Untitled


Note  |  Dermatol Pract Concept 2015;5(2):11 69

DERMATOLOGY PRACTICAL & CONCEPTUAL
www.derm101.com

Summary

• Metaphoric or analogical terminology is common in clini-

cal dermatology, dermoscopy and dermatopathology.

• Metaphoric language in dermatology has been criticized 

for a perceived lack of clear definition and specificity, and 

non-metaphoric (descriptive) terms and diagnostic algo-

rithms have attempted to be constructed.

• Metaphors are pervasive in human language and appear to 

be deeply rooted in our conceptual frameworks.

• The utility of metaphors in dermoscopy is discussed, with 

particular reference to research in the cognitive sciences.

Introduction: Metaphoric language 
in dermatology, dermoscopy and 
dermatopathology
Metaphor is a complex subject in language and cognitive 

science. It is a linguistic and conceptual tool commonly used 

in science and the arts and has been defined as “understand-

ing and experiencing one kind of thing in terms of another” 

[1]. Metaphors are a special form of analogy or association, 

and aim to help the learner comprehend and communicate 

new or unfamiliar (“target”) information based on known or 

familiar (“source”) knowledge.

Lakoff and Johnson propose that our abstract thoughts 

are largely metaphoric, and that metaphoric language is 

secondary to this [1]. In their view “metaphoric thought is 

unavoidable, ubiquitous, and mostly unconscious,” having 

developed automatically in childhood as we learn to func-

tion in our everyday world [1]. Consequently, they regard 

metaphor as a natural and unavoidable aspect of human 

language.

Metaphors are widely used in the dermatology lexicon, 

developed to aid recognition and description of clinical, der-

moscopic, and dermatopathologic criteria. Examples include 

the prefix “lichen” in lichen planus, “bamboo” hair, “guttate” 

psoriasis, “arborizing” telangiectasias, “saw tooth” pattern, 

and so on. Besides explicit metaphoric terminology, covert 

metaphoric concepts are also common. The “disease as an 

enemy” metaphor is one example, wherein the dermatologist 

uses various diagnostic or therapeutic “armamentaria” to 

“fight” or “combat” the disorder [2].

However, metaphors have received criticism in the derma-

tologic literature in recent years. Notably, Ackerman says [3]: 

“. . . clichés are ubiquitous in dermatology and pathology in 

general and in dermatopathology in particular, the realm of 

inflammatory skin diseases being no exception. . . . Images 

like ‘corps ronds and grains’ . . . , ‘dilapidated brick wall’ . . . , 

‘tombstone pattern’ . . . , ‘festooning’ . . . , ‘flame figures’ . . . , 

‘ground-glass cytoplasm’ . . . , and ‘saw tooth pattern’ . . . may 

be picturesque, but none of them lend themselves to definition 

meaningfully by those who mouth them . . . Moreover, not a 

single one of those whimsical mental pictures has specificity.” 

Metaphoric and descriptive terminology in 
dermoscopy: Lessons from the cognitive sciences

Jason Giacomel1, Iris Zalaudek2, Ashfaq A. Marghoob3

1 Skin Spectrum Medical Services, Como, Western Australia, Australia
2 Department of Dermatology, Medical University of Graz, Graz, Austria
3 Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center, Hauppauge, NY, USA

Citation: Giacomel J, Zalaudek I, Marghoob AA. Metaphoric and descriptive terminology in dermoscopy: Lessons from the cognitive 
sciences. Dermatol Pract Concept 2015;5(2):11. doi: 10.5826/dpc.0502a11

Copyright: ©2015 Giacomel et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, 
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Corresponding author: Jason Giacomel, MBBS, PO Box 270, South Perth, WA, 6951, Australia. Tel. +61 8 9450 2113; Fax. +61 8 9450 
2116. Email: jasongiacomel@gmail.com



70 Note  |  Dermatol Pract Concept 2015;5(2):11

several metaphoric terms used in traditional dermoscopic 

nomenclature have been defined clearly and have high speci-

ficity [8,9]; for example, “spoke-wheel” pigmentation in 

pigmented basal cell carcinoma (BCC) [8] (Table 1).

Descriptive terminology is perhaps most useful when 

it is brief and describes simple dermoscopic features (such 

as a line, circle or dot), but becomes difficult when dealing 

with complex criteria (i.e., composed of multiple basic ele-

ments); for example, “spoke-wheel” pigmentation, or the 

“strawberry” pattern of facial actinic keratoses [10] (Table 1). 

Metaphors may be able to express succinctly not only basic 

morphologic elements of a dermoscopic feature or features, 

but also information relating to an often complex combina-

tion or arrangement of these features (e.g., the “strawberry” 

pattern of erythema surrounding hyperkeratotic follicles).

Lengthy descriptive text is typically harder to visualize 

and is less memorable than a striking and suitable metaphor, 

which can usually be expressed in one or few words and is 

frequently visual in nature (e.g., “spoke-wheel” pigmenta-

tion). However, descriptive terminology might still be useful 

in defining or explaining such a metaphor.

Schematic illustration

Clinical dermatology and dermoscopy are highly visual dis-

ciplines. We can infer, therefore, that the use of illustration 

would be an effective strategy for teaching and communicat-

ing features of skin disease.

Schematic illustration is well known in psychology for 

facilitating effective learning and communication of technical 

or scientific concepts. Levin and Mayer [11] and Carney and 

Levin [12] discuss and attempt to explain this by postulating 

that pictures make text more:

i. Concentrated: pictures focus the reader’s attention on 

key points in the text;

ii. Compact or concise: a picture can highlight essential 

information that may take many sentences of text to 

explain (i.e., “a picture is worth a thousand words”);

iii. Concrete: allows for easy visualization of text content. 

Concrete pictures overcome the barrier of literal language 

to describe complex or abstract concepts;

iv. Coherent: provides a framework or clear structure for the 

text material. For example, sequential diagrams showing 

a cause-and–effect process; and

v. Comprehensible: links new, complex, unfamiliar text to 

a reader’s previous knowledge. That is, aims to help the 

student understand new, difficult ideas more effectively 

by connection to past knowledge and experience.

Learning and communication in dermoscopy appears to 

be assisted significantly by using illustration. However, effec-

tive terminology is required to describe or label these pictures. 

[3] In sum, Ackerman regards these metaphors as lacking 

clear definition and specificity.

Similarly, Kittler criticizes metaphors in traditional der-

moscopy [4]: “The images invoked by metaphoric terms and 

opaque expressions result inevitably in failure to conjure 

the very same construct in the brain of any two individuals. 

Examples: ‘Leaf-like areas,’ ‘fingerprint-like structures,’ ‘fat 

fingers,’ ‘radial streaming,’ ‘moth-eaten border,’ ‘blue gray 

veil,’ and ‘honeycomb-like pattern.’ Those images impede 

repeatable diagnosis by dermatoscopy and prevent rational 

communication between dermatoscopists.”

Moreover, Alendar et al [5] write, under the heading “No 

need for metaphoric language,” that “The current language 

of dermatoscopy consists mainly of metaphoric terms that 

are badly defined. This language is extremely confusing and 

discourages students to learn the technique profoundly.”

The above viewpoints present metaphors as being non-

scientific, lacking clear definition and specificity. Metaphor is 

thus regarded as a hindrance to understanding and commu-

nication in dermatology, dermatopathology and dermoscopy.

Descriptive terminology in 
dermoscopy
Following on from their criticism of metaphoric language 

in dermoscopy, Kittler and colleagues have set about con-

structing a new dermoscopic vocabulary based on descrip-

tive (analytic) language only, exempt from metaphors [4,6]. 

Pigmented structures are described as “lines,” “pseudo-

pods,” “circles,” “clods,” and “dots.” Vessel morphologies 

are broadly described as “dots,” “clods,” and “linear” vessels, 

with the latter subdivided into “straight,” “looped,” “curved,” 

“serpentine,” “helical, ” and “coiled” type vessels (Table 1).

However, some of these terms are actually metaphoric. 

For example, “clod” is a metaphor, meaning “a lump of earth 

or clay” [7]. Hence, the traditional dermoscopic metaphor 

(ovoid nest or lacunae/saccule) has been exchanged for a 

new metaphor (blue/gray or red clods). “Pseudopod” is also 

used, which is employed also in traditional dermoscopy and 

is metaphoric, meaning ‘false foot’ (in Greek).

Similarly, “branched serpentine” vessels describe “arbo-

rizing telangiectasias” [6] (Table 1). However, “branched” is 

fundamentally a metaphor, meaning a natural subdivision of 

a plant stem or tree trunk [7]. “Serpentine” vessel is used as 

an alternative to the traditional descriptor “linear irregular.” 

However, “serpentine” is metaphoric, meaning to resemble 

the shape of a serpent or snake [7]. Paradoxically, the tra-

ditional term in this case (“linear irregular”) is essentially 

descriptive rather than metaphoric.

Although the descriptive terminologies mentioned above 

have been well defined by Kittler and colleagues [4,6], their 

specificities have yet to be thoroughly assessed. Conversely, 



Note  |  Dermatol Pract Concept 2015;5(2):11 71

TABLE 1. Metaphoric and descriptive terminologies listed for various dermoscopic features. 
Definitions are also provided.

Dermoscopic feature 
(pictorial)

Metaphoric term
Descriptive (analytic) 

term*
Definition

‘Comma’ vessel  Curved vessel Broad, curved, slightly unfocused vessels. 
Stereotypically present in dermal nevi.

‘Hairpin’ vessel Looped vessel Vascular loops resembling a hairpin 
in morphology. May also be twisted. 
Surrounded by a whitish halo when 
occurring in keratinizing tumors, such as 
KA and invasive SCC.

‘Glomerular’ vessel Coiled vessel Tortuous vessels, frequently arranged in 
clusters and resembling the glomerular 
apparatus of the kidney. Classically seen 
in BD.

‘Arborizing’ vessel Branched (serpentine) 
vessel

Classical arborizing telangiectasias 
resemble tree branches in morphology. 
Stem vessels of large diameter branch 
irregularly into focused, fi ner capillaries. 
A hallmark of BCC.

‘Strawberry pattern’ Red structureless 
pattern interrupted by 
follicular openings and 
white circles

Erythema between hyperkeratotic hair 
follicles (white-yellow circles), resembling 
the surface of a strawberry. Typical of 
facial AK.

‘Spoke-wheel’ 
pigmentation

Central (often darker) 
clod with radial lines

Radial lines, usually tan-colored, meeting 
at an often darker central (circular to 
ovoid) axis (i.e., ‘hub’ of the spoke wheel). 
Highly specifi c for pigmented BCC.

‘(Maple) leaf-like’ 
pigmentation

Radial lines connected 
to a common base

Discrete, brown to gray-blue, bulbous 
extensions resembling a (maple) leaf. Not 
connected to a pigment network. Highly 
specifi c for pigmented BCC.

*According to Kittler H et al [4,6].
Abbreviations: AK = actinic keratosis; BCC = basal cell carcinoma; BD = Bowen disease (squamous cell carcinoma in situ); KA = 
keratoacanthoma; SCC = squamous cell carcinoma.



72 Note  |  Dermatol Pract Concept 2015;5(2):11

many aspects, involves seeing one kind of thing in terms of 

another kind of thing—what we have called metaphorical 

thought. Metaphor is thus imaginative rationality.”

We already see evidence of experientialism in dermatol-

ogy by the way art can compliment science through the close 

personal study of visual artwork (painting). Such experience 

can improve student’s recognition and description of essential 

visual features in dermatologic conditions [19,20,21]. Fur-

thermore, the paintings are basically being used as metaphors 

for patients, with Braverman referring to the paintings as 

“patient surrogates” [21]. That is, the process of developing 

visual analytical skills might be considered in a broad sense 

to be an example of analogical (metaphoric) learning: with 

the appreciation of the visual details (clues) in the narrative 

paintings and their possible interpretations being used as a 

“source” metaphor to extrapolate to a more precise descrip-

tion, diagnosis and interpretation of “target” dermatological 

features in patients. It is also an example of experiential learn-

ing which aims to heighten observational and diagnostic skills 

(and hence optimized management decisions) in the student, 

rather than to rely on less effective methods such as mere 

memorization or rote learning.

Apt metaphoric concepts and language could therefore 

have a place in dermatology and dermoscopy. However, to 

be effective in diagnosis and communication the metaphoric 

term should be well constructed and used appropriately. We 

propose that the following four parameters should be con-

sidered; that is, the metaphor should be:

(1) Well defined and clearly pictured. There should be ade-
quate text to clearly explain the (prototypical) metaphor. 

Descriptive terminology may be useful in describing the 

metaphor.

(2) Useful. The metaphor should help the student understand 
a new concept that has a degree of difficulty or complexity 

(e.g., “strawberry” appearance of facial actinic keratosis). 

If the new information is relatively simple and nontechni-

cal a metaphor may not be required—a brief descriptive, 

“Kittlerian” type term (e.g., “line,” “circle”) may be more 

effective. Furthermore, if an effective descriptor already 

exists for a given feature then introducing subsequent, less 

effective metaphors should be avoided.

(3) Fairly straightforward and commonplace (i.e., easily 
and quickly recognized, and easily remembered). The 

metaphor should be seen commonly in the everyday life 

of the target audience; and

(4) Similar in appearance to the dermatologic or dermo-
scopic feature it is describing. The metaphor (“source”) 
should resemble the feature (“target”) in form or struc-

ture and preferably also in color. Too little or too much 

(visual) detail should be avoided. Differences between the 

metaphor and the dermoscopic feature should be appreci-

ated, in order to avoid over-generalization of the meta-

This can be provided by metaphor or descriptive text (or a 

mixture of both).

Visual metaphors

Visual metaphors (graphic analogies) are a form of pictorial 

representation and psychological studies have demonstrated 

that students perform better when text is accompanied by 

visual metaphors or analogies [13,14]. Performance has been 

measured by using parameters such as recognition and recall 

of new knowledge, comprehension and application (i.e., 

problem solving). Furthermore, visual metaphors can assist 

students in solving complex or highly technical (scientific) 

problems [15], and tend to make lessons more interesting and 

enjoyable for students [14].

Synthesis: Seeking a clear, effective 
terminology in dermoscopy

Evidence from psychology contradicts the notion that all 

metaphors (analogical concepts) are ineffective. Contrarily, 

well-constructed metaphors can assist learning, comprehen-

sion, and problem solving in technical subjects.

The idea that metaphors should be avoided in science may 

reflect the general notion that science should seek objectiv-

ity, and that “metaphor and other kinds of poetic, fanciful, 

rhetorical, or figurative language can always be avoided in 

speaking objectively, and they should be avoided, since their 

meanings are not clear and precise and do not fit reality in any 

obvious way.” [1] This is indeed the prevalent idea in the West 

and may in part be traced back to ancient and enormously 

influential thinkers like Plato, who would banish poetry (and 

poets) from his utopian Republic [16].

However, not all ancient philosophers were opposed to 

metaphor. Aristotle believed that “It is a great thing, indeed, 

to make proper use of the poetic forms, . . . But the great-

est thing by far is to be a master of metaphor” [1,17]; and 

“ordinary words convey only what we know already; it is 

from metaphor that we can best get hold of something fresh” 

[1, 18]. This function of metaphor as a tool of learning or 

understanding (with new “target” information built on old 

“source” knowledge) was one of the fundamental purposes 

of metaphor mentioned in the Introduction.

Rather than rational, empirical, unemotive science being 

an antithesis to the subjective, emotive, imaginative arts, these 

two fields might alternatively be seen to compliment each 

other. In this way, imagination is intertwined with rationality 

and scientific creativity, rather than being separate to it. As 

Lakoff and Johnson [1] state: “ . . . metaphor unites reason 

and imagination. Reason, at the very least, involves catego-

rization, entailment, and inference. Imagination, in one of its 



Note  |  Dermatol Pract Concept 2015;5(2):11 73

“shiny white streaks/lines” [25, 26, 27]. These metaphors 

also appear to lack specificity, being so far described in 

malignant as well as benign conditions such as BCC, 

melanoma, dermatofibroma and Spitz nevi [24-27].

(2) “Clods”: A clod of earth may have various sizes and 

shapes, and colors. The metaphor appears therefore to 

be somewhat vague (not readily visualized) and appears 

to lack high specificity; for example, “red clods“ may 

describe both “red lacuna” (seen in hemangioma) and 

“blood spots” (a feature that may be present in tumors 

such as invasive SCC). “White clods” may refer to kera-

totic follicles (facial AK), the whitish globules in sebaceous 

hyperplasia, or the whitish globular structures of balloon 

cell nevi, to name but a few. The various types of “clods” 

could be described instead using pre-existing (but perhaps 

more precisely defined) metaphoric terms, for example 

“globule,” “ovoid nest,” and “lacunae” [4,6].

Metaphors are enmeshed in our everyday language and 

appear to be deeply rooted in our conceptual frameworks, 

influencing (often subconsciously or automatically) the way 

we think, learn, and act [1]. The difficulty of extricating 

metaphor from human language in general, and dermoscopy 

in particular, is mentioned above and highlighted in Kit-

tler’s work [4,6] which aims to create a novel dermoscopic 

method without metaphoric language, but which paradoxi-

cally contains new metaphoric terms (such as “clod” and 

“serpentine”). As Lakoff and Johnson propose: “You don’t 

have a choice as to whether to think metaphorically. Because 

metaphorical maps are part of our brains, we will think and 

speak metaphorically whether we want to or not. Since the 

mechanism of metaphor is largely unconscious, we will think 

and speak metaphorically, whether we know it or not” [1].

With the traditional language of dermoscopy and the 

newer Kittlerian terminology now co-existing side-by-side, 

the dermoscopy lexicon has ironically become more difficult 

to learn and communicate. There are now multiple terms to 

describe the same dermoscopic feature. These difficulties have 

been potentiated by a proliferation of various algorithms for 

diagnosing both pigmented and non-pigmented skin lesions. 

Ideally, we should have a consistent, clear, effective, and sim-

plified dermoscopy language (with as few algorithms as possi-

ble), which is used universally. This will require critical review 

of terms used in both traditional and Kittlerian systems and an 

expert consensus reached on the favored term to be used for 

each feature, whether it be metaphoric or simply descriptive. 

A project with these objectives is currently being organized by 

the International Skin Imaging Collaboration [28].

References
 1.  Lakoff G, Johnson M. Metaphors We Live By. 2nd edition. Chi-

cago: The University of Chicago Press, 2003.

phor. A metaphor may be particularly useful in describing 

a feature that has a high specificity (e.g., “spoke wheel” 

and “leaf-like” pigmentation in pigmented BCC), how-

ever it may still have utility if it evokes a relatively short 

list of differential diagnoses.

As Williams notes in the cognitive science field [22]: 

“Visual metaphors must be constructed carefully in order 

to be effective instructional strategies. The metaphor must 

be familiar to the target learner. It must come from everyday 

experiences and be appropriate for the developmental/educa-

tional level of the learner. The metaphor designer must create 

visuals with an appropriate degree of accuracy and detail.

Too much detail and the learner may not be able to 

interpret the metaphor; too little detail and the learner may 

not be able to find enough analogies to use the metaphor. A 

very accurate metaphor may contain so much detail that the 

learner is overwhelmed and abandons the attempt to interpret 

it at all. Finally, the judicious use of text along with the visual 

metaphor may assist in the interpretation.”

If care is taken to clearly construct, define and illustrate 

a metaphor then interobserver variability or confusion in 

interpretation of the dermoscopic feature may be potentially 

minimized. Effectively constructed metaphors would defray 

many of the general criticisms of metaphor made by Acker-

man, Kittler and Alendar in the Introduction.

Dermoscopic metaphors which appear to satisfy the 

above parameters include (see Table 1):

(1) “Arborizing” telangiectasias: the morphology of these 

vessels closely resemble tree branches, and are highly 

specific for BCC [8];

(2) “Spoke-wheel” pigmentation: the form and also color of 

the “source” metaphor can closely resemble the dermo-

scopic feature it is symbolizing, and has a high specificity 

(reported as 100%) for pigmented BCC [8];

(3) “Strawberry” pattern: a concise, compact description of 

the complex arrangement of dermoscopic features seen in 

facial actinic keratoses [10]. An accurate representation of 

structure and colors. The high sensitivity and specificity 

of this feature has recently been reported [23].

An effective metaphor allows a dermoscopic feature to be 

quickly visualized and easily remembered. Complex descrip-

tive language is avoided, which may confuse the student and 

not be readily committed to memory. However, if a metaphor 

does not fulfill the above-mentioned four parameters it may 

be ineffective and obstruct learning. Furthermore, these 

opaque metaphors may also lack high specificity. In our view, 

examples of less effective dermoscopic metaphors include:

(1) “Chrysalis” or “crystalline” structures [24, 25]: These 

are relatively complicated metaphors that do not closely 

resemble the corresponding dermoscopic feature. Instead, 

simple descriptive language may be effective, namely 



74 Note  |  Dermatol Pract Concept 2015;5(2):11

15.  Beveridge M, Parkins E. Visual representation in analogical prob-
lem solving. Memory and Cognition 1987;15:230-7.

16.  Plato. The Republic (360 BC). Translated by Benjamin Jowett. 
Project Gutenberg online eBook. Available at: http://www.guten-
berg.org/files/150/150.txt. Accessed 23rd April 2014.

17.  Aristotle. Poetics (section 1459a). Translated by S. H. Butcher. The 
Internet Classics Archive. Available at: http://classics.mit.edu//
Aristotle/poetics.html. Accessed 12th September 2014.

18.  Aristotle’s Rhetoric (section 1410b). Translated by W. Rhys 
Roberts. Online version available at: http://rhetoric.eserver.org/
aristotle/rhet3-10.html. Accessed 12th September, 2014.

19.  Dolev JC, Friedlaender LK, Braverman IM. Use of fine art to 
enhance visual diagnostic skills. JAMA 2001;286(9):1020-1.

20.  Naghshineh S, Hafler JP, Miller AR, et al. Formal art observation 
training improves medical students’ visual diagnostic skills. J Gen 
Intern Med 2008;23(7):991-7.

21.  Braverman IM. To see or not to see: how visual training can 
improve observational skills. Clin Dermatol 2011;29(3):343-6.

22.  Williams VS. Creating Effective Visual Metaphors. www.personal.
psu.edu/staff/v/q/vqw/Portfolio/VislMeta.pdf. Accessed 25th June 
2012.

23.  Huerta-Brogeras M, Olmos O, Borbujo J, et al. Validation of der-
moscopy as a real-time noninvasive diagnostic imaging technique 
for actinic keratosis. Arch Dermatol 2012;148(10):1159-64.

24.  Marghoob AA, Cowell L, Kopf AW, Scope A. Observation of 
chrysalis structures with polarized dermoscopy. Arch Dermatol 
2009;145(5):618.

25.  Liebman TN, Rabinovitz HS, Balagula Y, Jaimes-Lopez N, Mar-
ghoob AA. White shiny structures in melanoma and BCC. Arch 
Dermatol 2012;148(1):146.

26.  Giacomel J, Zalaudek I. Dermoscopy of superficial basal cell 
carcinoma. Dermatol Surg 2005;31(12):1710-3.

27.  Zalaudek I, Ferrara G, Broganelli P, et al. Dermoscopy Patterns of 
Fibroepithelioma of Pinkus. Arch Dermatol 2006;142(10):1318-
22.

28.  Website. International Society for Digital Imaging of the Skin 
(ISDIS). http://www.isdis.net/index.php/projects. Accessed 3rd 
October 2014.

 2.  Lebwohl M. Combining the new biologic agents with our current 
psoriasis armamentarium. J Am Acad Dermatol 2003;49(Sup-
pl):118-24.

 3.  Ackerman AB, Böer A, Bennin B, Gottlieb GJ. Histologic Diag-
nosis Of Inflammatory Skin Diseases: An Algorithmic Method 
Based On Pattern Analysis. 3rd edition. New York: Ardor Scriben-
di Ltd. 2005:367-8.

 4.  Kittler H. Dermatoscopy: Introduction of a new algorithmic 
method based on pattern analysis for diagnosis of pigmented skin 
lesions. Dermatopathology: Practical & Conceptual 2007;13(1):3.

 5.  Alendar F, Kittler H, Helppikangas H, Alendar T. Clear defini-
tions, simple terminology, no metaphoric terms. Expert Rev 
Dermatol 2008;3:27-29.

 6.  Kittler H, Riedl E, Rosendahl C, Cameron A. Dermatoscopy of 
unpigmented lesions of the skin: A new classification of vessel 
morphology based on pattern analysis. Dermatopathology: Practi-
cal & Conceptual 2008;14(4):3.

 7.  Merriam-Webster Dictitionary, Online Edition—available at: 
www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary. Accessed 5th February 
2014.

 8.  Menzies, SW, Westerhoff K, Rabinovitz H, et al. The surface 
microscopy of pigmented basal cell carcinoma. Arch Dermatol 
2000;136:1012-16.

 9.  Menzies SW, Crotty KA, McCarthy WH. The morphologic cri-
teria of the pseudopod in surface microscopy. Arch Dermatol 
1995;131(4):436–40.

10.  Zalaudek I, Giacomel J, Argenziano G, et al. Dermoscopy of facial 
non-pigmented actinic keratosis. Br J Dermatol 2006;155(5):951-
56.

11.  Levin JR, Mayer RE. Understanding illustrations in text. In: Brit-
ton B, Woodward A. (eds.). Learning from Textbooks: Processes 
and Principles Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1993: 95-134.

12.  Carney RN, Levin JR. Pictorial illustrations still improve students’ 
learning from text. Educational Psychology Review 2002;14(1):5-
26.

13.  Royer JM, Cable GW. Illustrations, analogies, and facilitative 
transfer in prose learning. Journal of Educational Psychology 
1976;68:205-9.

14.  Rigney JW and Lutz KA. Effect of graphic analogies on concepts 
in chemistry on learning and attitude. Journal of Educational 
Psychology 1976;68:305-11.