Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing: A Randomized Controlled Trial


Research Articles

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive 
Writing: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Carina Schlintl 1 , Anne Schienle 1

[1] Clinical Psychology, University of Graz, BioTechMed, Graz, Austria. 

Clinical Psychology in Europe, 2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215, https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

Received: 2023-01-24 • Accepted: 2023-04-11 • Published (VoR): 2023-06-29

Handling Editor: Winfried Rief, Philipps-University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany

Corresponding Author: Anne Schienle, Clinical Psychology, University of Graz, Universitätsplatz 2/III, A-8010, 
Graz, Austria. Phone: +43 (0)316 380 – 5086. E-mail: anne.schienle@uni-graz.at

Supplementary Materials: Materials, Preregistration [see Index of Supplementary Materials]

Abstract
Background: Expressive writing (EW: a personal form of writing about emotional distress, 
without regard to writing conventions) can improve physical and mental health. The present study 
investigated whether EW can reduce pathological skin-picking. In addition, the effects of two 
modalities of writing were contrasted with each other: computer vs. paper/pencil.
Method: A total of 132 females with self-reported pathological skin-picking participated in a two-
week intervention. They either carried out six EW sessions or wrote about six abstract paintings 
(control condition), using either paper/pencil or a computer. Before and after each session, 
participants rated their affective state and the urge to pick their skin via a smartphone application. 
Questionnaires for assessing skin-picking severity were completed before and after the two-week 
intervention.
Results: The urge for skin-picking decreased directly after a writing session. The reduction was 
more pronounced in participants of the EW group, who also experienced reduced tension and 
increased feelings of relief at the end of a writing session. EW also reduced the severity of focused 
skin-picking after the two-week intervention. The writing modality had no differential effect on 
skin-picking symptoms.
Conclusions: This study identified beneficial effects of EW on pathological skin-picking. A future 
study could investigate EW as a potential tool in the context of (online) psychotherapy for skin-
picking disorder.

Keywords
skin-picking, expressive writing, app-assisted approach, tension, relief

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons 
Attribution 4.0 International License, CC BY 4.0, which permits unrestricted use, 
distribution, and reproduction, provided the original work is properly cited.

https://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.32872/cpe.11215&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2023-06-29
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2285-0650
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2173-6626
https://www.psychopen.eu/
https://cpe.psychopen.eu/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/


Highlights
• Expressive writing (EW) reduces pathological skin-picking.
• EW reduces the urge for skin-picking.
• EW increases feelings of relief.
• The beneficial effects of EW are associated with trait anxiety.

Skin-picking is a common behavior in the general population. While occasional manipu­
lation of the skin in the form of picking at scabs, bumps, or the cuticles around finger­
nails can be considered normal and generally as not having any negative consequences, 
more frequent and intense skin-picking can lead to somatic problems (skin lesions, 
infections, scars) and impaired socio-emotional functioning. In this case, excessive skin-
picking has developed into a mental disorder, labeled as skin-picking disorder (SPD; 
American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Research suggests that (benign as well as pathological) skin-picking often occurs in 
reaction to the experiencing of negative affective states (e.g., anger, anxiety). It usually 
provides short-term relief of tension and elicits positive feelings (Bohne et al., 2002). 
Indeed, many people who pick their skin report that they find it soothing, satisfying, 
and/or rewarding (Gallinat et al., 2021; Schienle & Wabnegger, 2020). Thus, skin-picking 
can be seen to serve emotion regulation, which can be functional (as in occasional 
skin-picking), or dysfunctional (as in SPD).

Several studies have shown associations between excessive skin-picking and difficul­
ties in emotion regulation (Prochwicz et al., 2018; Schienle et al., 2018; Snorrason et 
al., 2010). For example, Snorrason et al. (2010) demonstrated that difficulties in emotion 
regulation (e.g., difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior under distress), as well as 
increased emotional reactivity, predicted pathological skin-picking. A study by Schienle 
et al. (2018) also found strong associations between excessive skin-picking and emotion 
dysregulation. More specifically, the severity of focused skin-picking (i.e., skin-picking 
performed with full awareness, in contrast to automatic skin-picking) was predicted by 
difficulties in controlling impulsive behaviors, self-disgust (the tendency to feel disgusted 
by one's behavior), and disgust proneness (the tendency to experience disgust towards 
potential transmitters of disease). Further, Prochwicz et al. (2018) investigated a non-clin­
ical sample (university students) and also found an association between a strategy for 
emotion regulation and skin-picking severity. It was shown in that study that those who 
used cognitive reappraisal more often (i.e., re-evaluation of emotion-eliciting situations/ 
cognitive distancing) reported a lower skin-picking severity.

The studies mentioned above suggest that excessive skin-picking might be used as 
an alternative strategy for controlling one’s negative emotions when other effective 
strategies are not at hand. Along this line of reasoning, the emotion regulation model 
of SPD (e.g., Snorrason et al., 2010) holds that skin-picking is an emotion regulation 

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing 2

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


strategy used by people who have difficulties in applying more adaptive strategies. Based 
on these findings, it would appear important to offer alternative methods for emotion 
regulation to those who pick their skin excessively. One possible approach is expressive 
writing (EW).

EW can be described as personal and emotional writing without regard to form 
or writing conventions (e.g., spelling, punctuation, grammar). EW was first introduced 
by Pennebaker and Beall (1986) who asked students to write about their thoughts and 
feelings associated with a stressful/traumatic or neutral event. The protocol in that inves­
tigation included four writing sessions, each lasting 15 minutes. It was found that EW 
fostered favorable physical and mental health-related outcomes: a reduction of visits to 
the university health center during a 6-month follow-up period and improved well-being. 
Further, two meta-analyses support the notion that EW about upsetting experiences 
produces improvements in mood as well as in indicators of quality of life (Pavlacic et al., 
2019; Reinhold et al., 2018).

The mechanisms underlying the positive effects of EW are still under investigation. 
Pennebaker et al. (1990) have suggested that the process of EW can help one to better 
understand a distressing event that has taken place (gaining insight), and further, that 
EW can promote better problem-solving. EW has also been suggested to support disin­
hibition (catharsis), self-regulation, social integration, and acceptance of the negative 
experience (Frattaroli, 2006; Pavlacic et al., 2019). Finally, other authors have emphasized 
the role of exposure in EW (Frattaroli, 2006). Participants subject to EW interventions 
repeatedly confront themselves with thoughts and feelings regarding an upsetting event. 
Similarities can be drawn between this approach and exposure (or flooding) therapy, 
which promotes habituation, extinction, and cognitive restructuring. Based on meta-ana­
lytical findings, Frattaroli (2006) concluded that exposure theory has received the most 
empirical support for explaining EW effects.

In the case of excessive skin-picking, it is very likely that EW possesses an additional 
positive component: The mechanical requirements of writing (either by hand or by com­
puter) make skin-picking difficult to perform at the same time. Thus, EW incorporates a 
form of ‘stimulus control’ (by reducing the opportunity to perform skin-picking), which 
has been identified as a successful psychological treatment strategy for skin-picking 
disorder (Snorrason et al., 2017). Further, the process of writing – holding the pen and 
performing up and down movements – is somewhat similar to the physical movements 
involved in skin-picking. Along these lines, patients with SPD have reported that draw­
ing (e.g., pencil sketches) can be a replacement behavior for skin-picking (Atkin, 2017). 
Thus, it is assumed that the process of writing in EW, particularly in the paper/pencil 
form, may contribute to its effectiveness in reducing skin-picking.

The present study investigated whether a two-week intervention with EW (including 
six writing sessions) could reduce pathological skin-picking. Short-term effects of EW 
(e.g., changes in the urge to pick one’s skin directly after a writing session), as well 

Schlintl & Schienle 3

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


as mid-term effects (e.g., changes in self-reported skin-picking severity), were assessed. 
Further, the effects of two modalities of writing on the urge for skin-picking were 
contrasted with each other: computer vs. paper/pencil. The following hypothesis had 
been preregistered: Expressive writing (particularly paper/pencil writing) reduces skin-
picking behavior. In addition, an exploratory regression analysis was carried out to 
identify variables (e.g., number of completed writing sessions, trait anxiety) that were 
associated with the effectiveness of expressive writing (in terms of reduction in the urge 
for skin-picking).

M e t h o d

Participants
Participants with self-reported pathological skin-picking were invited to participate in a 
study on the effects of different writing interventions (this was carried out via postings 
on social media, and self-help groups for skin-picking disorder). The invitation included 
a link to an online survey that checked that participants met inclusion/exclusion crite­
ria. Inclusion criteria were female sex, because of a higher prevalence of skin-picking 
behavior in the female population (APA, 2013), and scores ≥ 7 on the Skin Picking 
Scale-Revised (SPS_R, Gallinat et al., 2016). Exclusion criteria included an existing diag­
nosis of a psychotic disorder, substance dependence, posttraumatic stress disorder, or 
depression with severe symptoms. Furthermore, participants who reported skin diseases 
were excluded. A total of 308 participants were eligible; of them, 158 could be contacted 
and agreed to participate in the study. Twenty-six participants (16%) dropped out of the 
study during the intervention. Data from 132 participants were included in the analyses 
(see Supplementary Figure 1: CONSORT flow diagram). 34% of the females participated 
in self-help groups during the course of the study.

The participants were randomly allocated to one of four groups: (a) Expressive 
Writing (paper/pencil), (b) Expressive Writing (computer), (c) Picture description (pa­
per/pencil), (d) Picture description (computer). The four groups did not differ in the 
number of participants, mean age, years of education, and reported symptom severity of 
skin-picking as assessed by the Skin Picking Scale (SPS_R; Gallinat et al., 2016) and the 
Milwaukee Inventory for the Dimensions of Adult Skin-picking (MIDAS; Walther et al., 
2009; M = 22.36, SD = 4.56). Moreover, participants did not differ in trait anxiety and trait 
depression according to the State-Trait Anxiety and Depression Inventory (STADI; Laux 
et al., 2013). For group characteristics see Table 1.

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing 4

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Table 1

Group Characteristics (Means, Standard Deviations, F/Chi-Square Statistics)

Characteristic

Expressive 
Writing (paper/

pencil)

Expressive 
Writing 

(computer)

Picture 
Description 

(paper/pencil)

Picture 
Description 
(computer) Statistics

M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Mean age (years) 28.21 (8.13) 27.71 (10.69) 30.29 (11.80) 27.50 (6.98) F(3,128) = .61, p = .608, 

ηp2 = .014

Years of education 14.09 (2.14) 13.68 (2.17) 13.68 (2.26) 14.03 (2.16) F(3,128) = .34, p = .796, 
ηp2 = .008

SPS_R 14.58 (4.15) 14.58 (3.78) 14.11 (4.13) 14.47 (4.04) F(3,128) = .11, p = .954, 
ηp2 = .003

MIDAS (focused) 22.88 (4.97) 22.55 (3.84) 21.50 (5.28) 22.70 (3.81) F(3,128) = .661, p = .578, 
ηp2 = .015

STADI_depression 20.70 (5.75) 21.45 (6.07) 21.58 (6.04) 22.00 (5.87) F(3,128) = .268, p = .849, 
ηp2 = .006

STADI_anxiety 23.97 (5.55) 23.55 (5.41) 23.74 (6.32) 25.20 (5.19) F(3,128) = .530, p = .662, 
ηp2 = .012

N N N N
Number of participants 33 31 38 30 χ2(3) = 1.15, p = .765

Dropout rate 9 3 8 6 χ2(3) = 2.23, p = .527

Note. SPS_R = skin picking scale revised; MIDAS (focused) = subscale focused picking of the Milwaukee 
Inventory for the Dimensions of Adult skin picking; STADI_depression = subscale trait depression of the State 
Trait Anxiety and Depression Inventory; STADI_anxiety = subscale trait anxiety of the State Trait Anxiety and 
Depression Inventory.

All participants provided written informed consent before participating. This study was 
preregistered on the German Register for Clinical Studies (DRKS00029224; 2022/06/07) 
and approved by the ethics committee of the University (GZ. 39/79/63 ex 2021/22).

Questionnaires
Before and after the two-week intervention participants filled out the following ques­
tionnaires via online surveys:

a. German version of the Skin Picking Scale-Revised (Gallinat et al., 2016), which 
assesses symptom severity and impairment due to skin-picking during the last week. 
The eight items (e.g., How strong was your urge to pick your skin?) are answered on 

Schlintl & Schienle 5

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


5-point scales (0 = no urge; 4 = very strong urge). An overall score (total SPS_R; 
Cronbach’s alpha = .81) was computed that reflects the severity of skin-picking. A 
score of 7 represents the clinical cut-off (Gallinat et al., 2016).

b. The Milwaukee Inventory for the Dimensions of Adult Skin-picking (MIDAS; 
Walther et al., 2009) is a self-report questionnaire with two subscales: automatic 
skin-picking (Cronbach’s α = 0.62; e.g., I don't notice that I have picked my skin until 
after it's happened.) and focused skin-picking (Cronbach’s α = 0.75; e.g. I experience 
an extreme urge to pick before I pick). The six items of each subscale are judged on 
5-point Likert scales (1 = not at all; 5 = very much). Due to the low Cronbach’s α of 
the automatic skin-picking subscale, no further analyses were performed with this 
subscale.

c. The trait version of the State-Trait Anxiety and Depression Inventory (STADI; Laux 
et al., 2013) has two subscales: Depression (α = .913) and Anxiety (α = .866), with ten 
items each (e.g., depression: “I am sad”; anxiety: “I worry that something might 
happen”) that are scored on a four-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 
(very much).

App-Assisted Interventions
All participants of the four intervention groups were asked to set aside at least 10 
minutes for each writing session in a quiet place without disturbance. In total, six writing 
sessions had to be completed within a two-week period (with a maximum of one writing 
session per day). The participants had the option to write more than six times during the 
two weeks if they felt to do so. Before and after each writing session, the participants 
rated their affective state (pleasantness, tension, relief, urge to pick the skin) via a 
smartphone app on 100-point Likert scales (0 = I do not feel good, tense, relieved, no urge 
to pick my skin; 100 = I feel good, tense, relieved, a strong urge to pick my skin). The 
rating interval (pre vs. post-writing) was set to 10 minutes (it was not possible to provide 
the app ratings earlier).
The group-specific instructions for the writing sessions were as follows:

a. Expressive writing: Expressive writing is an intervention in which people spend a few 
minutes writing about specific, personally relevant topics over several days. Let your 
thoughts and feelings wander freely while writing. Expressive writing has been 
studied since the 1980s and offers a beneficial way to engage with one's emotions 
and manage them. Write for at least 10 minutes about a topic that is currently on 
your mind. Explore your thoughts and emotions openly that you perceive while 
writing. Spelling, syntax, or grammar are irrelevant. It is desirable to get into a flow 
of writing. Choose a time of the day that suits you best and find a quiet place where 
you will not be disturbed (e.g., put your mobile phone in flight mode).

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing 6

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


b. Picture description: A picture description is a visual representation translated into 
language. It is meant to be a reproduction of what is seen in the picture. For 
example, image descriptions enable visually impaired people to find access to 
pictorial representations such as paintings or photographs. The detailed descriptions 
train analytical and structural thinking, which are important skills for problem-
solving and finding new solutions. Choose a time of the day that suits you best and 
find a quiet place where you will not be disturbed (e.g., put your mobile phone in 
flight mode). Describe for at least 10 minutes one of the abstract pictures that you 
have received from us. Write about the appearance of the image as factually and 
neutrally as possible, as if you were describing it to a visually impaired person.

Half of the participants were asked to use paper and pencil to complete the task, while 
the other half of the participants were assigned to the computer-writing groups. The 
written texts remained with the participants; the experimenters had no access to the 
texts.

Procedure
After the first online survey (checking of inclusion/exclusion criteria), eligible partici­
pants were scheduled for a personal meeting where they were randomly allocated to 
one of four interventions: (a) Expressive Writing (paper/pencil), (b) Expressive Writing 
(computer), (c) Picture description (paper/pencil), (d) Picture description (computer). All 
participants received further information about the study, including instructions for 
using the smartphone app. After participants completed the two-week writing interven­
tion, they were asked to fill out a second online survey (questionnaires). Moreover, 
participants were asked to count the words written in each session. We consider the 
number of written words as a proxy for the time spent writing. Further, we chose 
this measure to detect potential noncompliance (e.g., refusal to engage in writing). The 
procedure is depicted in Figure 1.

Statistical Analysis
Self-reports assessed via the smartphone app: Mixed-model analyses of variance (ANOVAs) 
were conducted to compare the two INTERVENTIONS (Expressive Writing (EW) vs 
Picture Description (PD)) and the two WRITING MODALITIES (paper pencil (pp) vs 
computer (c)), before vs after a writing session (factor: TIME). This was done for the 
dependent measures: urge to pick one’s skin, feelings of tension, relief, and pleasantness. 
The ratings were averaged across the number of writing sessions during the two weeks. 
Moreover, word count (number of written words) was compared between the INTER­
VENTIONS via an ANOVA.

Schlintl & Schienle 7

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Questionnaires: Mixed-model analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were computed to com­
pare the questionnaire scores (SPS-R; MIDAS; STADI_depression, STADI_anxiety) be­
tween INTERVENTIONS and TIME (before and after the two-week intervention).

Exploratory regression analyses: To identify variables (number of completed writing 
sessions, word count, trait anxiety, trait depression) that are associated with the effec­
tiveness of Expressive Writing (reduction in the urge to pick one’s skin before vs. after 
a writing session), a multiple linear regression analysis was conducted. The model was 
assessed for multicollinearity (all variance inflation factors (VIFs) < 1.5; tolerance > 0.7) 
and residual distribution (Cook’s distance < 0.3, Durbin Watson > 1.5 and < 2.5). All 
analyses were conducted with SPSS version 28.

Figure 1

Procedure

Note. SPS-R: skin-picking scale (revised); MIDAS (Milwaukee inventrory of the dimensions of adult skin-
picking); STADI: subscales trait anxiety/ depression of the state trait anxiety and depression inventory.

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing 8

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


R e s u l t s

Self-Reports Assessed Via the Smartphone App
Number of Completed Writing Sessions

On average, participants completed four writing sessions (range: 1-12). The number of 
sessions did not differ between the INTERVENTION groups, MEWpp = 3.88, SD = 2.71; 
MEWc = 4.55, SD = 2.49, MPDpp = 3.76, SD = 2.39, MPDc = 2.97, SD = 2.54; F(3,128) = 2.003, 
p = .117, ηp2 = .045.

Word Count

The ANOVA that was carried out revealed that the four INTERVENTION groups differed 
in the number of written words per writing session, F(3,128) = 14.36, p < .001, ηp2 = .252. 
Tukey post-hoc comparisons (see Supplementary Table S1) showed that the EWc group 
had the highest word count (M = 316, SD = 154), followed by the EWpp group (M = 210, 
SD = 84), the PDc group (M = 205, SD = 135), and the PDpp group (M = 142, SD = 51).

Urge to Pick One’s Skin

The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of TIME, F(1,128) = 50.64, p < .001, ηp2 = 
.283, and a significant interaction TIME x INTERVENTION, F(1,128) = 8.75, p = .004, 
ηp2 = .064. All other effects were non-significant (all p > .05; see Supplementary Table 
S2). After a session of expressive writing, participants reported a reduced urge to pick 
their skin compared to before the session, t(63) = 7.02, p < .001. After a session of picture 
description, the urge to pick was less intense compared to before the PD session, t(67) = 
3.12, p = .003; Figure 2.

The reduction in the urge to pick was more pronounced in the expressive writing 
groups (Mdiff = -15.19, SD = 17.30) than in the picture description groups (Mdiff = -6.43, 
SD = 16.97; t(130) = 2.94, p = .004).

Relief

The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of TIME, F(1,128) = 10.07, p = .002, 
ηp2 = .073, and a significant interaction TIME x INTERVENTION, F(1,128) = 9.83, p = 
.002, ηp2 = .071. Post hoc comparisons showed that participants felt more relieved after 
expressive writing than before, t(63) = 4.02; p < .001. In the picture description groups, 
the participants did not significantly differ in their ratings for relief before and after a 
writing session, t(67) = .04; p = .979; Figure 2. All other effects were non-significant (all p 
> .005; also see Supplementary Table S2).

Schlintl & Schienle 9

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Tension

The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of TIME, F(1,128) = 29.95, p < .001, ηp2 = 
.190, and a significant interaction TIME x INTERVENTION, F(1,128) = 4.52, p = .036, ηp2 = 
.034. Post hoc comparisons showed that after both expressive writing, t(63) = 5.23; p < 
.001, and picture description, t(67) = 2.52; p = .014, participants reported reduced feelings 
of tension compared to before writing. The reduction of tension was more pronounced in 
the expressive writing groups, Mdiff = -12.60, SD = 19.28, than in the picture description 
groups, Mdiff = -5.61, SD = 18.35; t(130) = 2.14, p = .035. For means and standard deviations 
see Figure 2. All other effects were non-significant (all p > .005; also see Supplementary 
Table S2).

Pleasantness

The ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect TIME x INTERVENTION, F(1,128) 
= 7.88, p = .006, ηp2 = .058. All other effects were non-significant (all p > .005, see 
Supplementary Table S2). Post hoc comparisons revealed that participants in the picture 
description groups felt more pleasant than participants in the expressive writing groups 

Figure 2

Means and Standard Deviations for the App-Data

Note. EWpp = expressive writing paper/pencil; EWc = expressive writing computer; PDpp = picture description 
paper pencil; PDc = picture description computer; EW = expressive writing; PD = picture description.

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing 10

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


before the session t(130) = 2.31; p = .023. After the session, the groups did not differ in 
valence ratings, t(130) = .12; p = .905. In the picture description groups, participants felt 
more unpleasant after the writing than before, t(67) = 2.56; p = .013. In the expressive 
writing group, participants did not significantly differ in their pleasantness ratings before 
and after the session, t(63) = 1.41; p = .165. For means and standard deviations see 
Figure 2.

Questionnaire Data
Skin Picking Scale (Revised)

The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of TIME, F(1,128) = 28.53, p < .001, ηp2 = 
.182. After the two-week intervention, participants scored lower on the SPS-R (M = 
12.89, SD = 4.72) than before (M = 14.42, SD = 4.00) independent of INTERVENTION 
and WRITING MODALITY. All other effects were non-significant (all p < .005; see 
Supplementary Table S3).

Milwaukee Inventory for the Dimensions of Adult Skin-Picking (Focused)

The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of TIME, F(1,128) = 5.56, p = .020, ηp2 = 
.042, and an interaction effect TIME x INTERVENTION, F(1,128) = 7.46, p = .007, ηp2 = 
.055. Post hoc comparisons showed that participants of the expressive writing groups 
scored lower on the focused picking scale of the MIDAS after the intervention (M = 
21.47, SD = 4.65) than before, M = 22.72, SD = 4.42; t(63) = 4.04, p < .001. In contrast, 
participants of the picture description groups did not differ in their scores before (M = 
22.03, SD = 4.69) and after the two-week intervention, M = 22.10, SD = 4.46; t(67) = .20; 
p = .842. All other effects were non-significant (all p > .05; see Supplementary Table S3).

State-Trait Anxiety Depression Inventory

The ANOVA revealed no significant effects for trait anxiety and trait depression (all p > 
.05; see Supplementary Table S3).

Regression Analysis
The regression equation for the dependent variable ‘reduction in the urge to pick one’s 
skin’ (before minus after a session of EW) with the predictors number of writing ses­
sions, word count, depression, and anxiety, was significant, R 2 = .17; F(4,63) = 2.98, p = 
.026. Trait Anxiety was a significant positive predictor. Participants with a higher level of 
trait anxiety showed a greater reduction in the urge to pick their skin due to expressive 
writing (for statistics see Table 2).

Schlintl & Schienle 11

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Table 2

Results of the Multiple Linear Regression Analysis for the Association Between “Reduction in the Urge to Pick” 
(Before Minus After a Writing Session) and “Number of Writing Sessions,” “Wordcount”, “STADI_Anxiety” and 
“STADI_Depression”

Variable B SE B β t p

95.0% CI B

r srLL UL
(Constant) -13.973 10.913 -1.280 .205 -35.810 7.864

wordcount .005 .016 .036 .303 .763 -.026 .036 .014 .039

frequency -.751 .800 -.113 -.939 .352 -2.352 .850 -.106 -.121

STADI_anxiety 1.091 .461 .344 2.368 .021 .169 2.014 .390 .295

STADI_depression .244 .431 .083 .567 .573 -.618 1.106 .263 .074

Note. SE B = standard error of B; 95% CI B = 95% confidence interval for B; r = bivariate correlation, and sr = 
partial correlation; wordcount = average number of written words per writing session; frequency = number of 
writing sessions; STADI_anxiety = subscale trait anxiety of the state trait anxiety and depression inventory; 
STADI_depression = subscale depression of the state trait anxiety and depression inventory.

D i s c u s s i o n
This study investigated the effects of expressive writing (using an app-assisted approach) 
on excessive skin-picking behavior. Each participant was asked to complete six writing 
sessions over two weeks that either focused on emotional experiences with personal rele­
vance (expressive writing), or the description of abstract paintings (control condition).

The main findings of this study were that expressive writing (EW) produced posi­
tive short-term and mid-term effects on skin-picking behavior. Directly after a writing 
session, the two EW groups (computer, paper/pencil) reported a reduced urge to pick 
their skin. Interestingly, the control groups also expressed less of an urge to manipulate 
their skin after describing a painting. This latter finding implies the positive effects of 
distraction on skin-picking behavior. This is in line with clinical recommendations which 
suggest, for example, distracting one’s hands with stress balls, fidgets, or tangle toys to 
reduce skin-picking (e.g., Snorrason, Goetz, & Lee, 2017). Similarly, cognitive-behavioral 
therapy for skin-picking disorder typically includes stimulus control techniques as well 
as habit reversal training: This involves those affected being taught to engage in harm­
less motor behaviors (like clenching one’s fists), which in turn prevent skin-picking (e.g., 
Snorrason, Goetz, & Lee 2017).

Importantly, the effects of EW on skin-picking go beyond distraction and motor 
control. In the present study, EW was associated with a more pronounced reduction in 
the urge to pick one’s skin than picture description (a reduction of -15 vs. -6 points on 
a scale ranging from 0 to 100). Moreover, only EW was associated with the reduction of 
focused skin-picking as indexed by the MIDAS. Whereas the control groups showed no 
change, the EW groups showed an average reduction of one point in their MIDAS scores. 

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing 12

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Thus, EW and picture description exhibited differential effects on skin-picking symptoms 
(with small to moderate effect sizes).

EW also demonstrated immediate effects on participants’ affective states. Directly 
after a writing session, participants in the EW groups reported a greater reduction of 
tension than those in the control groups. In addition to this, those in the EW groups 
also experienced increased feelings of relief (this positive emotion occurs as a response 
to a threat that has abated or disappeared). Previous findings have suggested that EW 
exerts its effects through habituation, and/or through the (re)structuring of anxious 
feelings (Sabo-Mordechay et al., 2019; Pennebaker & Chung, 2011; Perry & Ward-Smith, 
2018). In this sense, the findings of the present study imply that EW may have assisted 
participants in reducing their emotional distress, which in turn reduced the need for 
skin-picking (i.e., the emotional distress may have no longer been pronounced enough to 
trigger skin-picking). This interpretation is also in line with exposure theory: When pa­
tients repeatedly confront themselves with negative feelings, this repetition and exposure 
can eventually lead to extinction of those feelings and associated thoughts (see Frattaroli, 
2006).

An exploratory analysis was carried out which attempted to identify variables associ­
ated with the effectiveness of EW. This regression analysis showed that the number of 
writing sessions completed and the number of words written during a session did not 
contribute significantly to the positive effects of EW. In the present study, participants 
completed on average four writing sessions; this was below the six sessions they were 
originally instructed to carry out. Nonetheless, this amount of writing was sufficient to 
reduce skin-picking behavior. This finding is also in line with recommendations based on 
a meta-analysis by Frattaroli (2006) who investigated optimal conditions for EW effects; 
these conditions included completing a minimum of only three writing sessions. Thus, 
the average of four writing sessions carried out in the current study can be seen as 
sufficient to produce positive results.

A further finding of the current study was that there was a general trend toward 
more words being written on the computer compared to handwriting. This appears to 
reflect different writing speeds for each modality. An unexpected finding was that the 
writing modality had no differential effect on the reduction of skin-picking symptoms. 
We had assumed that the process of writing (performing up and down movements) 
would be similar to the physical movements involved in skin-picking, and could there­
fore be an efficient replacement behavior. The null findings of the current study, howev­
er, are in line with results reported in the meta-analysis by Frattaroli (2006). In that study, 
it was concluded that the mode of disclosure did not moderate EW outcomes; studies 
using handwritten disclosure did not produce larger effects than studies using typed 
disclosure.

The present investigation also showed that high levels of reported trait anxiety were 
associated with more positive effects of EW (in terms of a greater reduction in the urge 

Schlintl & Schienle 13

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


to pick one’s skin). Anxiety has been shown to be a typical elicitor of skin-picking 
episodes (e.g., Yeo & Lee, 2017). Further, patients with skin-picking disorder report 
elevated trait anxiety and show elevated rates of comorbid anxiety disorders (Schienle et 
al., 2022). Other studies have demonstrated that EW is effective at reducing anxiety and 
associated problems (e.g., test anxiety; see Park et al., 2014; Robertson et al., 2021; Shen 
et al., 2018). For example, Park et al. (2014) showed that highly math-anxious individuals 
performed significantly worse on a math test than individuals with low anxiety. Notably, 
a subsequent EW intervention significantly reduced the group difference in test scores. 
The authors of that study proposed that the EW might have enabled participants to 
more effectively identify and differentiate their emotional experience, which may have 
led to the use of better emotion regulation strategies. Further, the use of specific words 
in the EW task related to anxiety, cause, and insight, was positively related to math 
performance (also see Shen et al., 2018). Thus, confrontation with anxious feelings, as 
well as cognitive restructuring, appear to be important components involved in the 
positive effects of EW on anxiety and related problems; both components are elements of 
exposure therapy, which is a highly effective method for reducing symptoms of anxiety 
and other negative emotions (e.g., Hollon & Beck, 1994; Margraf & Schneider, 1990; 
Ruhmland & Margraf, 2001). In the current study, while trait anxiety was not found to 
be reduced on average after the EW intervention, trait anxiety was however identified as 
a moderator for the effects of EW on the urge to perform skin-picking (i.e., participants 
high in trait anxiety were found to benefit more from EW). Considering this, in future 
EW studies that focus on excessive skin-picking, text analyses could be implemented 
to further elucidate anxiety-associated mechanisms of EW in the context of this dysfunc­
tional behavior. Further, additional trait variables associated with affective processing in 
the context of pathological skin-picking (e.g., disgust propensity, difficulties in emotion 
regulation) should be investigated (Schienle et al., 2018).

It is important to mention the potential limitations of the present study. First, we only 
studied females. Therefore, the results cannot be generalized to males or other groups. 
Second, some of the participants took part in self-help groups during the study; this 
could have biased results. However, none of the participants received any other form 
of psychological treatment during the course of the study. Third, observed changes in 
skin-picking behavior were based on the self-reports of participants. In future studies, 
objective measures could be introduced (e.g., photos of affected skin before and after 
the EW intervention). Finally, participants received a brief intervention lasting only two 
weeks. The implementation of EW as an additional component in a (longer-lasting) 
psychotherapy would very likely enhance its effectiveness. Further, this type of psycho­
therapy would not have to be based on conventional face-to-face interactions but could 
be provided via online counseling. The present study underlines how technologies such 
as app-assisted interventions can be used to promote beneficial effects for reducing psy­
chological symptoms, in this case, pathological skin-picking. Such e-therapy approaches 

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing 14

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


might also enhance the effectiveness of EW interventions, since larger effects of EW 
have been obtained when participants have disclosed at home vs. in other (non-private) 
settings (Frattaroli, 2006).

Conclusion
This study revealed positive immediate effects of EW on skin-picking, including a re­
duced urge for skin-picking and increased feelings of relief. Mid-term effects of EW on 
skin-picking were also found, relating to a reduction in focused skin-picking (according 
to self-reports). The beneficial effects of EW were independent of the writing modality 
(paper/pencil vs. computer) and were also found to be associated with trait anxiety.

Funding: The authors have no funding to report.

Acknowledgments: We would like to thank Hannah Fink and Carla Danczewitz for their help in conducting this 

study.

Competing Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest concerning the research, authorship, 

and/or publication of this article.

Ethics Statement: All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the 

ethical standards of the University of Graz and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or 

comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. 

The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Graz (GZ. 39/79/63 ex 2021/22).

Data Availability: The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, 

without undue reservation.

S u p p l e m e n t a r y  M a t e r i a l s
The Supplementary Materials contain the following items (for access see Index of Supplementary 
Materials below):

1. The pre-registration protocol for the study.
2. Follow-up tests (Tukey post-hoc comparisons) for the analysis of variance (ANOVA) that 

compared the four interventions (Expressive Writing: paper/pencil; Expressive Writing: 
computer; Picture Description: paper/pencil; Picture description: computer) concerning word 
count (number of written words during a session) are provided in the Supplementary Table 
S1.

3. F-statistics (F, df, p, part η2) for the mixed-model analyses of variance (ANOVAs) to compare 
the two INTERVENTIONS (Expressive Writing (EW) vs Picture Description (PD)) and the two 
WRITING MODALITIES (paper pencil (pp) vs computer (c)), before vs after a writing session 

Schlintl & Schienle 15

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://www.psychopen.eu/


(factor: TIME) concerning the app ratings (urge to pick one’s skin, feelings of tension, relief, 
and pleasantness) are provided in Supplementary Table S2.

4. F-statistics (F, df, p, part η2) for the mixed-model analyses of variance (ANOVAs) to compare 
the questionnaire scores (SPS-R; MIDAS; STADI_depression, STADI_anxiety) between 
INTERVENTIONS and TIME (before and after the two-week intervention) are provided in 
Supplementary Table S3.

5. Supplementary Figure S1 depicts the CONSORT flow diagram.

Index of Supplementary Materials

Schlintl, C., & Schienle, A. (2022). Effect of expressive writing on emotions and thoughts in 
dermatillomania [Pre-registration protocol; DRKS-ID: DRKS00029224]. German Clinical Trials 
Register. https://drks.de/search/en/trial/DRKS00029224 

Schlintl, C., & Schienle, A. (2023). Supplementary materials to "Reduction of pathological skin-picking 
via expressive writing: A randomized controlled trial" [Additional information]. PsychOpen 
GOLD. https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.12906 

R e f e r e n c e s

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th 
ed.). https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

Atkin, L. (2017, November). Re-imagining compulsive skin picking: Art for recovery [Video]. Ted 
Conferences. 
https://www.ted.com/talks/liz_atkin_re_imagining_compulsive_skin_picking_art_for_recovery

Bohne, A., Wilhelm, S., Keuthen, N. J., Baer, L., & Jenike, M. A. (2002). Skin picking in German 
students: Prevalence, phenomenology, and associated characteristics. Behavior Modification, 
26(3), 320–339. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445502026003002

Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators: A meta-analysis. Psychological 
Bulletin, 132(6), Article 823. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.823

Gallinat, C., Keuthen, N. J., & Backenstrass, M. (2016). A self-report instrument for the assessment 
of dermatillomania: Reliability and validity of the German skin picking scale-revised. 
Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik, Medizinische Psychologie, 66(6), 249–255. 

Gallinat, C., Stürmlinger, L. L., Schaber, S., & Bauer, S. (2021). Pathological skin picking: 
Phenomenology and associations with emotions, self-esteem, body image, and subjective 
physical well-being. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, Article 732717. 
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.732717

Hollon, S., & Beck, A. T. (1994). Cognitive and cognitive-behavioral therapies. In A. E. Bergin & S. 
L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (4th ed., pp. 393–442). Wiley.

Laux, L., Hock, M., Bergner-Köther, R., Hodapp, V., & Renner, K.-H. (2013). Das State-Trait-Angst-
Depressions-Inventar (STADI). Manual. Hogrefe.

Margraf, J., & Schneider, S. (1990). Panik. Angstanfälle und ihre Behandlung (2nd ed.). Springer.

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing 16

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.12906
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
https://www.ted.com/talks/liz_atkin_re_imagining_compulsive_skin_picking_art_for_recovery
https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445502026003002
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.823
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.732717
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Park, D., Ramirez, G., & Beilock, S. L. (2014). The role of expressive writing in math anxiety. Journal 
of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20(2), 103–111. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000013

Pavlacic, J. M., Buchanan, E. M., Maxwell, N. P., Hopke, T. G., & Schulenberg, S. E. (2019). A meta-
analysis of expressive writing on posttraumatic stress, posttraumatic growth, and quality of 
life. Review of General Psychology, 23(2), 230–250. https://doi.org/10.1177/1089268019831645

Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: Toward an understanding of 
inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(3), 274–281. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.95.3.274

Pennebaker, J. W., & Chung, C. K. (2011). Expressive writing: Connections to physical and mental 
health. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), Oxford handbook of health psychology (pp. 417-437). Oxford 
University Press.

Pennebaker, J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L. K. (1990). Accelerating the coping process. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 58(3), 528–537. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.3.528

Perry, J. A., & Ward-Smith, P. (2018). Expressive writing as an intervention to decrease distress in 
pediatric critical care nurses. Clinical Nursing Studies, 6(3), 96–100. 
https://doi.org/10.5430/cns.v6n3p96

Prochwicz, K., Kłosowska, J., & Kałużna-Wielobób, A. (2018). The relationship between emotion 
regulation strategies, personality traits and skin picking behaviours in a non-clinical sample of 
Polish adults. Psychiatry Research, 264, 67–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.03.046

Reinhold, M., Bürkner, P.-C., & Holling, H. (2018). Effects of expressive writing on depressive 
symptoms—A meta‐analysis. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 25(1), Article e12224. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101749

Robertson, S. M., Short, S. D., Sawyer, L., & Sweazy, S. (2021). Randomized controlled trial assessing 
the efficacy of expressive writing in reducing anxiety in first-year college students: The role of 
linguistic features. Psychology & Health, 36(9), 1041–1065. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2020.1827146

Ruhmland, M., & Margraf, J. (2001). Effektivität psychologischer Therapien von generalisierter 
Angststörung und sozialer Phobie: Meta-Analysen auf Störungsebene. Verhaltenstherapie, 11(1), 
27–40. https://doi.org/10.1159/000050322

Sabo-Mordechay, D. S., Nir, B., & Eviatar, Z. (2019). Expressive writing – Who is it good for? 
Individual differences in the improvement of mental health resulting from expressive writing. 
Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice, 37, 115–121. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctcp.2019.101064

Schienle, A., Schlintl, C., & Wabnegger, A. (2022). A neurobiological evaluation of soft touch 
training for patients with skin-picking disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 36, Article 103254. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103254

Schienle, A., & Wabnegger, A. (2020). Two subtypes of pathological skin-picking: Evidence from a 
voxel-based morphometry study. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 25, 
Article 100534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2020.100534

Schlintl & Schienle 17

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000013
https://doi.org/10.1177/1089268019831645
https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.95.3.274
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.3.528
https://doi.org/10.5430/cns.v6n3p96
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.03.046
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101749
https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2020.1827146
https://doi.org/10.1159/000050322
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctcp.2019.101064
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103254
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2020.100534
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Schienle, A., Zorjan, S., Übel, S., & Wabnegger, A. (2018). Prediction of automatic and focused skin 
picking based on trait disgust and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and 
Related Disorders, 16, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.10.006

Shen, L., Yang, L., Zhang, J., & Zhang, M. (2018). Benefits of expressive writing in reducing test 
anxiety: A randomized controlled trial in Chinese samples. PLoS ONE, 13(2), Article e0191779. 
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191779

Snorrason, I., Goetz, A. R., & Lee, H. (2017). Psychological treatment of excoriation disorder. In J. S. 
Abramowitz, D. McKay, & E. A. Storch (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of obsessive compulsive 
disorders (Vol. 2, pp. 990-1008). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118890233.ch56

Snorrason, Í., Smari, J., & Olafsson, R. P. (2010). Emotion regulation in pathological skin picking: 
Findings from a non-treatment seeking sample. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental 
Psychiatry, 41(3), 238–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.01.009

Walther, M. R., Flessner, C. A., Conelea, C. A., & Woods, D. W. (2009). The Milwaukee Inventory for 
the Dimensions of Adult Skin Picking (MIDAS): Initial development and psychometric 
properties. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40(1), 127–135. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.07.002

Yeo, S. K., & Lee, W. K. (2017). The relationship between adolescents’ academic stress, impulsivity, 
anxiety, and skin picking behavior. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 28, 111–114. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2017.03.039

Clinical Psychology in Europe (CPE) 
is the official journal of the 
European Association of Clinical 
Psychology and Psychological 
Treatment (EACLIPT).

PsychOpen GOLD is a publishing 
service by Leibniz Institute for 
Psychology (ZPID), Germany.

Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing 18

Clinical Psychology in Europe
2023, Vol. 5(2), Article e11215
https://doi.org/10.32872/cpe.11215

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.10.006
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191779
https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118890233.ch56
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.01.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.07.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2017.03.039
https://www.psychopen.eu/

	Reduction of Pathological Skin-Picking Via Expressive Writing
	(Introduction)
	Method
	Participants
	Questionnaires
	App-Assisted Interventions
	Procedure
	Statistical Analysis

	Results
	Self-Reports Assessed Via the Smartphone App
	Questionnaire Data
	Regression Analysis

	Discussion
	Conclusion

	(Additional Information)
	Funding
	Acknowledgments
	Competing Interests
	Ethics Statement
	Data Availability

	Supplementary Materials
	References