emergency
care

journal
contributiÊ specialisticiÊ all'urgenza

MaterialeÊ protettoÊ daÊ copyright.Ê NonÊ fotocopiareÊ oÊ distribuireÊ elettronicamenteÊ senzaÊ lÕ autorizzazioneÊ scrittaÊ dellÕ editore.

35

em
er

ge
nc

y 
ca

re
 jo

ur
na

l -
 o

rg
an

iz
za

zi
o
ne

, c
lin

ic
a,
 r

ic
er

ca
 •

 A
nn

o
 V

II 
nu

m
er

o
 3

 •
 S

et
te

m
br

e 
20

11
 •

 w
w

w
.e

cj
.it

ComplexÊ decision-making:Ê initialÊ resultsÊ ofÊ anÊ empiricalÊ study
Pier Luigi Baldi

Professor of General Psychology, Catholic University, Milan”

Dopo l’esame di alcuni lavori scientifici su emozioni e processi 
decisionali, vengono esposti i primi dati di una ricerca sperimentale, 
condotta su un gruppo di studenti universitari, avente per oggetto 
gli effetti della densità decisionale sul rischio d’errore. I risultati 

mettono chiaramente in luce che le condizioni di carico decisionale, 
a cui è stato sottoposto il gruppo sperimentale, ne hanno determinato 
un numero di errori significativamente più elevato rispetto al 
gruppo di controllo. 

SINTESI

Introduction
The incompleteness of exclusively rational explanations of various 
kinds of decision-making, and the need for an approach that takes 
account of emotion, have been evident in many psychological stud-
ies since Zajonc’s early findings were published in the late 1960s1. 
A survey of the literature raises interesting issues. According 
to Bower & Cohen2, emotions work as an attention filter: how 
stimuli are interpreted depends on the decision-maker’s mood 
at the time. Thus, an angry person might make radical, even 
destructive choices, while an anxious person would tend to 
avoid choosing altogether, or at any rate, choose what seems 
likely to be the least risky option. More recently, however, Bensi 
& Giusberti3 have speculated that anxiety gives rise to the 
diametrical opposite of this: anxious people will make a hurried 
decision – any decision – rather than endure their state of 
insecurity any longer. This is in line with what Garety, Hemsley 
and Wessely4 call the jumping to conclusions decision-making 
style, where decisions receive little or no considered thought. 
However, Bower & Cohen and Bensi & Giusberti’s conclusions 
need not be seen as mutually exclusive; they simply describe 
possible responses which vary according to the personalities of 
subjects and the situations they find themselves in.
As regards the influence of positive moods on decision-making, 
Isen5 notes that they promote flexible, productive thinking, and 
therefore appropriate, creative decision-making. 
So Rumiati & Bonini6 seem fully justified in saying that “the 
belief that rational decision-making requires the elimination of 
emotional influence is an unwarranted limitation.”
Neuroscientists have also stressed the importance of emotion 
in decision-making. One need only cite Antonio Damasio, 
among the most authoritative of neuroscientists concerned with 
decision-making, who proposes7 that somatic markers – automatic 
emotional associations responsible for identifying how acceptable 
decision choices are – play a fundamental role in decision-making. 
Adopting what seems to be a counter-intuitive approach, Damasio 
maintains that somatic markers precede rational thought, meaning 
that emotion, not reason, lies at the heart of decision-making. 
The implications of all this on activities that involve complex 
decision-making – those that take place in hospital emergency 
rooms being a prime example – will be evident.
Rastelli, Cavazza & Cervellin8 speak of the “huge numbers of 
patients and massive, unmanageable overcrowding” in hospital 
emergency rooms that have long been a focus of study in the UK 
and USA. In recent decades, say the authors, the health services in 
the main European countries have gradually reduced not only the 
number of hospital beds per 1000 inhabitants but also the number 

of hospitals, despite a gradual increase in demand for emergency 
treatment caused to a significant degree by increases in the numbers 
of immigrants and elderly people. All this, they say, is having a 
negative impact on emergency services as well as individual health 
workers8. Waiting times for treatment are getting longer and the 
system itself is increasingly unable to cope, while health workers 
often feel threatened by verbal and/or physical violence on the part 
of patients and their minders. In such circumstances, the greater 
likelihood of burn-out in health workers cannot be ignored.
The negative emotional impact of burn-out and feeling 
physically threatened, and therefore the increased likelihood of 
error in a profession prone decision-making stress (i.e., never-
ending, obligatory, unavoidably hasty decision-making where 
pressure of time may be an appreciable stress factor, at least in 
subjective terms) will immediately be apparent.
What is certain, however, is that in order to make well-founded 
observations about this state of affairs, we need solid research 
data able to predict the likelihod or otherwise of error when 
making decisions in clearly defined stress conditions.

AimÊ andÊ hypothesis
The research study, still very much of an exploratory nature, 
attempted to induce in the experimental group a degree of 
decision-making stress greater than that in the control group 
using multiple-choice tests based on logical series. Assuming 
my hypothesis to be correct, the experimental group would have 
significantly lower scores than the control group.

MethodÊ
Participants 
Forty university students aged 19-25 years. Table 1 shows the 
composition of the sample.

MaterialÊ andÊ procedure
Fifteen multiple-choice alphanumeric logical series. In each 
series the correct answer was one of the three or four choices 
(see Table 2).
Group members performed the task individually and remained 
anonymous at all times. I tried to introduce an element of 
decision-making stress by asking the participants to do the test 
as quickly as possible and giving them a new one as soon as they 
had finished the previous one. Further stress was added for half 
of the experimental group (totalling 5 males and 5 females) by 
giving them four choices; the remaining 5 males and 5 females 
had three choices.

A brief survey of key literature on emotions and decision-making 
introduces an empirical study of a group of university students 
exploring the effects of decision-making complexity on error risk. 

The results clearly show that decision-making under stress in the 
experimental group produces significantly more errors than in the 
stress-free control group.

ABSTRACT



contributiÊ specialisticiÊ all'urgenza
em

er
ge

nc
y 

ca
re

 jo
ur

na
l -

 o
rg

an
iz

za
zi

o
ne

, c
lin

ic
a,
 r

ic
er

ca
 •

 A
nn

o
 V

II 
nu

m
er

o
 3

 •
 S

et
te

m
br

e 
20

11
 •

 w
w

w
.e

cj
.it

MaterialeÊ protettoÊ daÊ copyright.Ê NonÊ fotocopiareÊ oÊ distribuireÊ elettronicamenteÊ senzaÊ lÕ autorizzazioneÊ scrittaÊ dellÕ editore.

36

The control group was given the same test without being asked 
to do it as quickly as possible. Here, too, 5 males and 5 females 
did the test with four choices, while the other 5 males and 5 
females had three choices.

Statistical analysis 
Pearson’s r correlation coefficient (product-moment correlation 
coefficient) between time taken and number of correct answers 
was calculated for all the participants.
Student’s t-test revealed any significant differences between 
scores and time taken to complete the test, in terms of sex 
difference, test conditions and number of choices (Tables 3-5).

ResultsÊ andÊ commentÊ
The r correlation coefficient between time taken and number 
of correct answers was highly significant (n = 40; r = .43; p 
<.01), showing that working in stress-free conditions enhanced 
performance. The fact that allowing more time to do the test 
resulted in higher scores is in line with expectations: successful 
completion of alphanumeric series as challenging as the ones 
used in this experiment is undoubtedly facilitated by being able 
to work without strict time limits. 
T-test values were also as predicted, with significantly higher 
scores (p < .001) in the control group, which benefited from 
working in stress-free conditions. The lack of stress was 
reflected in the fact that they took significantly longer to do the 
test than the experimental group. Moreover, the added stress 
of having four rather than three choices resulted significantly 
higher scores for tests with only three choices.

ConclusionsÊ
In my view, the most important finding of the study is the difficulty 
of performing under stress: being asked to do the test as quickly 
as possible, doing the test in the presence of someone probably 
perceived as a judge of performance, and the impossibility of 
being able to review all or part of one’s work before handing 
in the test to the examiner, may have jointly created a state of 
emotional unease. I don’t think having been able to briefly review 
answers before handing in the test would have narrowed the gap 
between the experimental group and control group scores, given 
the highly significant differences between them.
The study certainly makes no claim to be a full simulation of 

Group

Males Females
Total

Choices

ThreeÊ Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Four

Choices

ThreeÊ Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Four

Experimental 5 5 5 5 20

Control 5 5 5 5 20

Total 10 10 10 10 40

Table 1

Composition of sample (n = 40).

LogicalÊ series ThreeÊ choices FourÊ choices

VUZ20Ê RQS16Ê NMO12 BAD8 HGI8 GHI8 HGI10 BAD8 
GHI8 HGI8

Table 2

Example of alphanumeric logical series with three and four 
choices.

Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê m
Stressful conditions
Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê s

422,5

239,5 t = -4,75
***

Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê m
Stress-free conditions
Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê s

Ê 701,95

108,3

t-test on independent samples (n = 40). ***pÊ <Ê 0,001;Ê g.diÊ l.:Ê 38.

Table 3

Time taken (in seconds) to perform test in stressful and stress-
free conditions.

Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê m
Stressful conditions
Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê s

7,3

2,49 t = -4,73***

Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê m
Stress-free conditions
Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê s

11,05

2,5

t-test on independent samples (n = 40). ***pÊ <Ê 0,001;Ê g.diÊ l.:Ê 38.

Table 4

Test score in stressful and stress-free conditions. 

Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê m
ThreeÊ choices
Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê s

10,45

2,87 t = 2,8**

Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê m
FourÊ choices
Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê Ê s

Ê 7,9

2,88

t-test on independent samples (n = 40). **pÊ <Ê 0,01;Ê g.diÊ l.:Ê 38.

Table 5

Score by number of choices. 

the decision-making problems met with in real life, especially 
in a hospital’s emergency ward. However, it does clearly show 
that induced emotion and decision-making complexity have a 
far from negligible impact on error risk.  

References
1.  Zajonc RB. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J Pers Soc 

Psychol 1968, 9(2), 1-27.
2.  Bower GH, Cohen PR. Emotional influences in memory and 

thinking: Data and theory. In: Clark MS, Fiske ST (eds). 
Affect and cognition: The 17th Annual Carnegie Symposium on 
Cognition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale (NJ), 1982.

3.  Bensi L, Giusberti F, L’ansia e lo stile “salto alle conclusioni”. 
Atti del congresso AIP, Sezione di Psicologia Sperimentale. 
Cagliari, 18-20 settembre 2005.

4.  Garety PA, Hemsley DR, Wessely S. Reasoning in deluded 
schizophrenic and paranoid patients: Biases in performance on a 
probabilistic inference task. J Nerv Ment Dis 1991, 179, 194-201. 

5.  Isen AM. Positive affect and decision making. In: Lewis M, 
Haviland JM (eds). Handbook of Emotions. Guilford, New 
York, 2000.

6.  Rumiati R, Bonini N. Psicologia della decisione. Il Mulino, 
Bologna, 2001.

7.  Damasio AR. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the 
Human Brain. Avon, New York, 1994.

8.  Rastelli G, Cavazza M, Cervellin G. Sovraffollamento 
in Pronto Soccorso. Analisi del fenomeno e proposte di 
gestione. Emerg Care J 2010, 2, 25-35.