nigeria (Analytic report at the National level). Government of the Federal Republic of Nigeria. Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2004). Sacred and secular: Religion and politics worldwide. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511791017 Oduyoye, M. O. (2001). Keynote address. In D. O. Akintunde (Ed.), African culture and the quest for women’s rights (pp. 1–10). Sefer. Okolie, V. O., Cisana, S., Schanfield, M. S., Adekoya, K. O., Oyedeji, O. A., & Podini, D. (2018). Population data of 21 autosomal STR loci in the Hausa, Igbo and Yoruba people of Nigeria. International Journal of Legal Medicine, 132(3), 735–737. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00414-017-1722-3 Olurode, L., & Olusanya, P. O. (1994). Nigerian heritage: the Yoruba example. Rebonik. Opefeyitimi, A. (2009). “Women of the world” in Yorùbá culture. In A. Opefeyitimi (Ed.), Women in Yoruba culture: a dozen of academic articles (pp. 121–133). Penthouse Publications. Oyewumi, O. (1997). The invention of women: Making an African sense of western gender discourse. University of Minnesota Press. Psaki, S. R., McCarthy, K. J., & Mensch, B. S. (2018). Measuring gender equality in education: Lessons from trends in 43 countries. Population and Development Review, 44(1), 117–142. https://doi.org/10.1111/padr.12121 Population Division. (2019). World Population Prospects: The 2019 Revision. Department of Economic and Social Affairs, United Nations. https://population. un.org/wpp/ Roberts, P. (2016). “We have never been behaviourally modern”: The implications of material engagement theory and metaplasticity for understanding the late pleistocene record of human behaviour. Quaternary International, 405(A), 8–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.03.011 Schwab, W. B. (1955). Kinship and lineage among the Yoruba. Africa: Journal of the International African Institute, 25(4), 352–374. https://doi.org/10.2307/1156875 Sharma, P. (2010). Measuring personal cultural orientations: Scale development and validation. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 38(6), 787–806. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-009-0184-7 Smith, P. J. O. (2001). Making words sing and dance: Sense, style and sound in Yoruba prose translation. Meta, 46(4), 744–751. https://doi.org/10.7202/004197ar Soyinka, W. (1982). The forest of a thousand daemons: a hunter’s saga (a translation of Ògbójú Ọdẹ nínú Igbó Irúnmọlè by D. O. Fágúnwà). Nelson Publishers. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511791017 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00414-017-1722-3 https://doi.org/10.1111/padr.12121 https://population.un.org/wpp/ https://population.un.org/wpp/ https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.03.011 https://doi.org/10.2307/1156875 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-009-0184-7 https://doi.org/10.7202/004197ar Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 517 Soyinka, W. (2010). In the forest of Olodumare (a translation of Igbó Olódùmarè by D. O. Fágúnwà). Nelson Publishers. Strain, C. R. (2007). Moving like a starfish: Beyond a unilinear model of student transformation in service learning classes, Journal of College and Character, 8(1). https://doi.org/10.2202/1940-1639.1150 Swidler, A. (2013). African affirmations: The religion of modernity and the modernity of religion. International Sociology, 28(6), 680–696. https://doi. org/10.1177/0268580913508568 The changing global religious landscape. (2020, April 5). Pew Research Centre. https://www.pewforum.org/2017/04/05/the-changing-global-religious-landscape/ van Hiel, A., & Brebels, L. (2011). Conservatism is good for you: Cultural conservatism protects self-esteem in older adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(1), 120–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.002 van Hiel, A., & Kossowska, M. (2007). Contemporary attitudes and their ideological representation in Flanders (Belgium), Poland, and the Ukraine. International Journal of Psychology, 42, 16–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590500411443 Weber, M. (1946). From Max Weber: Essays in Sociology (H. H. Gerth & C. Wright Mills, Trans. & Eds.). Oxford University Press. https://archive.org/details/ frommaxweberessa00webe/ Wilson, E. K. (2017). ‘Power differences’ and ‘the power of difference’: The dominance of secularism as ontological injustice. Globalizations, 14(7), 1076–1093. https://doi.org/10.1080/14747731.2017.1308062 World Bank. (2012). World development report 2012: Gender equality and development. World Bank. https://openknowledge.worldbank.org/ handle/10986/4391 World Commission on Environment and Development. (1987). Our common future, From one Earth to one World: An overview by the World Commission on Environment and Development. United Nations. https://digitallibrary.un.org/ record/139811 Wuthrow, R. (2007). After the baby boomers: How twenty- and thirty- somethings are shaping the future of American religion. Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400831227 Zuckerman, P. (2009). Atheism, secularity, and well-being: How the findings of social science counter negative stereotypes and assumptions. Sociology Compass, 3(6), 949–971. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9020.2009.00247.x https://changing-sp.com/ https://doi.org/10.2202/1940-1639.1150 https://doi.org/10.1177/0268580913508568 https://doi.org/10.1177/0268580913508568 https://www.pewforum.org/2017/04/05/the-changing-global-religious-landscape/ https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.002 https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590500411443 https://archive.org/details/frommaxweberessa00webe/ https://archive.org/details/frommaxweberessa00webe/ https://doi.org/10.1080/14747731.2017.1308062 https://openknowledge.worldbank.org/handle/10986/4391 https://openknowledge.worldbank.org/handle/10986/4391 https://digitallibrary.un.org/record/139811 https://digitallibrary.un.org/record/139811 https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400831227 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9020.2009.00247.x 518 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko Appendix TRADITIONAL ORIENTATION AND FOREST MYSTIFICATION: A STUDY AMONG RURAL DWELLERS OF ONIGAMBARI FOREST RESERVE OF OYO STATE NIGERIA Dear Respondent, I represent a group of researchers at the Federal College of Forestry, Forestry Research Institute of Nigeria, Ibadan, working on the above-named project. The aim of the research is to understand differential attitudes associated with forests in communities around Onigambari forest reserve. Your anonymity is highly guaranteed. This means that your responses will not be traced back to you, your name or any other means of identification. The estimated number of participants is one hundred and ninety. This means that you and 189 other people will be required to respond to this questionnaire. This should not take more than 25 minutes. Your contribution will go a long way in helping to generate information about the study. Thank you very much for your cooperation. Dr. (Mrs.) F. M. Ibrahim 08055822100 Instructions: • Please answer the questions from your own perspective. It is about you, not someone else. • Express your own personal views and not those prevalent in the society. • Please feel free to provide true information, this is only an academic exercise. • Nobody can challenge you about your responses. • Tick your response like this √. Thank you very much for your cooperation. God bless you. 1 Age [Please write your specific age] ______________ 2 Marital Status Single _______ 1 Married______ 2 Divorced _____ 3 Widowed_____ 4 3 Highest educational qualification [Please select your highest educational qualification] No formal education 1 4 Religion Islam ________ 1 Christianity____ 2 Traditional _____ 3 Others_________ [Please state it] Primary school cert. 2 Secondary school cert. 3 NCE/OND/HSC/A Level 4 B.Sc./HND 5` Postgraduate Diploma 6 Masters degree 7 Doctorate (PhD) 8 5 Ethnicity Yoruba _______ 1 Igbo _________ 2 Hausa________ 3 others ___________ [Please state it] 6 Sex Male _____ 1 Female ___ 2 7 Employment (You may select more than one option) Unemployed 1 Farming 2 Trader 3 Artisan 4 Civil servant 5 Student 6 Others ________ [Please specify] 8 Age 16–25 _____ 1 26–35 _____ 2 36–45 _____ 3 46–55 _____ 4 56–65 _____ 5 66–above ___ 6 Changing Societies & Personalities, 2021, Vol. 5, No. 3, pp. 496–520 519 ATTITUDE TO GENDER Kindly indicate how well you agree with these statements. Provide only one response (√) for each statement CODE ITEMS RESPONSES 9 ATG1 A father’s major responsibility is to provide financially for his children Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 10 ATG2 Men are more sexual than women Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 11 ATG3 Some types of work are just not appropriate for women Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 12 ATG4 Mothers should make most decisions about how children are brought up Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 13 ATG5 Mothers should work only if necessary Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 14 ATG6 Girls should be protected and watched over more than boys Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 15 ATG7 Only some types of work are appropriate for both men and women Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 16 ATG8 For many important jobs, it is better to choose men instead of women Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 ATTITUDES TO AGEING Kindly indicate how well these statements are correct. Provide only one response (√) for each statement CODE ITEMS RESPONSES 17 ATA1 As people get older, they are better able to cope with life Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 18 ATA2 Wisdom comes with age Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 19 ATA3 There are many pleasant things about growing older Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 20 ATA4 I am more accepting of myself as I have grown older Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 ATTITUDES TO RELIGION Kindly indicate how well these statements are true. Please provide only one response (√) for each item CODE ITEMS RESPONSES 21 ATR1 I seek God’s help Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 22 ATR2 I put my trust in God Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 23 ATR3 I try to find comfort in my religion Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 24 ATR4 I pray more than usual Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 CULTURAL ENTHUSIASM How truly do these statements apply to you? Kindly avoid sharing opinions with anyone. Please, kindly respond to the questionnaire carefully CODE ITEM RESPONSES 25 COR1 I am proud of my culture Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 26 COR2 Respect for tradition is important for me Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all 0 27 COR3 I value a strong link to my past Totally true0 Fairly true1 Not true at all2 28 COR4 Traditional values are important for me Totally true0 Fairly true1 Not true at all2 https://changing-sp.com/ 520 Fausat Motunrayo Ibrahim, Benson Osikabor, ... Olawale Julius Aluko FOREST MYSTIFICATION Kindly indicate how well you agree with these statements. Provide only one response (√) for each statement CODE ITEMS RESPONSES 29 MYS1 Forest is not just the habitat of flora and fauna but also of spirits Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 30 MYS2 Special creations that are neither humans nor animals are there in forests Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 31 MYS3 There are creations in the forest that cannot be seen with ordinary eyes Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 32 MYS4 Trees like ìrókò can provide directions to hunters in the forest Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 33 MYS5 There is no deity (orisa) that does not have connection with forest Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 34 MYS6 There are some animals in forests that are actually spiritual and should not be consumed Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 35 MYS7 There are mysterious trees in forests Strongly agree4 Agree3 Disagree2 Strongly Disagree1 SPIRITUALITY Kindly indicate how well these statements are true. Kindly respond to this questionnaire carefully CODE ITEMS RESPONSES 36 ATS1 I am convinced of a rebirth of man (or his soul) Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 37 ATS2 I am convinced of existence of higher powers and beings Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 38 ATS3 I am convinced that soul has his origin in a higher dimension Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0 39 ATS4 I am convinced that man is a spiritual being Totally true2 Fairly true1 Not true at all0