Perceived Discrimination and Psychological Well-Being Among Immigrants Living in Greece: Separation as Mediator and Interdependence as Moderator


Research Reports

Perceived Discrimination and Psychological Well-Being Among Immigrants 
Living in Greece: Separation as Mediator and Interdependence as Moderator

Evangelia Kateri 1, Donna Papastylianou 2, Evangelos Karademas 1

[1] Department of Psychology, University of Crete, Rethymno, Greece. [2] Department of Psychology, Νational and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, 
Greece. 

Europe's Journal of Psychology, 2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83, https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

Received: 2017-09-25 • Accepted: 2019-10-22 • Published (VoR): 2022-02-25

Handling Editor: Natalia Wentink Martin, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom

Corresponding Author: Evangelia Kateri, Department of Psychology, University of Crete, Gallos campus, Rethymno 74100, Greece. E-mail: 
ekateri@gmail.com

Abstract
The present study was based on the rejection-identification model regarding migrants acculturation. Personal perceived 
discrimination, acculturation attitudes, self-construal, and psychological well-being were examined simultaneously in Albanian and 
Indian immigrants residing in Greece (N = 233). It was hypothesized that perceived discrimination would be related negatively to 
immigrants’ psychological well-being, both directly and indirectly. A positive relationship was expected between perceived 
discrimination and separation and a negative relationship between discrimination and integration, or assimilation. It was, also, 
expected a positive relationship of perceived discrimination to interdependent self-construal and a negative relationship to 
independent self-construal. Furthermore, it was examined the mediating role of separation in the association of perceived 
discrimination with psychological well-being and the moderating role of interdependent self-construal in the association of perceived 
discrimination with psychological well-being. According to the results, perceived discrimination was positively related to separation 
and negatively to integration, but was related neither to independent nor to interdependent self-construal. Perceived discrimination 
was, also, positively related to depression directly and indirectly. Fewer depressive symptoms were reported by those immigrants who 
face discrimination but also select separation. Immigrants with high levels of interdependence, also, do seem to be protected from 
depression and anxiety. The interpretation of these findings signifies that, when immigrants who perceive discrimination choose 
separation from the host country, they may reduce their depression feelings, by fitting into relationships with in-group members. 
Interdependence and the perception of immigrants self as a social unit, also, may act protectively for their psychological well-being, 
enhancing the identification with the in-group, as well.

Keywords
acculturation attitudes, perceived discrimination, self-construal, psychological well-being, immigrants

Discrimination is one of the major stressors that confront ethnic minority and immigrant groups. Even though discrim­
ination is a real and daily experience for many minority groups, it is difficult to determine it objectively (Paradies, 
2006). This paper considers only perceived personal discrimination, namely the perception of receiving an unfair and 
differential treatment on the basis of ethnicity or race (Blank, Dabady, & Citro, 2004; Schmitt, Branscombe, Posmes, & 
Garcia, 2014).

Theoretically, the present study is based on the rejection-identification model (Branscombe, Schmitt, & Harvey, 1999; 
Cronin, Levin, Branscombe, van Laar, & Tropp, 2011; Postmes & Branscombe, 2002) that derives from social identity 
theory. Social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) suggests that individuals strive to maintain a positive social 

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons 
Attribution 4.0 International License, CC BY 4.0, which permits unrestricted use, 
distribution, and reproduction, provided the original work is properly cited.

https://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.5964/ejop.1531&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2022-02-25
https://ejop.psychopen.eu/
https://www.psychopen.eu/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/


identity being formed mainly from comparisons between in-groups and out-groups, in which in-group favouritism 
prevails, boosting self-esteem. Social groups who face discrimination maintain psychological well-being by becoming 
more identified with their own ethnic in-group and rejecting the out-group members and their negative evaluations 
(Branscombe et al., 1999; Cronin et al., 2011; Jasinskaja-Lahti, Liebkind, & Solheim, 2009; Postmes & Branscombe, 2002).

Perceived Discrimination and Psychological Well-Being
Several studies have demonstrated that perceived discrimination holds a negative relationship to mental health and 
physical health outcomes (Gee, Michael-Spencer, Chen, Yip, & Takeuchi, 2007; Paradies, 2006), like more depressive 
and anxiety symptoms (Schmitt et al., 2014), higher suicide and psychosis rates, and various physical health problems 
(Chakraborty & Mckenzie, 2002; Franzini & Fernandez-Esquer, 2004; Pascoe & Richman, 2009). Furthermore, this 
relationship to health is direct and/or indirect (Heim, Hunter, & Jones, 2010; Lindert, Korzilius, Van de Vijver, Kroon, & 
ArendsToth, 2008).

The Role of Acculturation Attitudes: Separation as a Mediator
There is evidence that perceived discrimination is related to acculturation process, namely to all those changes that 
arise when individuals and groups of different backgrounds come into contact (Sam, 2006). Berry (1997) proposed 
that individuals may adopt different attitudes in their effort to maintain their cultural identity and participate in 
the larger society. Thus, integration may facilitate maintenance of fundamental cultural traits, and, at the same time, 
enables the individual to participate in the wider social network. Assimilation refers to a person’s lack of interest in 
maintaining his/her own cultural identity while focusing on daily interaction with other cultures. Separation is defined 
as the immigrants’ tendency to remain separate from the host country and to focus on the culture of origin. Finally, 
marginalization occurs when there is little interest in both cultural maintenance and contact and participation.

Longitudinal evidence (Ramos, Cassidy, Reicher, & Haslam, 2016), as well as a meta-analytic review by Schmitt et 
al. (2014), suggest that perceived discrimination is a negative experience predisposing immigrants towards the pursue 
of particular acculturation attitudes. Several studies suggest that individuals experiencing high discrimination may seek 
separation from the mainstream community (Berry, 2001; Berry, Phinney, Sam, & Vedder, 2006). In contrast, when indi­
viduals perceive low discrimination, they are more likely to endorse either integration or assimilation (Jasinskaja-Lahti, 
Liebkind, Horenczyk, & Schmitz, 2003; Sam & Berry, 2010).

A number of studies indicate that integration is preferred by most immigrants as it can promote well-being and 
enhances adaptation (Berry, 1997; Berry et al., 2006; Papastylianou, 1992, 2000). However, immigrants who pursue sepa­
ration seem to report more in-group contact and connectedness with their ethnic community and better psychological 
well-being (Gungor, 2007; Jasinskaja-Lahti et al., 2003; Musso, Inguglia, & Coco, 2015; Safdar, Calvez, & Lewis, 2012; 
Safdar, Lay, & Struthers, 2003). The mediating role of separation in the relationship of perceived discrimination to 
psychological well-being was not examined so far. However, other research findings suggest that the direct negative role 
of perceived discrimination to well-being could be suppressed by the positive effects of coping via ethnic identification 
and identification with in-group (Awad, 2010; Bergman, Horenczyk, & Abramovsky-Zitte, 2017; Branscombe et al., 1999; 
Cronin et al., 2011; Schmitt & Branscombe, 2002).

The Role of Self-Construals: Interdependent Self-Construal as a Moderator
The term self-construal describes how individuals from different cultural backgrounds define and make meaning of the 
self (Kim, Triandis, Kagitcibasi, Choi, & Yoon, 1994; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 2001). Independent self-construal 
has been used to refer to individuals who perceive themselves to be separate from others, and having as an aim the 
accomplishment of personal goals (Cross, Hardin, & Gercek-Swing, 2011; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Singelis, 1994). In 
contrast, interdependent self-construal refers to individuals who perceive themselves to be defined by their relations to 
others, and having as an aim the acceptance by the in-group and the maintenance of harmony with significant others 
(Cross & Madson, 1997; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Singelis, 1994).

Immigrants with different self-construals tend to differ in their acculturation attitudes (Liem, Lim, & Liem, 2000; 
Yamada & Singelis, 1999). While integration is related to equally advanced independence and interdependence (Yamada 

Kateri, Papastylianou, & Karademas 71

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://www.psychopen.eu/


& Singelis, 1999), research findings relate separation to interdependent self-construal and assimilation to independent 
self-construal (Liem et al., 2000). Moreover, people with interdependent self-construal tend to keep a distance from out-
groups (Triandis, 1994), to maintain their culture of origin and perceive more discrimination (Hunter, 2008). However, 
because they are more committed to their heritage culture group, they avoid rejection from their own ethnic in-group 
and report better psychological well-being (Branscombe et al., 1999; Ferenczi, Marshall, & Bejanyan, 2015; Gungor, 2007; 
Phalet & Hagendoorn, 1996).

The Present Study: The Research Context and Hypotheses
The Research Context

In recent decades, an increasing number of people have immigrated to Greece from the Balkans and several Asian coun­
tries (Κuriakou, 2004). The present study included participants from two immigrant populations in Greece, Albanians 
and Indians. Regarding the Albanian immigrant community, it represents the largest immigrant community in Greece, 
while Indian community, on the other hand, counts more than 10,000–15,000 (Μarkoutsoglou, Kassou, Mosxobos, & 
Ptoxos, 2006).

Most studies in Greece suggest that Albanian immigrants in Greece prefer integration or assimilation (Georgas & 
Papastulianou, 1993; Motti-Stefanidi, Dalla, Papathanasiou, Takis, & Pavlopoulos, 2006; Papastylianou, 1992, 2000), while 
immigrants coming from more culturally distant countries, like India, report separation (Besevegis & Pavlopoulos, 2008; 
Kateri, Tsouvelas, & Karademas, 2019; Tsouvelas & Pavlopoulos, 2018). Immigrants in Greece are generally perceived 
as a threat to Greek cultural identity, social security and welfare systems (Triantafyllidou, 2000). However, high group 
discrimination in Albanian and Pontian immigrants in Greece was not necessarily resulted in high personal discrimina­
tion if protective factors were present (e.g., high self-esteem, and fewer stressors in immigrants’ lives) (Motti-Stefanidi 
& Asendorpf, 2012). Social support was, also, associated positively with separation of Indian immigrants in Greece 
minimizing the levels of immigrant’s depression who choose separation (Kateri et al., 2019).

Hypotheses

Based on the rejection-identification model (Branscombe et al., 1999; Cronin et al., 2011; Postmes & Branscombe, 
2002), the acculturation attitude of separation and interdependent self-construal were examined as factors directing im­
migrants to their in-group, minimizing the negative effects of perceived discrimination to their psychological well-being. 
Possible differences between Albanians and Indians in mediation and moderation analyses were, also, examined, given 
that these two groups are different regarding cultural traits, acculturation and psychological well-being (Besevegis & 
Pavlopoulos, 2008; Μarkoutsoglou et al., 2006).

(H1) It was hypothesized a positive relationship of perceived discrimination to separation and a 
negative relationship of perceived discrimination to integration and assimilation.

(H2) It was hypothesized a positive relationship of perceived discrimination to interdependent 
self-construal and a negative relationship of perceived discrimination to independent self-construal.

(H3) It was hypothesized a direct relationship of perceived discrimination to depression and anxi­
ety.

(H4) It was hypothesized that the indirect effects of perceived discrimination on psychological 
well-being would be through the acculturation attitude of separation. Moreover, a positive relation­
ship of perceived discrimination to separation was expected to result in turn in less anxiety and 
depression.

(H5) It was hypothesized that the strength of the relationship between perceived discrimination 
and anxiety/depression would depend on (or be moderated by) the level of interdependent self-
construal. That is, at the higher levels of interdependent self-construal immigrants would report 
relatively lower levels of anxiety and depressive symptoms.

Separation as Mediator and Interdependence as Moderator 72

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://www.psychopen.eu/


M e t h o d

Measures
Factor analyses were conducted for all measures; analyses were carried out separately for Indians and Albanians. 
Factorial agreement between the two samples was addressed next, by using Tucker’s Phi, coefficient of congruence. 
According to Van de Vijver and Leung (1997), values higher than 0.090 indicate factorial similarity.

Perceived Discrimination Scale

The Perceived Discrimination Scale was developed by Phinney, Madden, and Santos (1998). Respondents reported 
to what extent they agreed or disagreed with statements about differential and negative treatment because of their 
ethnicity using a scale ranging from “never” (1) to “very often” (5). Given that the scale has not been evaluated in a 
Greek context, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), using Lisrel 8.30 (Joresjog & Sorbom, 1996), was conducted. The 
analysis confirmed the existence of a single general factor (χ2 = 17.53, df = 14, RMSEA = 0.03, AGFI = 0.95, NNFI = 0.99, 
CFI ≥ 0.99, p < .05). Coefficient of congruence (Tucker’s phi) was 0.99. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.86.

Acculturation Attitudes Scale

This scale was developed for purposes of the international study conducted by Berry et al. (2006). It consists of 20 items 
and measures four factors, namely, assimilation, integration, separation and marginalization. The items refer to five 
domains of life, including marriage, language, cultural traditions, social activities and friends. Participants responded to 
what extent they agreed or disagreed with each statement, using a Likert-type scale ranging from “strongly disagree” 
(1) to “strongly agree” (5). In the present study, CFA conducted for this scale, given that it was the first time that the 
scale was used in Greek. The four–factor model moderately fit the data (χ2 = 228.56, df = 144, RMSEA = 0.05, AGFI = 
0.90, NNFI = 0.86, CFI = 0.90, p < .001). Coefficients of congruence (Tucker’s phi) were 0.98 for assimilation, integration 
and separation, and 0.93 for marginalization (5). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.60 for integration, 0.71 for separation, 0.61 for 
marginalization and 0.57 for assimilation.

Self-Construal Scale

The Self-Construal Scale (SCS) is a 30-item questionnaire that assesses both independent and interdependent self-con­
strual (Singelis, 1994). Each dimension is measured with 15 items. Participants responded to what extent they agreed 
or disagreed with each statement, using a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). 
This scale was used in this study as adapted in Greek by Georgas, Berry, Van de Vijver, Kagitcibasi, and Poortinga 
(2006). Coefficients of congruence (Tucker’s phi) was 0.92 for interdependent and 0.80 for independent self-construal. 
Cronbach’s alpha was 0.62 for the independent self-construal and 0.63 for the interdependent self-construal.

Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D)

Depression was measured with the CES-D scale (Radloff, 1977) as adapted in Greek by Fountoulakis et al. (2001). It 
consists of 20 items and participants responded to what extent they agreed or disagreed with each statement, using a 
Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Coefficient of congruence (Tucker’s phi) was 
0.81. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.83.

State Anxiety Inventory

Anxiety was measured with the State Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, Gorush, & Lushene, 1970) as adapted in Greek 
by Liakos and Giannitsi (1984), which evaluates the anxiety the person feels at the moment of measurement. This scale 
consists of 20 items and participants responded to what extent they agreed or disagreed with each statement, using 
a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much). Coefficient of congruence (Tucker’s phi) was 0.55. 
Cronbach’s alpha was 0.85.

Kateri, Papastylianou, & Karademas 73

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Procedure
The inclusion criterion for participants in the study was being first generation and older than 18 years old, due to 
various adaptation differences in these groups (Berry et al., 2006; Sam, 2006). Immigrants were recruited mainly in their 
work place using a convenience sampling technique. Completion of the questionnaire took place during their break 
and participation in the study was voluntary. Each measure was translated to Punjabi and Albanian from English and 
translated back to English (Brislin, 1970). The Indian questionnaire was translated into Punjabi by the Department of the 
Linguistic Studies of the Punjabi University in India and back translated into English by an Indian living in Greece for 
more than 10 years, with a MSc. in Sociology from a Greek university. The translation into Albanian was performed by 
an Albanian living in Greece for more than 20 years, with a BSc. degree of Sociology from a Greek university and back 
translated into English by a second generation Albanian immigrant who was born in Greece and was a student at School 
of Education. In order to examine the indirect effects of perceived discrimination on anxiety and depression through 
acculturation attitudes as well as the moderation role of self-construal, a set of analyses were performed in PROCESS, 
which is a freely-available computational tool for SPSS and SAS, developed by Hayes (2013).

Participants
Two hundred and thirty-three immigrants in Greece participated in the study. One hundred and fifteen were Indians 
and one hundred and eighteen were Albanian. In a preliminary analysis, Albanians and Indians were examined 
regarding their demographic characteristics. Most of the participants were male (75.4% of Indians and 56.4% of the 
Albanian sample), married (77.1% of Indians and 78.6% of Albanians) and unskilled workers (62.2% of Indians and 57.3% 
of Albanians). The average age was 33.0 years for Indians (SD = 8.0) and 35.0 years for Albanians (SD = 8.0), while the 
age range for both Albanians and Indians was 18–64. Their average years of education was 11.2 years (SD = 2.7) for 
Indians and 11.8 years (SD = 2.4) for Albanians. Regarding the years of residence in Greece, the average years were 
7.7 (SD = 3.9) for Indians and 10.7 (SD = 4.2) for Albanians. As far as their religion was concerned, the majority of 
Indians were Sikhs (81.3%), while heterogeneity was noticed in the Albanian sample: 31.2% were Orthodox’s, 31.2% were 
Muslims and 21.4% were Atheists.

Also, in preliminary analyses, Albanian and Indian immigrants were compared regarding years of residence in 
Greece, age, years of education, anxiety and depression. Albanian immigrants had lived for in Greece for longer 
compared to Indians and this difference was statistically significant, t(219) = −5.38, p < .001. The differences between 
the two ethnic groups were not significant as far as age, t(223) = −1.23, p > .05, and years of education were concerned, 
t(214) = −1.58, p > .05. t-test indicated a significant difference, regarding anxiety, t(156) = −3.63, p < .001, and depression, 
t(187) = −2.12, p < .05, regarding ethnicity as well. For this reason, the mediation analyses were, also, performed 
separately for Albanians and Indians and moderation analyses were first performed without covariates and then with 
ethnicity as one.

R e s u l t s
Table 1 presents the bivariate correlations among perceived discrimination, psychological well-being, acculturation atti­
tudes, and independent/interdependent self-construal. According to the results, perceived discrimination was positively 
related to separation (r = .46, p < .01) and negatively to integration (r = −.39, p < .01). It was not related to assimilation 
(r = −.02, p > .05) and marginalization (r = .03, p > .05) (H1). Regarding self-construal, perceived discrimination was not 
related to independent (r = −.09, p > .05) or interdependent self-construal (r = −.10, p > .05) (H2).

Regarding the direct effects of perceived discrimination on depression and anxiety (H3), the results indicated a 
statistically significant direct effect of perceived discrimination on depression, B = 0.20, SE = 0.09, p < .05, 95% CI 
[0.01, 0.40]. Regarding the indirect effects of perceived discrimination on depression and anxiety (H4), it was examined 
whether separation, integration, assimilation, and marginalization acted as mediators. The indirect effect of perceived 
discrimination on depression through separation was statistically significant, B = −0.11, SE = 0.05, p < .01, 95% CI 
[−0.24, −0.02] (see Figure 1, Table 2). However, the indirect effect of perceived discrimination on anxiety through 
separation was not statistically significant, B = −0.05, SE = 0.07, p > .05, 95% CI [−0.21, 0.08]. No indirect effects on the 

Separation as Mediator and Interdependence as Moderator 74

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://www.psychopen.eu/


relationship between perceived discrimination and psychological well-being were found for integration, assimilation, 
and marginalization (see Table 2). Next, it was examined whether the indirect role of separation to the relationship 
between perceived discrimination and depression depend on ethnicity (i.e., Albanians vs. Indians). Analyses revealed 
that the indirect role of separation was statistically significant for both Albanians, B = −0.49, SE = −0.06, 95% CI [−0.15, 
−0.00], and Indians, B = 0.50, SE = −0.08, p < .01, 95% CI [−0.22, −0.00].

Table 1

Means, Standard Deviations (SD) and Bivariate Correlations Among Psychological Well-Being, Acculturation Attitudes, Perceived Discrimination, 
Independent and Interdependent Self-Construal

Study Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Anxiety 44.50 10.74 1.00
2. Depression 17.85 10.22 .63** 1.00
3. Integration 19.78 3.72 .05 −.05 1.00
4. Separation 14.50 5.47 −.02 −.14 −.36** 1.00
5. Assimilation 9.02 3.72 −.03 .15 −.03 .13 1.00
6. Marginalization 8.49 3.60 .04 .14 −.18* .30** .42** 1.00
7.Perceived Discrimination 11.99 7.82 .10 .06 −.39** .46** −.02 .03 1.00
8. Independent Self-Construal 4.98 .66 −.10 .00 .21** −.05 .09 −.03 −.09
9. Interdependent Self-Construal 5.08 .63 −.06 −.07 .20** −.16* −.03 −.08 −.10

Note. N = 233.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

Table 2

The Indirect and Direct Effects of Perceived Discrimination on Depression and Anxiety Through Acculturation Attitudes (Mediation)

Bootstrapping 95% CIsa

Acculturation Attitudes B SE p

Indirect Effects

B SE p

Direct Effects

LL UL LL UL
Dependent Variable: Depression

Integration 0.00 0.05 .95 −0.11 0.09 0.04 0.10 .76 −0.17 0.25

Assimilation −0.00 0.01 .28 −0.03 0.02 0.01 0.10 .88 −0.18 0.21

Separation −0.11 0.05 .01 −0.24 −0.02 0.20 0.09 .03 0.01 0.40

Marginalization 0.00 0.01 .06 −0.02 0.04 0.05 0.10 .61 −0.15 0.25

Dependent Variable: Anxiety
Integration −0.06 0.05 .15 −0.20 0.01 0.20 0.12 .10 −0.04 0.46

Assimilation 0.00 0.01 .44 −0.01 0.07 0.06 0.12 .60 −0.17 0.30

Separation −0.05 0.07 .46 −0.21 0.08 0.20 0.12 .11 −0.05 0.46

Marginalization −0.00 0.01 .65 −0.01 0.04 0.12 0.11 .29 −0.10 0.34
aBootstrapping bias corrected and accelerated (5,000 bootstrap samples). Indirect and direct effects are significant at p < .05 for the 95% bootstrap 
confidence intervals, when the derived intervals do not include values of zero.

Kateri, Papastylianou, & Karademas 75

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Figure 1

Graphical Representation of the Statistically Significant Indirect (Mediation) Effects (B-Coefficients, SE in Parenthesis)

Note. Perceived D. = Perceived Discrimination.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Subsequently, it was tested whether interdependent self-construal moderate the relationship of perceived discrimination 
to depression and anxiety (H5). The variables were mean-centered first (Hayes, 2013). As shown in Table 3, the 
correlation between perceived discrimination and depression, as well as between perceived discrimination and anxiety 
was statistically significant only at the higher levels of interdependent self-construal (+1 SD). No moderation effect of 
independent self-construal was found. The statistically significant moderation effects of interdependent self-construal 
on the relationship of perceived discrimination to anxiety and depression are presented in Figure 2. When ethnicity 
was used as a covariate for the moderation analyses, the relationship between perceived discrimination and depression 
was statistically significant at the medium level of independent self-construal, B = 0.33, SE = 0.13, p < .05, 95% CI [0.06, 
0.61] and at the higher level of interdependent self-construal, B = 0.41, SE = 0.13, p < .01, 95% CI [0.15, 0.67]. Regarding 
the relationship between perceived discrimination and anxiety, it was statistically significant at the higher level of 
interdependent, B = 0.48, SE = 0.14, p < .01, 95% CI [0.21, 0.76] and independent self-construal, B = 0.39, SE = 0.15, p < 
.05, 95% CI [0.09, 0.69]. The statistical significant moderation effects using ethnicity as covariate are presented in Figure 
3. According to this figure, both independent and interdependent self-construal moderated the relationship of perceived 
discrimination to anxiety and depression.

Table 3

Bootstrapping Results for Moderation Effects of Interdependent/Independent Self-Construal on the Relationship of Perceived Discrimination to Depression 
and Anxiety

Self-Construal B SE t p

95% CIa

LL UL
Dependent Variable: Depression t

Interdependent Self-Construal
Low (−1 SD) −0.21 0.12 −1.7 .08 −0.45 0.02
M 0.04 0.09 0.51 .60 −0.13 0.23
High (+1 SD) 0.30* 0.12* 2.3 .01 0.05 0.56

Independent Self-Construal
Low (−1 SD) −0.12 0.12 −0.96 .33 −0.36 0.12
M 0.05 0.09 0.61 .53 −0.12 0.24
High (+1 SD) 0.23 0.13 1.76 .07 −0.02 0.50

Separation as Mediator and Interdependence as Moderator 76

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Self-Construal B SE t p

95% CIa

LL UL
Dependent variable: Anxiety

Interdependent Self-Construal
Low (−1 SD) −0.12 0.18 −0.66 .50 −0.48 0.23
M 0.10 0.11 0.92 .35 −0.11 0.32
High (+1 SD) 0.32* 0.12* 2.62 .04 0.08 0.57

Independent Self-Construal
Low (−1 SD) −0.01 0.16 −0.06 .95 −0.34 0.32
M 0.11 0.11 0.97 .33 −0.11 0.33
High (+1 SD) 0.23 0.14 1.64 .10 −0.04 0.51

Note. SD = standard deviation; CI = confidence intervals; SE = standard error; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit.
aBootstrapping bias corrected and accelerated (5,000 bootstrap samples). Intervals that do not contain zero are deemed to be significant.
*p < .05.

Figure 2

Graphical Representation of the Statistically Significant Moderation Effects (B-Coefficients, SE in Parenthesis)

Note. Perceived D. = Perceived Discrimination; S-C = Self-Construal.
*p < .05.

Figure 3

Graphical Representation of the Statistically Significant Moderation Effects Using Ethnicity as Covariate (B-Coefficients, SE in Parenthesis)

Note. Perceived D. = Perceived Discrimination; S-C = Self-Construal.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

D i s c u s s i o n
The results of the present study in Albanian and Indian immigrants in Greece supported rejection-identification model 
(Branscombe et al., 1999; Cronin et al., 2011; Postmes & Branscombe, 2002). Separation mediated the relationship of 
perceived discrimination to depression, reducing depressive symptoms, and, at the higher levels of interdependent 
self-construal immigrants reported relatively lower levels of anxiety and depressive symptoms.

Kateri, Papastylianou, & Karademas 77

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Initially, in accordance to the study hypothesis, separation was positively related to perceived discrimination, 
whereas integration was related negatively (Jasinskaja-Lahti et al., 2003). Interestingly assimilation was not connected to 
perceived discrimination at all. A possible explanation might be that assimilation promotes personal goals, acquisition 
of social skills, language proficiency and sustained contact with host community members (Ataca & Berry, 2002; Kosic, 
Mannetti, & Sam, 2006). It is possible that perceived discrimination connects to assimilation indirectly, through factors 
acting as mediators, like acceptance by hosts (Motti-Stefanidi, Pavlopoulos, & Asendorpf, 2018).

Secondly, the relationship of perceived discrimination to self-construal was examined. Contrary to the study hypoth­
esis, no statistical significant relationship of these variables was identified. In the context of the present study, perceived 
discrimination can be conceived as an ego-threat condition that may elicit interpersonal behaviours depending on the 
self-esteem of the individuals. In the case of independence, perceived discrimination of high self-esteem individuals may 
elicit self-serving bias, while, in the case of interdependence, perceived discrimination of low self-esteem individuals 
may elicit interpersonal appraisals (Vohs & Heatherton, 2001). Future study of the role of self-esteem in the relationship 
of perceived discrimination to self-construal is needed to test these assumptions.

Third, it was examined the relationship of perceived discrimination to anxiety and depression. In accordance 
with the study hypothesis, a direct relationship of perceived discrimination to depression was identified, confirming 
previous studies which suggested that perceived discrimination is a negative factor regarding immigrants’ psychological 
well-being (Gee et al., 2007; Paradies, 2006; Pascoe & Richman, 2009).

Regarding the indirect relationship of perceived discrimination to psychological well-being, separation was found 
to operate as a mediator. Fewer depressive symptoms were reported by those immigrants who face discrimination 
but also select separation. The experience of discrimination is conceptualized as a threat against personal identity 
and intensifies identification with the in-group (Jetten, Branscombe, Schmitt, & Spears, 2001). Given that separation 
signifies maintenance of the culture of origin (Sam & Berry, 2010), it may enhance identification with the in-group 
(Jasinskaja-Lahti et al., 2003). Furthermore, the avoidance of contact with host community members, as another defining 
feature of separation (Sam & Berry, 2010), may help immigrants feel safer from their rejection (Mossakowski, 2003), by 
asking and receiving more social support by family and friends (Kateri et al., 2019).

However, it should be noted that, contrary to the study hypothesis (H3), no direct relationship of perceived 
discrimination to anxiety was found, nor separation was found to operate as a mediator. It is important to note that 
anxiety and depression are distinct from each other (Crockett, Iturbide, Torres, & Carlo, 2007). Anxiety may be related to 
factors that were not measured in the present study, such as unemployment, difficulties in language, or lacking official 
documents or residence permits. It is possible that stress coping mechanisms (Essau & Trommsdorff, 1996) may affect 
anxiety more than perceived discrimination.

Regarding the role of interdependent self-construal in the relationship between perceived discrimination and depres­
sion/anxiety, the results support the moderation hypothesis. Immigrants with high levels of interdependence do seem 
to be protected from depression and anxiety. It is plausible that the more the devalued groups recognize the prejudice 
against them, the more they increase their emotional investment in their in-group (where they feel accepted), protecting 
their psychological well-being (Branscombe et al., 1999; Cronin et al., 2011; Ferenczi et al., 2015).

The differences in the results when ethnicity was included in the analyses seem to be quite interesting. The indirect 
role of separation to the relationship between perceived discrimination and depression remained statistically significant 
for both Albanian and Indian immigrants, indicating that separation may prove helpful in the case of perceived 
discrimination, regardless of the immigrant’s ethnicity (Gungor, 2007; Jasinskaja-Lahti et al., 2003; Musso et al., 2015). 
Regarding the moderation effects, both interdependent self-construal and independent self-construal seem to minimize 
the relationship between perceived discrimination and depression and between perceived discrimination and anxiety. It 
could thus, be assumed that both forms of construal coexist in the development of self, by satisfying the basic human 
needs of autonomy and relatedness, which are essential for the psychological well-being of any individual (Kagitcibasi, 
2005).

Separation as Mediator and Interdependence as Moderator 78

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Limitations and Conclusions
A number of limitations need to be considered in the interpretation of the present findings. First of all, internal 
consistencies were low to moderate for the scales of acculturation attitudes and self-construal. Moreover, although CFA 
seemed to confirm the existence of four factors (i.e., integration, assimilation, separation, and marginalization) in the 
Acculturation Attitudes Scale, the relevant indices are not considered to be satisfactory. Furthermore, coefficients of 
congruence were low for the scales of depression, anxiety, and independent self-construal, indicating factorial dissimi­
larity between Albanian and Indians for these scales. All these findings are raising concerns about the cross-cultural 
validity of constructs in non-Western cultures (Heine, Lehman, Peng, & Greenholtz, 2002). Moreover, a convenient 
sample was used which also represents only a small sub-set of immigrants in Greece, limiting the ability to generalize 
the results. Furthermore, given the cross-sectional nature of the study design, the direction of causality assumed in the 
mediation analyses may not be accurate (Maxwell & Cole’s, 2007).

Nevertheless, the present findings make a unique theoretical contribution to the protective role of interdependent 
self-construal in immigrants who experience discrimination. Social identity theories (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) stress the 
importance of inclusion for well-being and highlight the negative role of rejection from important social groups. How­
ever, interdependent individuals tend to perceive themselves more as a social unit (e.g., in connection with the social 
context) than as separate, autonomous and unique individuals (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Singelis, 1994) and this form 
of self-contrual may have positive consequences in the case of perceived discrimination. Thus, there is a possibility that 
more interdependent individuals would avoid contacts with out-group members (Triandis, 1994), exposing themselves to 
a lesser extent to discriminatory events and experience less stress. Interdependent individuals, also, tend to rely more 
on the approval of the in-groups for their self-validation (Gungor, 2007), increasing social support behaviours (Morry & 
Kito, 2009).

We must be cautious with the interpretation of the result findings regarding separation. Separation seems to be a 
tactic that temporarily might ensure wellbeing to some extend to migrants (Berry, 2006). However, it is possible that sep­
aration might, be an obstacle in the case of their sociocultural adaptation (Berry, 1997; Berry et al., 2006). Furthermore, 
separation in the long-term may end in out-group rejection and segregation (Bourhis, Montaruli, El‐Geledi, Harvey, & 
Barrette, 2010), making immigrants feel more depressed.

Funding: The authors have no funding to report.

Acknowledgments: The authors have no additional (i.e., non-financial) support to report.

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

R e f e r e n c e s

Ataca, B., & Berry, J. W. (2002). Psychological, sociocultural, and marital adaptation of Turkish immigrant couples in Canada. 
International Journal of Psychology, 37(1), 13-26. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590143000135

Awad, G. H. (2010). The impact of acculturation and religious identification on perceived discrimination for Arab/Middle Eastern 
Americans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16(1), 59-67. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016675

Besevegis, E., & Pavlopoulos, V. (2008). Consequences of immigration: Immigrant adaptation [in Greek]. In E. Besevegis (Ed.), 
Immigrants in Greece: Acculturation and psychosocial adaptation (pp. 94–106). Athens, Greece: Immigration Policy Institute.

Bergman, Y. S., Horenczyk, G., & Abramovsky-Zitter, R. (2017). Perceived discrimination and well-being among the ultra-Orthodox in 
Israel: The mediating role of group identity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 48(9), 1320-1327. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117722859

Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation and adaptation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46(1), 5-34. 
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.1997.tb01087.x

Berry, J. W. (2001). A psychology of immigration. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 615-631. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00231

Kateri, Papastylianou, & Karademas 79

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590143000135
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016675
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117722859
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.1997.tb01087.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00231
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Berry, J. W. (2006). Stress perspectives on acculturation. In D. Sam & J. W. Berry (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of acculturation 
psychology (pp. 43–58). New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.

Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Sam, D. L., & Vedder, P. (2006). Immigrant youth: Acculturation, identity, and adaptation. Applied 
Psychology: An International Review, 55, 303-332. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00256.x

Blank, R. M., Dabady, M., & Citro, C. F. (2004). Measuring racial discrimination. Washington, DC, USA: The National Academies Press.
Bourhis, R. Y., Montaruli, E., El‐Geledi, S., Harvey, S. P., & Barrette, G. (2010). Acculturation in multiple host community settings. 

Journal of Social Issues, 66(4), 780-802. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2010.01675.x
Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T., & Harvey, R. D. (1999). Perceiving pervasive discrimination among African Americans: 

Implications for group identification and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 135-149. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022

Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185-216. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/135910457000100301

Chakraborty, A., & McKenzie, K. (2002). Does racial discrimination cause mental illness? The British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 
475-480. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.180.6.475

Crockett, L. J., Iturbide, M. I., Torres, M., & Carlo, G. (2007). Acculturative stress, social support, and coping: Relations to psychological 
adjustment among Mexican American college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13(4), 347-355. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.13.4.347

Cronin, T. J., Levin, S., Branscombe, N. R., van Laar, C., & Tropp, L. R. (2011). Ethnic identification in response to perceived 
discrimination protects well-being and promotes activism: A longitudinal study of Latino college students. Group Processes & 
Intergroup Relations, 3, 393-407. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430211427171

Cross, S. E., Hardin, E. E., & Gercek-Swing, B. (2011). The what, who, why, and where of self-construal. Personality and Social 
Psychology Review, 15, 142-179. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310373752

Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122(1), 5-37. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.5

Essau, C. A., & Trommsdorff, G. (1996). Coping with university related problems. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 315-328. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022196273004

Ferenczi, N., Marshall, T. C., & Bejanyan, K. (2015). The protective and detrimental effects of self-construal on perceived rejection 
from heritage culture members. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 100. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00100

Fountoulakis, K., Iacovides, A., Kleanthous, S., Samolis, S., Kaprinis, S., Sitzoglou, K., & Bech, P. (2001). Reliability, validity and 
psychometric properties of the Greek translation of the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression (CES-D) Scale. BMC 
Psychiatry, 1, Article 6. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-1-6

Franzini, L., & Fernandez-Esquer, M. E. (2004). Socioeconomic, cultural and personal influences on health outcomes in low income 
Mexican-origin individuals in Texas. Social Science and Medicine, 59, 1629-1646. https://doi.org/10.1016/jsocscimed.2004.02.014

Gee, G. C., Michael-Spencer, K., Chen, J., Yip, T., & Takeuchi, D. T. (2007). The association between self-reported racial discrimination 
and 12-month DSM-IV mental disorders among Asian Americans nationwide. Social Science and Medicine, 64, 1984-1996. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.013

Georgas, J., Berry, J. W., Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Kagitcibasi, C., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2006). Families across cultures: A 30-nation 
psychological study. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.

Georgas, J., & Papastylianou, A. (1993). Acculturation of Pontic-Greek and Northern-Eperus Greek in Greece. Athens, Greece: General 
Secretary of Migrant Greeks.

Georgas, J., & Papastylianou, D. (1996). Αcculturation and ethnic identity: The remigration of ethnic Greeks to Greece. Ιn H. Grad, A. 
Blanco, & J. Georgas (Eds.), Key issues in cross-cultural psychology: Selected Papers from the XΙΙ International Congress of the 
International Αssociation for Cross-Cultural Psychology (pp. 114–127). Lisse, Netherlands: SWETS & ZEITLINGER.

Gungor, D. (2007). The interplay between values, acculturation and adaptation: A study on Turkish-Belgian adolescents. International 
Journal of Psychology, 42, 380-392. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590600878657

Hayes, E. (2013). An introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY, 
USA: Guilford Press.

Separation as Mediator and Interdependence as Moderator 80

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00256.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2010.01675.x
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022
https://doi.org/10.1177/135910457000100301
https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.180.6.475
https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.13.4.347
https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430211427171
https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310373752
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.5
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022196273004
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00100
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-1-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/jsocscimed.2004.02.014
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.013
https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590600878657
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Heim, D., Hunter, S. C., & Jones, R. (2010). Perceived discrimination, identification, social capital, and well-being: Relationships with 
physical health and psychological distress in a U.K. minority ethnic community sample. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 
1145-1164. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110383310

Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K., & Greenholtz, J. (2002). What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert 
scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 903-918. 
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.6.903

Hunter, C. D. (2008). Individualistic and collectivistic worldviews: Implications for understanding perceptions of racial discrimination 
in African Americans and British Caribbean Americans. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 55(3), 321-332. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.321

Jasinskaja-Lahti, I., Liebkind, K., Horenczyk, G., & Schmitz, P. (2003). The interactive nature of acculturation: Perceived 
discrimination, acculturation attitudes and stress among young ethnic repatriates in Finland, Israel and Germany. International 
Journal of Intercultural Relations, 27, 79-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0147-1767(02)000061-5

Jasinskaja-Lahti, I., Liebkind, K., & Solheim, E. (2009). To identify or not to identify? National disidentification as an alternative 
reaction to perceived ethnic discrimination. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 58(1), 105-128. 
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00384.x

Jetten, J., Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T., & Spears, R. (2001). Rebels with a cause: Group identification as a response to perceived 
discrimination from the mainstream. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1204-1213. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201279012

Joresjog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8 reference guide. Lincolnwood, IL, USA: Scientific Software International.
Kagitcibasi, C. (2005). Autonomy and relatedness in cultural context: Implications for self and family. Journal of Cross-Cultural 

Psychology, 36(4), 403-422. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022105275959
Kateri, E., Tsouvelas, G., & Karademas, E. (2019). The interplay between acculturation, social support, and psychological well-being of 

Albanian and Indian immigrants’ in Greece. Journal of Psychiatriki, 30(4), 311-319. https://doi.org/10.22365/jpsych.2019.304.311
Kim, U., Triandis, H. C., Kagitcibasi, C., Choi, S.-C., & Yoon, G. (1994). Introduction. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S.-C. 

Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp. 1–19). Newbury Park, CA, USA: SAGE.
Kosic, A., Mannetti, L., & Sam, D. L. (2006). Self-monitoring: A moderating role between acculturation strategies and adaptation of 

immigrants. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30(2), 141-157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.09.003
Κuriakou, Ι. (2004). Statistical data for immigrants in Greece: Analytical study for the available data and recommendations for the 

compliance with the standards of the European Union (Study in account of I.M.E.P.O, University of Panteion). Retrieved from 
http://www.mmo.gr/

Liakos, A., & Giannitsi, S. (1984). The reliability and validity of Greek state anxiety Spielberger scale. Brain, 21, 71-76. 
Liem, R., Lim, B. A., & Liem, J. H. (2000). Acculturation and emotion among Asian Americans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority 

Psychology, 6, 13-31. https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.6.1.13
Lindert, A. T., Korzilius, H., Van de Vijner, F. J. R., Kroon, S., & Arends-Toth, J. (2008). Perceived discrimination and acculturation 

among Iranian refugees in the Netherlands. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32, 578-588. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2008.09.003

Μarkoutsoglou, Μ., Κassou, Μ., Mosxobos, A., & Ptoxos, C. (2006). Asian immigrants in Greece. Retrieved from 
www.Idec.gr/iier/new/asian_migrants_en.pdf

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 
98, 224-253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 23-44. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.23

Morry, M. M., & Kito, M. (2009). Relational-interdependent self-construal as a predictor of relationship quality: The mediating roles of 
one's own behaviors and perceptions of the fulfillment of friendship functions. Journal of Social Psychology, 149(3), 305-322. 
https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.149.3.305-322

Mossakowski, K. N. (2003). Coping with perceived discrimination: Does ethnic identity protect mental health? Journal of Health and 
Social Behaviour, 44, 318-331. https://doi.org/10.1037/t01210-000

Kateri, Papastylianou, & Karademas 81

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110383310
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.6.903
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.321
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0147-1767(02)000061-5
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00384.x
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201279012
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022105275959
https://doi.org/10.22365/jpsych.2019.304.311
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.09.003
http://www.mmo.gr/
https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.6.1.13
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2008.09.003
http://www.Idec.gr/iier/new/asian_migrants_en.pdf
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.23
https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.149.3.305-322
https://doi.org/10.1037/t01210-000
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Motti-Stefanidi, F., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2012). Perceived discrimination of immigrant youth living in Greece: How does group 
discrimination translate into personal discrimination? European Psychologist, 17(2), 93-104. 
https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000116

Motti-Stefanidi, F., Dalla, M., Papathanasiou, A., Takis, N., & Pavlopoulos, V. (2006). Ethnic identity, acculturation strategies and 
psychological strength of immigrant/repatriates students. In P. Kordoutis & V. Pavlopoulos (Eds.), Research fields in social 
psychology (pp. 108–125). Athens, Greece: Atrapos.

Motti-Stefanidi, F., Pavlopoulos, V., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2018). Immigrant youth acculturation and perceived discrimination: 
Longitudinal mediation by immigrant peers' acceptance/rejection. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 59, 36-45. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2018.03.001

Musso, P., Inguglia, C., & Coco, A. (2015). Acculturation profiles and perceived discrimination: Associations with psychosocial well-
being among Tunisian adolescents in Italy. Social Inquiry into Well-Being, 1(1), 76-90. https://doi.org/10.13165/SIIW-15-1-1-06

Papastylianou, A. (1992). The psychological adaptation of student-children repatriates: Research to repatriate students of high-school in 
Baribombe [Doctoral thesis]. Athens, Greece: National Archives of PhD Theses.

Papastylianou, Α. (2000). Psychosocial adaptation and stress in English speaking, Pontiacs, and Βορειοηπειρώτες adolescents students 
in Greece. Children and Adolescent, 2, 112-132. 

Paradies, Y. (2006). A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. International Journal of 
Epidemiology, 35, 888-901. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyl056

Pascoe, E. A., & Richman, L. S. (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 
531-554. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016059

Phalet, K., & Hagendoorn, L. (1996). Personal adjustment to acculturative transitions: The Turkish experience. International Journal of 
Psychology, 31, 131-144. https://doi.org/10.1080/002075996401142

Phinney, J. S., Madden, T., & Santos, L. J. (1998). Psychological variables as predictors of perceived ethnic discrimination among 
minority and immigrant adolescents. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 937-953. 
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01661.x

Postmes, T., & Branscombe, N. Y. (2002). Influence of long-term racial environment composition on subjective well-being in African 
Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 735-751. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.3.735

Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological 
Measurement, 1, 385-401. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306

Ramos, M. R., Cassidy, C., Reicher, S., & Haslam, S. A. (2016). A Longitudinal study of the effects of discrimination on the 
acculturation strategies of international students. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 47(3), 401-420. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022116628672

Safdar, S., Calvez, S., & Lewis, R. (2012). Multi-group analysis of the MIDA model: Acculturation of Indian and Russian immigrants to 
Canada. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 36, 200-212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.11.006

Safdar, S., Lay, C., & Struthers, W. (2003). The process of acculturation and basic goals: Testing a multidimensional individual 
difference acculturation model with Iranian immigrants in Canada. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 52, 555-579. 
https://doi.org/10.1111/1464-0597.00151

Sam, D. L. (2006). Acculturation of immigrant children and women. In D. Sam & J. W. Berry (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of 
acculturation psychology (pp. 403–419). Cambridge: University Press.

Sam, D. L., & Berry, J. W. (2010). Acculturation when individuals and groups of different cultural backgrounds meet. Perspectives on 
Psychological Science, 5(4), 472-481. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610373075

Schmitt, M. T., & Branscombe, N. R. (2002). The meaning and consequences of perceived discrimination in disadvantaged and 
privileged social groups. European Review of Social Psychology, 12(1), 169-199. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792772143000058

Schmitt, M. T., Branscombe, N. R., Postmes, T., & Garcia, A. (2014). The consequences of perceived discrimination for psychological 
well-being: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140(4), 1-28. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035754

Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 
20, 580-591. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205014

Spielberger, G. D., Gorush, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). The state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA USA: Consulting 
Psychologists Press.

Separation as Mediator and Interdependence as Moderator 82

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000116
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2018.03.001
https://doi.org/10.13165/SIIW-15-1-1-06
https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyl056
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016059
https://doi.org/10.1080/002075996401142
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01661.x
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.3.735
https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022116628672
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.11.006
https://doi.org/10.1111/1464-0597.00151
https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610373075
https://doi.org/10.1080/14792772143000058
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035754
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205014
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G Austin (Eds.), The psychology 
of intergroup relations (pp. 276–293). Chicago, IL, USA: Nelson-Hall.

Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and social behavior. New York, NY. USA: McGraw-Hill.
Triandis, H. C. (2001). Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality, 69, 907-924. 

https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.696169
Triantafyllidou, A. (2000). The political discourse on immigration in southern Europe: A critical analysis. Journal of Community & 

Applied Social Psychology, 10(5), 373-389. https://doi.org/10.1002/1099-1298
Tsouvelas, G., & Pavlopoulos, V. (2018). Greek host acculturation expectations towards immigrants from Albania and Pakistan. The 

European Journal of Counselling Psychology, 7(1), 191-199. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejcop.v7i1.151
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research. Newbury Park, CA, USA: SAGE.
Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2001). Self-esteem and threats to self: Implications for self-construal’s and interpersonal perceptions. 

Personality Processes and Individual Differences, 81(6), 1103-1118. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-351481.6.1103
Yamada, A.-M., & Singelis, T. M. (1999). Biculturalism and self-construal. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23, 697-709. 

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0147-1767(99)00016-4

A b o u t  t h e  A u t h o r s
Kateri Evangelia is an Adjunct Lecturer in Clinical Psychology at the Department of Psychology, University of Crete, Greece. 
Her research interests are mainly focused on the combination of Applied Cross-Cultural Psychology and Clinical Psychology, and 
specifically include immigrants and refugees adaptation and the role of culture in psychological health, psychological disorders, 
psychotherapy and, interpersonal relationships. E-mail: ekateri@gmail.com

Antonia (Dona) Papastylianou is a Professor in Social Psychology at the Νational and Kapodistrian University of Athens (Depart­
ment of Psychology). Her research interests are mainly focused in Applied Social Psychology and Cross-Cultural Psychology, and 
specifically include cross-cultural adaptation of migrants and remigrants, multicultural issues and psychosocial issues in the commun­
ity context, social values, social justice and social advocacy issues, emotions and group processes. E-mail: adpapast@psych.uoa.gr

Evangelos Karademas is a Professor of Clinical Health Psychology at the Department of Psychology, University of Crete, Greece. 
He also collaborates with the Foundation for Research and Technology—Hellas (FORTH). He is currently the President of the 
European Health Psychology Society. His research interests include patient adaptation to chronic illness, the role of stress and related 
factors in health and illness, individual differences in health, self-regulation and dyadic regulation. E-mail: karademas@uoc.gr

Kateri, Papastylianou, & Karademas 83

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(1), 70–83
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1531

https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.696169
https://doi.org/10.1002/1099-1298
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejcop.v7i1.151
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-351481.6.1103
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0147-1767(99)00016-4
https://www.psychopen.eu/

	Separation as Mediator and Interdependence as Moderator
	(Introduction)
	Perceived Discrimination and Psychological Well-Being
	The Role of Acculturation Attitudes: Separation as a Mediator
	The Role of Self-Construals: Interdependent Self-Construal as a Moderator
	The Present Study: The Research Context and Hypotheses

	Method
	Measures
	Procedure
	Participants

	Results
	Discussion
	Limitations and Conclusions

	(Additional Information)
	Funding
	Acknowledgments
	Competing Interests

	References
	About the Authors