Self Representations and Music Performance Anxiety: A Study With Professional and Amateur Musicians Research Reports Self Representations and Music Performance Anxiety: A Study With Professional and Amateur Musicians Claudia Castiglione a, Alberto Rampullo* b, Silvia Cardullo a [a] Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Messina, Messina, Italy. [b] Department of Political and Social Sciences, University of Catania, Catania, Italy. Abstract Individual, social and situational factors might play an important role on the experience of anxiety during musical performances. The present research focused on the relationship between self-representations, including musical self, and performance anxiety among a sample of Italian professional and amateur musicians (N = 100; age, M = 23.40, 50% females). We predicted that higher self-discrepancies (actual vs. future self) would be associated with higher performance anxiety in a musical setting (vs. a non musical one), via musical self, and only in professional musicians. The results confirmed our hypothesis. Higher discrepancies between actual and future self- representations were positively associated with higher performance anxiety levels via the musical self only in participants who play instruments at a professional level. Furthermore, musical self influenced performance anxiety levels in a music related setting (i.e., a concert) but not in a non musical one (i.e., an exam). Keywords: self-representations, musical performance, performance anxiety, proximity, semantic space Europe's Journal of Psychology, 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805, doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 Received: 2017-11-02. Accepted: 2018-04-06. Published (VoR): 2018-11-30. Handling Editors: Vlad Glăveanu, Department of Psychology, Webster University Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland; Andrew P. Allen, Maynooth University, Maynooth, Ireland *Corresponding author at: University of Catania, Department of Political and Social Sciences, Via Vittorio Emanuele II, 8, 95131, Catania, Italy. E-mail: albertorampullo@gmail.com This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Musical performance requires high levels of skill in several areas. Musicians need years of intensive training to develop the required sensorial, cognitive, and behavioural skills (Conrad & Roth, 2007; Kraus & Chandrasekaran, 2010; Standley, 2008). Furthermore, musicians place a great importance on the level and the quality of their performances because they recognize them as a fundamental element of their personal identity (Langendörfer, Hodapp, Kreutz, & Bongard, 2006). Indeed the quality of their performances affects their self- esteem and their career, and, in this context, their general tendency to perfectionism and meticulousness might be considered useful and desirable in the pursuit of their goals (Kirchner, 2003; Langendörfer et al., 2006). De- spite the common belief that years of practice could protect musicians from anxiety before or during musical performance, research showed that practice and experience cannot guarantee that musical performances will be free of feelings of anxiety and stress (Nagel, 1990; Taborsky, 2007). Many world-renowned musicians (e.g., Frederic Chopin, Sergei Rachmaninoff and Barbara Streisand) have ex- perienced music performance anxiety with negative physical and psychological consequences (Brodsky, 1996; Europe's Journal of Psychology ejop.psychopen.eu | 1841-0413 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ https://ejop.psychopen.eu/ https://ejop.psychopen.eu/ https://www.psychopen.eu/ Kenny, 2011). It could negatively affect not only the quality of the artist’s exhibition though an increase of anxi- ety levels before, or during, a performance in front of an audience, but it could have also negative effects on the self-esteem of the artist (Kenny, 2011; Sinden, 1999). Performance Anxiety in Musicians When an individual faces a threat, the human body reacts through a series of physical changes useful to facili- tate fight, flight or freeze response behaviours (Barlow, 2002; Maack, Buchanan, & Young, 2015; Martini, Timmons, & Tallitsch, 2012; Sinden, 1999). Anxiety levels have both positive and negative effects on musical performance. Intermediate levels of performance anxiety play an adaptive role, facilitating the use of some cop- ing strategy with positive effect on musical performance (Kenny, Davis, & Oates, 2004; Lehrer, Goldman, & Strommen, 1990; Steptoe & Fidler, 1987; Wolfe, 1989). However, several studies found that music performance anxiety had negative effects on musicians even if they are professional (James, 1998; Kenny et al., 2004; Van Kemenade, Van Son, & Van Heesch, 1995), and not only in training (Boucher & Ryan, 2011; Fehm & Schmidt, 2006). Indeed anxiety was shown to be positively related with poor musical performance experiences (Aderman et al., 1989; Kubzansky & Stewart, 1999) and, as a consequence of poor performance, with unhealthy coping strategies (Park, 2010) with the possibility to end prematurely their musical careers (Ryan & Andrews, 2009). Even so, more experienced students might perform better than less experienced peers under anxiety condi- tions (Hamann, 1982; Osborne, Kenny, & Holsomback, 2005). Research on musical performance anxiety has focused on several individual, social and situational factors like motivation, personality traits, emotions, type of instrument, performance setting, presence of an audience or type of performance (Kenny, 2011). Motivations behind musicians’ career path seem to play an important role. Individuals showed higher level of performance anxiety when their motivation was external, like desire to satisfy parents’ wishes (Seamands, 1999). Other individual factors might also play an important role. Perfectionism was shown to be associated with higher levels of anxiety in musicians (Kenny et al., 2004; Mor, Day, Flett, & Hewitt, 1995), especially socially prescribed perfectionism (Langendörfer et al., 2006). Actually, the social as- pect of performance seems to be a central aspect in how musicians cope with their anxiety levels. Cox and Kenardy (1993) found that anxiety increased when musicians performed solo on stage compared to practice and group sessions. Indeed the mere presence of other individuals may alter their performance if we consider Triplett’s research (1898) showing that cyclists register better race times when racing with others. Subsequently Zajonc (1968), using the theory of Hull-Spence (1956), extended Triplett’s (1898) results about social influences on performance. According to Zajonc (1968), the mere presence of a public creates a state of excitement re- sulting in a state of readiness. Thus, social influence can have facilitating or inhibitory effects on performance. The presence of co-actors or a public can improve the speed and accuracy of performance in simple or well- known tasks (social facilitation) and it can compromise performance in complex or poorly learned tasks (social inhibition; Bond & Titus, 1983). According Zajonc (1965) social facilitation occurs because the presence of others increases the individuals’ physiological activation, which makes some behaviour easier to perform. Physiological activation facilitates per- formance based on behaviours that are very accessible, simple, well learned or practiced. In opposition, phys- iological activation can inhibit instead performances based on complex or new behaviours. Social inhibition de- lineates all the conscious or unconscious avoidance behaviours of a specific situation or of a social interaction when individuals perceive the chance that other people might express feelings of disapproval (Denollet, 2013). Castiglione, Rampullo, & Cardullo 793 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Thus, social inhibition has proven to be related to social concerns for evaluations, anxiety about social interac- tions, social avoidance, and withdrawal (Denollet, 2013). Social inhibition also has an impact on musicians. For example, Brotons (1994) explored the effects of the performance setting (jury and nonjury situation) on music performance anxiety among a sample of music students, covering a wide range of music disciplines. Music per- formance anxiety was measured through several physiological, behavioural and self-reported measures and participants in a jury setting showed higher heart rates and anxiety levels as compared to a nonjury setting. Similarly, LeBlanc, Jin, Obert, and Siivola (1997) found that the presence of a group of spectators significantly increased anxiety levels and heart rates. Music performance anxiety have been linked to cognitive processes similar to social phobia and was proven to be higher when an audience was present during a solo performance as compared to group performance, in a private setting, and tape recording session (Osborne & Franklin, 2002). Musicians tend to react differently to an audience depending on their professional level. Music students experi- enced high levels of anxiety in front of an audience but more experienced students performed better than less experienced peers under anxiety conditions (Hamann, 1982; Osborne, Kenny, & Holsomback, 2005). Addition- ally, students who want to become professionals showed higher levels of performance anxiety (Robson & Kenny, 2017). A recent study (Barbar, de Souza Crippa, & de Lima Osório, 2014) among Brazilian musicians showed that professional and amateur musicians had similar music performance anxiety, but professional ones showed higher levels of general and social anxiety. Self Representations and Anxiety Representations about oneself shape and affect individuals’ behaviors, cognitions and emotions. These repre- sentations might concern beliefs about one’s own actual, ideal, undesired, future, or professional self (Hart et al., 1997; Higgins, Klein, & Strauman, 1985; Markus & Nurius, 1986; Ogilvie, 1987; Strauman & Higgins, 1988). The term future self delineates a set of knowledge about what one might become (Markus & Nurius, 1986). When individuals describe, or think about, themselves, they will use knowledge about characteristics that they believe they actually possess or they will likely acquire in the future. They will use self-knowledge about them- selves in the present and projected into the future. This set of knowledge, ‘Possible Selves’ (Markus & Nurius, 1986) refers to the beliefs about how an individual will, or might, become. Self-representations drive cognitions, emotions and behaviours and facilitate performances by focusing on ex- plicit goals and by strategically implementing plans of actions (Castiglione et al., 2015; Markus & Nurius, 1986; Markus & Ruvolo, 1989). Self-representations are structured in a semantic space (semantic space approach, Hart et al., 1997). Self representations’ descriptors and attributes delimit the position of self representations in the semantic space, the more similarly two self-representations are described or defined, the closer these rep- resentations will be located in the semantic space of an individual (Hart et al., 1997; Higgins et al., 1985). Closeness among self-representations might positively affect self-esteem, emotions, and behaviours (Field, Hart, & Horowitz, 1999; Hart & Fegley, 1995; Hart et al., 1997; Heppen & Ogilvie, 2003). Discrepancies and incongruences between self dimensions (Higgins et al., 1985; Markus & Nurius, 1986; Rogers, 1961) showed a negative effect on emotional states like depression, sadness, stress and anxiety (Hart et al., 1997; Heppen & Ogilvie, 2003; Higgins, 1989). Performances, goals and careers are also affected by self-concept (Oyserman & Markus, 1990). Self-images affect individuals’ cognitions, emotions (including anxiety) and behaviours (Castiglione, Licciardello, & Self Representations, and Music Performance Anxiety 794 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Rampullo, 2015; Hart, Field, Garfinkle, & Singer, 1997; Heppen & Ogilvie, 2003; Higgins, 1989; Markus & Nurius, 1986; Markus & Ruvolo, 1989) and thus, shape musicians’ performances and careers (Schnare, MacIntyre, & Doucette, 2012; Varvarigou, Creech, & Hallam, 2014). Self-representations were shown to play an important motivational role among musicians, with musical self positively affecting performances, goals and ca- reers in musicians (Oyserman & Markus, 1990; Schnare et al., 2012; Varvarigou et al., 2014). The latter have been related also to the performance setting, with musicians showing higher music performance anxiety in a jury situation (Brotons, 1994) or in a solo performance (Osborne & Franklin, 2002) compared to a non-jury sit- uation or a private setting, especially in professional musicians (Osborne et al., 2005). Some studies have examined the role of self representations in musicians (Schnare et al., 2012; Varvarigou et al., 2014) focusing on other individual and contextual factors, thus the present study aimed to extend previous literature examining the relationship between self representations and anxiety among a sample of Italian pro- fessional and amateur musicians. Aims and Hypotheses The present study focused on the relationship between self-representations and anxiety among a sample of Italian professional and amateur musicians. We predicted that self-discrepancies (actual vs. future self) would be negatively associated with musical self, thus higher self-discrepancies would be associated to a less positive representation of the musical self. We expected also that musical self would be negatively associated with mu- sic performance anxiety, thus a more positive representation of the musical self would be associated with lower levels of performance anxiety. Finally, we predicted that higher self-discrepancies (actual vs. future self) would be associated with higher performance anxiety in a musical setting as compared to a non-musical one, via the musical self, and only in professional musicians (see Figure 1). Figure 1. Suggested moderated mediation model. Self-discrepancies (actual vs. future self) associated with music performance anxiety, via the musical self, only in professional musicians. Castiglione, Rampullo, & Cardullo 795 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Method Participants The sample consisted of 100 musicians (n = 50 professionals, n = 50 males) with an average age of 23.40 (SD = 3.80; range 18-30) and an average of 11.26 years of playing an instrument (SD = 4.80; range 2-23). Measures Performance Anxiety Performance Anxiety was measured using an Italian Validated version (Pedrabissi & Santinello, 1989) of the State Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, 1983). It is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 40 items rated on a 6- point Likert scale (1-never to 6-always) about anxiety levels in different social situations. The two social situa- tions we used for the purpose of this research were: a university exam (an oral exam in front of other students; Exam, α = .90) and a concert (α = .88). Both are performance situation in front of an audience, but only the concert situation is a music performance. Higher score refers to higher levels of performance anxiety. Self Representations Two semantic differentials were used to measure actual and future self (Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1957). They were made up of 34 7-point bipolar scales (e.g., strong – weak). This version was already tested in the Sicilian context (see Castiglione, Licciardello, Mauceri, & Rampullo, 2012; De Caroli & Sagone, 2012). It was used to measure the representations of actual self (me as I am) (α = .86), future self (me as I will be) (α = .89), and musical self (the musician) (α = .85). Based on the semantic space approach (Hart et al., 1997), the loca- tion of self-representations in individuals’ semantic space is based on the similarity of the self-representations’ descriptors or attributes (Hart et al., 1997; Higgins et al., 1985). Thus, discrepancy was operationalized as Eu- clidean distance (Hafdahl, Panter, Gramzow, Sedikides, & Insko, 2000; Kirchler, Palmonari, & Pombeni, 1994) between descriptors of the actual and future self and using the Pythagorean Theorem (square root of the sum- med squared differences between each item/adjective pair). A value of 0 refers identical description of actual and future self, higher values refers to higher discrepancy between them. Background Questions Background questions were used to get age and gender information of our participants. Results We conducted a series of independent sample t-tests to verify professional level (professional vs. amateur) ef- fects on self-representations discrepancies (actual vs. future self), musical self, and performance anxiety (exam and concert). Professional level showed a significant effect on self-representations discrepancies (actual vs. future self) t(98) = 4.757, p < .001 and on concert performance anxiety t(98) = 2.222, p = .03. Professional participants showed higher self-representations discrepancies and concert performance anxiety compared to amateur participants (see Table 1). Self Representations, and Music Performance Anxiety 796 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations and Correlations Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 Amateur 1. Actual self vs future self discrepancies 1.15 .82 – 2. Musical self 5.07 .50 -.07 – 3. Exam performance anxiety 54.74 10.52 -.48** -.07 – 4. Concert performance anxiety 48.24 8.26 .30* -.47** .18 – Professional 1. Actual self vs future self discrepancies 1.81 .55 – 2. Musical self 4.90 .74 -.34* – 3. Exam performance anxiety 57.95 10.41 .33* -.12 – 4. Concert performance anxiety 52.28 9.85 .13 -.50** .38** – Overall 1. Actual self vs future self discrepancies 1.47 .77 – 2. Musical self 4.99 .64 -.23* – 3. Exam performance anxiety 56.35 10.54 -.44** -.12 – 4. Concert performance anxiety 50.26 9.27 .28** -.50** .31** – **p < .01. *p < .05. In Amateur participants, we found medium negative correlations between self discrepancies and exam perform- ance anxiety. Furthermore, we found medium negative correlation between musical self and concert perform- ance anxiety. In Professional participants, self discrepancies were negatively related with musical self and posi- tively with exam anxiety. Musical self was also negatively related with concert performance anxiety (see Table 1). Zero order correlations, means and standard deviations for self-representations discrepancies (actual vs. future self), musical self, and performance anxiety (exam and concert) are shown in Table 1. To test our hypothesis we followed Preacher, Rucker, and Hayes (2007) approach to moderated mediation analysis. Initially, we tested if the path from the predictor (i.e., self-discrepancies) to the mediator (i.e., musical self) was affected or not by the proposed moderator professional level (professional vs. amateur). Subsequent- ly, we tested the path between the mediator and the outcome measures (performance anxiety in musical and non-musical setting). Finally, the moderated mediation model was test through PROCESS macro, Model #7 (Hayes, 2013). Firstly, we tested through a multiple hierarchical regression analyses (Baron & Kenny, 1986) the effects of the predictor (self discrepancies) on the proposed mediator (musical self) and the interaction between the predictor and the professional level (professional vs. amateur). Variables were centred to avoid multicollinearity (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). To test the interaction between the predictor (self discrepancies) and professio- nal level, one interaction term was created. If the interaction term significantly contributed to the regression model, we proceeded to investigate conditional effect of self-discrepancies on musical self at moderator values (professional vs. amateur). We tested the effects of self-discrepancies and the professional level on musical self. Self discrepancies showed a significant and negative main effect, b = -.25, t(98) = -3.066, p = .003 on musical self, R2adj = .10, F(3, Castiglione, Rampullo, & Cardullo 797 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://www.psychopen.eu/ 96) = 4.67, p = .004. The addition of the interaction term significantly contributed to the regression model, R2change = .04, Fchange (1, 96) = 6.39, p = .01. The interaction between self discrepancies and the professional level was significant, t(96) = -2.528, p = .01, suggesting that the effect of self discrepancies on musical self de- pends on professional level. Simple slopes analysis showed that self-discrepancies did not significantly affect musical self for amateur musicians. On the other hand, high discrepancies between actual and future self had a significant negative effect on musical self-representation for professional musicians, b = -.45, SE = .14, t(96) = -3.189, p = .002 (see Figure 2). Figure 2. Musical self levels as a function of actual vs. future self-discrepancies and professional level. After testing the path between the predictor and the proposed mediator, we tested the paths between musical self representation and performance anxiety (exam and concert) through a series of regression analyses with musical self representation and professional level entered simultaneously in the model. Only musical self-repre- sentation had significant influence on concert performance anxiety: more positive musical self-representation was significantly associated with lower concert performance anxiety, b = -6.94, t = -5.433, p < .001, R2adj = .27, F(2, 97) = 17.944, p < .001. As hypothesized, musical self-representation and professional level were not signif- icantly associated with performance anxiety in a non-musical setting (exam). Finally, to test if the indirect effects of self-discrepancies on performance anxiety through musical self (media- tor) depend on professional level or not, we used the Model 7 of the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2013). We tes- ted the moderated mediation model with concert performance anxiety as outcome variable using bootstrapping with 5,000 resamples. The index of moderated mediation was significant (Hayes, 2013), index = 2.73, SE = 1.23, 95% CI [.80, 5.65], indicating that professional level moderated the mediation effect of musical self on the relationship between self-discrepancies and concert performance anxiety. Specifically, the indirect effect of self- discrepancies on concert performance anxiety via musical self was significant only for professional musicians, ab = 3.02, SE = 1.21, 95% CI [1.09, 5.99]. Self Representations, and Music Performance Anxiety 798 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Discussion Studies on musicians’ performance anxiety have explored different individual, social and situational variables like motivation, personality, emotions, or performance setting (Kenny et al., 2004; LeBlanc et al., 1997; Mor et al., 1995; Seamands, 1999). Performance setting (like jury or non-jury situation) might affect how the musician experiences the performance and the levels of anxiety that they feel (Brotons, 1994; Osborne & Franklin, 2002; Osborne et al., 2005). However, musicians might react differently based on their professional level, with profes- sionals experiencing higher levels of anxiety (Osborne et al., 2005) even if they have been shown to cope bet- ter under anxiety conditions (Hamann, 1982). Finally, self-representation might affect performance (Schnare et al., 2012; Varvarigou et al., 2014) and anxiety levels (Hart et al., 1997; Heppen & Ogilvie, 2003; Higgins, 1989) also in musicians. The present research focused on the relationship between self-representations and anxiety among a sample of Italian professional and amateur musicians. More specifically, we predicted that higher self-discrepancies (ac- tual vs. future self) would be associated with higher performance anxiety in a musical setting as compared to a non-musical one, via the musical self, and only in professional musicians. The results supported our prediction that self-discrepancies (actual vs. future self) were negatively associated with the representation of musical self, thus higher discrepancy between actual and future self was associated with less positive representation of musical self. These results seems support the motivational role of possible selves (Markus & Nurius, 1986), especially if we consider that the negative association between self discrepan- cies and musical self emerged only for professional musicians. Varvarigou et al. (2014) already described how supporting possible selves through positive role models elicited detailed descriptions of students’ musical self and a positive motivational effect toward musical activities. Coherently with previous results and with our predictions, musical self was negatively associated with perform- ance anxiety only in a musical setting (concert). These results support the idea that performance setting could play an important role. Previous research (Brotons, 1994; Osborne & Franklin, 2002; Osborne et al., 2005) al- ready showed how musicians reported different level of anxiety based on the performance setting (e.g. jury – non-jury situation). Finally, in line with literature on self dimensions (Castiglione et al., 2015; Markus & Nurius, 1986; Markus & Ruvolo, 1989), and with literature on differences between professional and amateur musicians (Hamann, 1982; Osborne et al., 2005), higher discrepancies between self-representations were associated with higher performance anxiety levels via the musical self only in participants who play instruments at a professio- nal level. Furthermore, coherently with literature on effects of the settings on music performance anxiety (Osborne & Franklin, 2002), musical self was associated with performance anxiety only in a music related set- ting. Results suggests that musical self might play an important role as a mechanism through which musicians could more easily manage performance anxiety in musical performance setting. Limits of the Study A major concern of the present study was that music performance anxiety was measured with only a self-repor- ted measure. Future studies should explore the effect of self dimensions on anxiety levels through the use of physiological and behavioural measures (Brotons, 1994; LeBlanc et al., 1997). Furthermore, data showed a positive correlation between exam and concert performance anxiety, thus indicating that anxious people might Castiglione, Rampullo, & Cardullo 799 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://www.psychopen.eu/ be more anxious no matter the context. Nevertheless musical self representation and professional level were not significantly associated with performance anxiety in a non-musical setting (exam), indicating that the setting might play, at least in part, an important role. Another limit is related to the use of a correlational design. Previous studies have addressed already the causal path linking self-images discrepancies to anxiety (Heppen & Ogilvie, 2003) employing different research de- signs (for example, a longitudinal design). Future studies should implement also other research designs (for ex- ample, an experimental design). Future studies could consider also other factors central for professional devel- opment and representation of self-dimensions like creativity (De Caroli, Licciardello, Sagone, & Castiglione, 2010; Gladding, 2008) or self-efficacy (Brouwers & Tomic, 2000; Skaalvik & Skaalvik, 2007). Conclusion The results of the present study showed for the first time that self-representations discrepancies were associ- ated with performance anxiety in a musical setting (vs. a non musical one), via musical self, only in professional musicians, thus extending previous literature on the relationship between possible selves (Markus & Nurius, 1986) and music performance anxiety. The results also seem fundamental for the literature on educational in- tervention to promote and sustain a musical career with youth scholars (Varvarigou et al., 2014). Intervetions focused only on contextual factors should include individual variables through the employment of strategies aimed at reducing self-images discrepancies and sustaining a positive musical self representation. Professional psychological support might be directed on both improvement of own self-image and supporting a positive rep- resentations of musicians as a professional group. Funding The authors have no funding to report. Competing Interests The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. Acknowledgments The authors have no support to report. R ef er enc es Aderman, M., Baker, T., Bohnenblust, B., Hunget, R., & Villines, M. (1989). Performance anxiety and non music majors. Dialogues in Instrumental Music Education, 13(1), 40-48. Barbar, A. E. M., de Souza Crippa, J. A., & de Lima Osório, F. (2014). Performance anxiety in Brazilian musicians: Prevalence and association with psychopathology indicators. Journal of Affective Disorders, 152-154(1), 381-386. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.09.041 Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press. Self Representations, and Music Performance Anxiety 800 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.09.041 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 Bond, C. F., & Titus, L. J. (1983). Social facilitation: A meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychological Bulletin, 94(2), 265-292. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.94.2.265 Boucher, H., & Ryan, C. A. (2011). Performance stress and the very young musician. Journal of Research in Music Education, 58(4), 329-345. doi:10.1177/0022429410386965 Brodsky, W. (1996). Music performance anxiety reconceptualized. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 11(3), 88-98. Brotons, M. (1994). Effects of performing conditions on music performance anxiety and performance quality. Journal of Music Therapy, 31(1), 63-81. doi:10.1093/jmt/31.1.63 Brouwers, A., & Tomic, W. (2000). A longitudinal study of teacher burnout and perceived self-efficacy in classroom management. Teaching and Teacher Education, 16(2), 239-253. doi:10.1016/S0742-051X(99)00057-8 Castiglione, C., Licciardello, O., & Rampullo, A. (2015). Possible future changes in menopausal women. Life Span and Disability, 18(2), 189-205. Castiglione, C., Licciardello, O., Mauceri, M., & Rampullo, A. (2012). Current experience and future plans in Italian adolescents of secondary schools. Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences, 46, 5593-5602. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.06.481 Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple correlation/regression analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Conrad, A., & Roth, W. T. (2007). Muscle relaxation therapy for anxiety disorders: It works but how? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21(3), 243-264. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.08.001 Cox, W. J., & Kenardy, J. (1993). Performance anxiety, social phobia, and setting effects in instrumental music students. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 7(1), 49-60. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(93)90020-L De Caroli, M. E., Licciardello, O., Sagone, E., & Castiglione, C. (2010). Methods to measure the extent to which teachers’ points of view influence creativity and factors of creative personality: A study with Italian pupils. Key Engineering Materials, 437, 535-539. doi:10.4028/www.scientific.net/KEM.437.535 De Caroli, M. E., & Sagone, E. (2012). Professional self representation and risk of burnout in school teachers. Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences, 46, 5509-5515. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.06.466 Denollet, J. (2013). Interpersonal sensitivity, social inhibition, and Type D personality: How and when are they associated with health? Comment on Marin and Miller. Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 991-997. doi:10.1037/a0033537 Fehm, L., & Schmidt, K. (2006). Performance anxiety in gifted adolescent musicians. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(1), 98-109. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.11.011 Field, N. P., Hart, D., & Horowitz, M. J. (1999). Representations of self and other in conjugal bereavement. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16(3), 407-414. doi:10.1177/0265407599163006 Castiglione, Rampullo, & Cardullo 801 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.94.2.265 https://doi.org/10.1177/0022429410386965 https://doi.org/10.1093/jmt/31.1.63 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0742-051X(99)00057-8 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.06.481 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.08.001 https://doi.org/10.1016/0887-6185(93)90020-L https://doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/KEM.437.535 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.06.466 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033537 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.11.011 https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407599163006 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Gladding, S. T. (2008). The impact of creativity in counseling. Journal of Creativity in Mental Health, 3(2), 97-104. doi:10.1080/15401380802226679 Hafdahl, A. R., Panter, A. T., Gramzow, R. H., Sedikides, C., & Insko, C. A. (2000). Free‐response self‐discrepancies across, among, and within FFM personality dimensions. Journal of Personality, 68(1), 111-151. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00093 Hamann, D. L. (1982). An assessment of anxiety in instrumental and vocal performances. Journal of Research in Music Education, 30(2), 77-90. doi:10.2307/3345040 Hart, D., & Fegley, S. (1995). Prosocial behavior and caring in adolescence: Relations to self‐understanding and social judgment. Child Development, 66(5), 1346-1359. doi:10.2307/1131651 Hart, D., Field, N. P., Garfinkle, J. R., & Singer, J. L. (1997). Representations of self and other: A semantic space model. Journal of Personality, 65(1), 77-105. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00530.x Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press. Heppen, J. B., & Ogilvie, D. M. (2003). Predicting affect from global self-discrepancies: The dual role of the undesired self. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22(4), 347-368. doi:10.1521/jscp.22.4.347.22898 Higgins, E. T. (1989). Self-discrepancy theory: What patterns of self-beliefs cause people to suffer? Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 93-136. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60306-8 Higgins, E. T., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1985). Self-concept discrepancy theory: A psychological model for distinguishing among different aspects of depression and anxiety. Social Cognition, 3(1), 51-76. doi:10.1521/soco.1985.3.1.51 James, I. (1998). Western orchestral musicians are highly stressed. Resonance: International Music Council, 26, 19-20. Kenny, D. (2011). The psychology of music performance anxiety. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. Kenny, D. T., Davis, P., & Oates, J. (2004). Music performance anxiety and occupational stress amongst opera chorus artists and their relationship with state and trait anxiety and perfectionism. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18(6), 757-777. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2003.09.004 Kirchler, E., Palmonari, A., & Pombeni, M. L. (1994). Social categorization processes as dependent on status differences between groups: A step into adolescents’ peer‐groups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(5), 541-563. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420240503 Kirchner, J. M. (2003). A qualitative inquiry into musical performance anxiety. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 18(2), 78-82. Kraus, N., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 11(8), 599-605. doi:10.1038/nrn2882 Kubzansky, L. D., & Stewart, A. J. (1999). At the intersection of anxiety, gender, and performance. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18(1), 76-97. doi:10.1521/jscp.1999.18.1.76 Self Representations, and Music Performance Anxiety 802 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://doi.org/10.1080/15401380802226679 https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.00093 https://doi.org/10.2307/3345040 https://doi.org/10.2307/1131651 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00530.x https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.22.4.347.22898 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60306-8 https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1985.3.1.51 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2003.09.004 https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420240503 https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2882 https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1999.18.1.76 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Langendörfer, F., Hodapp, V., Kreutz, G., & Bongard, S. (2006). Personality and performance anxiety among professional orchestra musicians. Journal of Individual Differences, 27(3), 162-171. doi:10.1027/1614-0001.27.3.162 LeBlanc, A., Jin, Y. C., Obert, M., & Siivola, C. (1997). Effect of audience on music performance anxiety. Journal of Research in Music Education, 45(3), 480-496. doi:10.2307/3345541 Lehrer, P. M., Goldman, N. S., & Strommen, E. F. (1990). A principal components assessment of performance anxiety among musicians. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 5(1), 12-18. Maack, D. J., Buchanan, E., & Young, J. (2015). Development and psychometric investigation of an inventory to assess fight, flight, and freeze tendencies: The fight, flight, freeze questionnaire. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 44(2), 117-127. doi:10.1080/16506073.2014.972443 Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. The American Psychologist, 41(9), 954-969. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954 Markus, H., & Ruvolo, A. (1989). Possible selves: Personalized representations of goals. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Goal concepts in personality and social psychology (pp. 211-241). Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum. Martini, F. H., Timmons, M. J., & Tallitsch, R. B. (2012). Anatomia Umana. Napoli, Italy: EdiSES. Mor, S., Day, H. I., Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (1995). Perfectionism, control, and components of performance anxiety in professional artists. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19(2), 207-225. doi:10.1007/BF02229695 Nagel, J. J. (1990). Performance anxiety and the performing musician: A fear of failure or a fear of success. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 5(1), 37-40. Ogilvie, D. M. (1987). The undesired self: A neglected variable in personality research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(2), 379-385. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.2.379 Osborne, M. S., & Franklin, J. (2002). Cognitive processes in music performance anxiety. Australian Journal of Psychology, 54(2), 86-93. doi:10.1080/00049530210001706543 Osborne, M. S., Kenny, D. T., & Holsomback, R. (2005). Assessment of music performance anxiety in late childhood: A validation study of the Music Performance Anxiety Inventory for Adolescents (MPAI-A). International Journal of Stress Management, 12(4), 312-330. doi:10.1037/1072-5245.12.4.312 Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL, USA: University of Illinois Press. Oyserman, D., & Markus, H. R. (1990). Possible selves and delinquency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(1), 112-125. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.1.112 Park, C. L. (2010). Making sense of the meaning literature: An integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 257-301. doi:10.1037/a0018301 Pedrabissi, L., & Santinello, M. (1989). Inventario per l’ansia di «Stato» e di «Tratto»: Nuova versione italiana dello STAI Forma Y: Manuale. Firenze, Italy: Organizzazioni Speciali. Castiglione, Rampullo, & Cardullo 803 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001.27.3.162 https://doi.org/10.2307/3345541 https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2014.972443 https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02229695 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.2.379 https://doi.org/10.1080/00049530210001706543 https://doi.org/10.1037/1072-5245.12.4.312 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.1.112 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018301 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42(1), 185-227. doi:10.1080/00273170701341316 Robson, K. E., & Kenny, D. T. (2017). Music performance anxiety in ensemble rehearsals and concerts: A comparison of music and non-music major undergraduate musicians. Psychology of Music, 45(6), 868-885. doi:10.1177/0305735617693472 Rogers, C. R. (1961). The characteristics of a helping relationship. In C. R. Rogers (Ed.), On becoming a person: A therapist's view of psychotherapy (pp. 39-58). Boston, MA, USA: Houghton Mifflin. Ryan, C., & Andrews, N. (2009). An investigation into the choral singer’s experience of music performance anxiety. Journal of Research in Music Education, 57(2), 108-126. doi:10.1177/0022429409336132 Schnare, B., MacIntyre, P., & Doucette, J. (2012). Possible selves as a source of motivation for musicians. Psychology of Music, 40(1), 94-111. doi:10.1177/0305735610391348 Seamands, D. A. (1999), Liberi dalla trappola della performance: Come liberarsi dall’ansia di riuscire. Varese, Italy: Uomini Nuovi. Sinden, L. M. (1999). Music performance anxiety: Contributions of perfectionism, coping style, self-efficacy, and self- esteem. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 60(3-A), 0590. Skaalvik, E. M., & Skaalvik, S. (2007). Dimensions of teacher self-efficacy and relations with strain factors, perceived collective teacher efficacy, and teacher burnout. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 611-625. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.611 Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (form Y)("self-evaluation questionnaire"). Palo Alto, CA, USA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Standley, J. M. (2008). Does music instruction help children learn to read? Evidence of a meta-analysis. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 27(1), 17-32. Steptoe, A., & Fidler, H. (1987). Stage fright in orchestral musicians: A study of cognitive and behavioural strategies in performance anxiety. British Journal of Psychology, 78(2), 241-249. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1987.tb02243.x Strauman, T. J., & Higgins, E. T. (1988). Self‐discrepancies as predictors of vulnerability to distinct syndromes of chronic emotional distress. Journal of Personality, 56(4), 685-707. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1988.tb00472.x Taborsky, C. (2007). Musical performance anxiety: A review of literature. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 26(1), 15-25. Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. The American Journal of Psychology, 9(4), 507-533. doi:10.2307/1412188 Van Kemenade, J. F., Van Son, M. J., & Van Heesch, N. C. (1995). Performance anxiety among professional musicians in symphonic orchestras: A self-report study. Psychological Reports, 77(2), 555-562. doi:10.2466/pr0.1995.77.2.555 Varvarigou, M., Creech, A., & Hallam, S. (2014). Partnership working and possible selves in music education. International Journal of Music Education, 32(1), 84-97. doi:10.1177/0255761413491060 Self Representations, and Music Performance Anxiety 804 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 https://doi.org/10.1080/00273170701341316 https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735617693472 https://doi.org/10.1177/0022429409336132 https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735610391348 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.611 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1987.tb02243.x https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1988.tb00472.x https://doi.org/10.2307/1412188 https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.77.2.555 https://doi.org/10.1177/0255761413491060 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Wolfe, M. L. (1989). Correlates of adaptive and maladaptive musical performance anxiety. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 4(1), 49-56. Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149(3681), 269-274. doi:10.1126/science.149.3681.269 Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2, Pt.2), 1-27. doi:10.1037/h0025848 A bout the Aut hor s Castiglione Claudia, Ph.D., is a research fellow at University of Messina, Italy. Her main research interests are Possible selves and Social Identity. Alberto Rampullo, Ph.D., is a Psychologist. His main research interests focuses on Intregroup Relations, Gender studies and Leadership. Silvia Cardullo is a Psychologist in Messina, Italy. Her main research interests are Music and Social psychology. Castiglione, Rampullo, & Cardullo 805 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2018, Vol. 14(4), 792–805 doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i4.1554 PsychOpen GOLD is a publishing service by Leibniz Institute for Psychology Information (ZPID), Trier, Germany. www.leibniz-psychology.org https://doi.org/10.1126/science.149.3681.269 https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025848 https://www.leibniz-psychology.org/ https://www.psychopen.eu/ Self Representations, and Music Performance Anxiety (Introduction) Performance Anxiety in Musicians Method Participants Measures Results Discussion Limits of the Study Conclusion (Additional Information) Funding Competing Interests Acknowledgments References About the Authors