Traumatic Rift: How Conspiracy Beliefs Undermine Cohesion After Societal Trauma? Research Reports Traumatic Rift: How Conspiracy Beliefs Undermine Cohesion After Societal Trauma? Michal Bilewicz a, Marta Witkowska* a, Myrto Pantazi b, Theofilos Gkinopoulos c, Olivier Klein d [a] Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland. [b] Oxford Internet Institute, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom. [c] School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, United Kingdom. [d] Faculty of Psychology and Education, University Libre de Bruxelles, Bruxelles, Belgium. Abstract Collective traumas may often lead to deep societal divides and internal conflicts. In this article, we propose that conspiracy theories emerging in response to victimizing events may play a key role in the breakdown of social cohesion. We performed a nationally representative survey in Poland (N = 965) two years after the Smoleńsk airplane crash in which the Polish president was killed, together with 95 political officials and high-ranking military officers. The survey found that people endorsing conspiratorial accounts of the Smoleńsk catastrophe preferred to distance themselves from conspiracy non-believers, while skeptics preferred greater distance to conspiracy believers. We also examined the role of people’s belief in the uniqueness of in-group historical suffering as an important antecedent of both conspiracy thinking and hostility towards outgroups (conspiracy believers and non-believers). Keywords: victimhood, collective trauma, conspiracy beliefs, social distance, Smoleńsk Europe's Journal of Psychology, 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93, https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 Received: 2018-07-04. Accepted: 2018-11-06. Published (VoR): 2019-02-28. Handling Editors: Ljiljana Lazarevic, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia; Iris Zezelj, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia *Corresponding author at: Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Stawki 5/7, 00-183 Warsaw, Poland. E-mail: marta.witkowska@psych.uw.edu.pl This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, CC BY 4.0 (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. "Don't wipe your treacherous mugs with the name of my late brother. You destroyed him, you murdered him!" – said the leader of the ruling Polish party, Jarosław Kaczyński, in a heated parliamentary debate in July 2017, while addressing the opposition party leaders. With these words he referred to the alleged responsibility of the former liberal government for the 2010 airplane crash in which the then President of Poland, Lech Kaczyński, was killed. Today, the two sides of the political debate in Poland still cannot agree upon the nature of the trau- matic event that happened more than eight years ago. Psychological research on large-scale societal traumas suggests that societies exposed to traumatic events may become more cohesive. Societal traumas can increase altruism and prosocial behavior (Staub & Vollhardt, 2008; Vollhardt, 2009), enhance ethnocentric responses (Fritsche, Jonas, & Kessler, 2011), and boost national- ism and patriotism (Skitka, 2005). Linguistic studies of on-line journals after the September 11th attacks point to increased social references immediately after the collective trauma occurred (Cohn, Mehl, & Pennebaker, 2004). On the other hand, it is obvious that such traumatic events often lead to deep societal divides and intra- group conflicts – particularly when they occur in the political domain. The crash of the airplane carrying the Europe's Journal of Psychology ejop.psychopen.eu | 1841-0413 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ https://ejop.psychopen.eu/ https://ejop.psychopen.eu/ https://www.psychopen.eu/ Rwandan president Juvenal Habyarimana and the Burundian president Cyprien Ntaryamira led to the Rwandan Genocide and a violent war in the Democratic Republic Congo. The September 11th attacks in the USA en- hanced decades-long partisan conflict between Democrats and Republicans. The massacre in the Beslan school in 2004 escalated Ingush-Ossetian hostilities and led to human rights abuses in Russia. This societal process, known as the “cycle of violence” (Littman & Paluck, 2015), begs for a deeper explanation, particularly as far as its psychological foundations are concerned. In this article, we analyze the key role of conspiracy theories in the breakdown of social cohesiveness after col- lective traumatic events. We suggest that large-scale societal traumas (such as terrorist attacks, wars, or the assassination of political leaders) can lead to disintegration and societal divisions due to the heightened need for a satisfactory explanation. When people confront large and significant events, they seek explanations that would be proportional to them in terms of significance and scale (Fiedler, Freytag, & Unkelbach, 2011). There- fore, large and significant events like accidents or disasters of national significance are more likely to elicit causal explanations that are less based on chance and rather entail the intentional activities of some individual or group. In other words, collective traumas are especially likely to elicit conspiratorial explanations, because the assumption that they are the mere result of random events is not psychologically satisfying (Klein, Van der Linden, Pantazi, & Kissine, 2015). Conspiracy theories seem to address this need, especially for those people who construe their social identity through membership in groups that are eternally victimized and the target of conspiracies and aggressive acts (Noor, Shnabel, Halabi, & Nadler, 2012; Schori-Eyal, Klar, Roccas, & McNeill, 2017). What is more, in becoming advocates of such theories, people may start considering non-believers to be potential enemies posing a threat to their epistemic certainty. Conspiracy non-believers, on the other hand, may tend to distance themselves from conspiracy believers, whom they take to be “carriers” of contagious, irrational interpretations of the traumatic event. Ultimately, a society suffering from a collective trauma may become high- ly polarized over these different interpretations of the traumatic event, thereby undermining social cohesiveness and leading to internal conflicts and clashes. Conspiracy Theories and Their Consequences The vast majority of psychological research on conspiracy theories has focused on the antecedents of people’s tendency to believe in conspiracies (Douglas & Sutton, 2011; Swami, 2012; van Prooijen & Jostmann, 2013) and the individual-level correlates of this tendency (Abalakina-Paap, Stephan, Craig, & Gregory, 1999; Bruder, Haffke, Neave, Nouripanah, & Imhoff, 2013; Goertzel, 1994). People who believe in conspiracies are less trust- ful, more insecure, and more anomic (Goertzel, 1994), alienated, hostile (Abalakina-Paap et al., 1999), uncer- tain (van Prooijen & Jostmann, 2013) and relatively deprived (Bilewicz & Krzeminski, 2010), while they lack a sense of (socio-political) control (Bruder et al., 2013; Whitson & Galinsky, 2008). However, little is known about the consequences of conspiracy beliefs – both at the individual and the societal level. The few studies testing the consequences of conspiracy theories have shown that conspiracy theories breed feelings of powerless- ness, uncertainty, and disillusionment, that in turn may lead to decreased political engagement, lower pro-envi- ronmental behavior (Jolley & Douglas, 2014a), and lower vaccination intentions (Jolley & Douglas, 2014b). Several studies suggest that the endorsement of conspiracy beliefs by community members might be associ- ated with lower levels of social cohesiveness within the community. When societies are confronted with a large- scale traumatic event, conspiracy-driven explanations of the event will be shared by part of the population, while at the same time other people will not endorse such beliefs. One could expect that due to mistrust, aliena- Bilewicz, Witkowska, Pantazi et al. 83 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 https://www.psychopen.eu/ tion, and deprivation, conspiracy believers would distance themselves from non-believers, thereby creating a rift in the societies affected by the traumatic event. Exclusive Victimhood as an Interpretative Framework Clearly, people differ in how prone they are to be drawn to conspiracy theories when faced with collective trau- matic events. In addition to the many individual psychological factors responsible for the endorsement of con- spiracy beliefs mentioned above, collective traumatic experiences encourage the use of larger collective narra- tives about the in-group’s historical position in order to explain current events. Such narratives, known as “group charters” (Liu & Hilton, 2005), define the historical role of a given national group as that of historical vic- tims, conquerors, heroes or villains. The role of historical victim could especially motivate the tendency to ex- plain contemporary traumatic events by means of conspiracy theories. Psychological research has identified many facets of perceived in-group victimhood, such as competitive vic- timhood (Noor, Brown, & Prentice, 2008), perpetual in-group victimhood (Schori-Eyal, Klar, & Ben-Ami, 2017), and exclusive victimhood consciousness (Vollhardt, 2015; Vollhardt & Bilali, 2015) that may play a destructive role in intergroup relations. All these facets of victimhood share a crucial feature, namely, the perception of the in-group as being unique in its victimhood. Past literature has shown that the perception of the in-group through such lenses of uniqueness affects intergroup processes, such as reconciliation or forgiveness (Noor et al., 2008). However, it is also likely that in-group victimhood affects the interpretation of political events and general levels of mistrust. One such effect can be, for example, that people who endorse narratives based on the uniqueness of past in-group suffering will be more inclined to believe in conspiracy theories regarding contem- porary events. Current Study: The Smoleńsk Plane Crash as a Societal Trauma Societal trauma is defined in social and political psychology “as an experience that invalidates one’s normal as- sumptions of order, predictability, safety, and identity, a very severe environmental challenge calling for the ut- most energization of coping resources” (Suedfeld, 1997, p. 850). Here, we would like to focus on a recent case of such a socially shared traumatic experience for Polish society – the Smoleńsk airplane crash in 2010. On April 10, 2010, a Polish Air Force flight crashed near the city of Smoleńsk, Russia. Ninety-six officials who were traveling to commemorate the 70th anniversary of the Katyń massacre died in the accident, including the Polish president, Lech Kaczyński and his wife, the former President of Poland Ryszard Kaczorowski, several Polish government officials, top military officers, and key politicians from both the political right and left (Harding, 2010). This sudden loss of the political and military elite created a sense of threat and instability among Poles. In the days following the crash, many spontaneous and organized acts of mourning took place throughout the whole country, including thousands of candles being lit in front of the Presidential Palace in War- saw, soon to become the center of these commemorative practices. Since the crash in 2010, every month a group of mourners assembles in front of the Presidential Palace to commemorate the late president, and to call for an investigation into the causes of the crash. The desire to explain the catastrophe has created a rift in Polish society. In a short time, the catastrophe be- came a breeding ground for conspiracy explanations (Borger & Pidd, 2011). The belief in Russian sabotage as the cause of the crash and the potential involvement of the Polish liberal government therein (the so-called Conspiracy Beliefs and Societal Trauma 84 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 https://www.psychopen.eu/ “Smolensk Conspiracy”), has been endorsed by 25% of Poles and has been particularly popular among right- wing voters (see Grzesiak-Feldman & Haska, 2012). Two years after the catastrophe, in 2013, we decided to perform a large-scale survey in order to assess how explanations of the airplane crash in Smoleńsk might have affected the cohesiveness of Polish society. Specifically, we wanted to assess whether the conspiratorial vs. non-conspiratorial interpretations of this event may enhance social distancing between conspiracy believers on the one hand and conspiracy non-believers on the other. We also sought to investigate the psychological antecedent of the relationship between conspiracy beliefs and intergroup hostility. Insofar as endorsement of conspiracy beliefs is associated with beliefs about the unique- ness of in-group suffering (Witkowska, Bilewicz, Pantazi, Gkinopoulos, & Klein, 2018), we predicted that these beliefs would foster acceptance of conspiracy explanations of current events, which, in turn, would lead to the derogation of in-group members who do not share conspiracy convictions. We tested these assumptions on a nation-wide representative sample of Polish citizens measuring the belief in the Smolensk Conspiracy. Method Participants and Procedure Data were collected as part of a 2013 nation-wide representative Polish survey (N = 965). 48.4% of the re- spondents were men and 51.6% were women (with an age range from 18 to 88, Mage = 46.05, SD = 17.52). In order to obtain an appropriate sample, participants were drawn from the population of Polish adults registered in PESEL – the Common Electronic Population Evidence System. They participated in face-to-face computer- assisted interviews, conducted by a leading Polish public opinion research institution (the Public Opinion Re- search Center, CBOS). Not all participants responded to all questions and the pattern of missing values did not seem to be related with any of the measured variables. In correlation and t-test analyses, the pairwise missing data deletion has been used. The SEM model we ran normally assumes the Full Information Maximum Likeli- hood technique of treating missing variables. However, due to missing data in the predictor variables it could not be implemented, therefore 65 cases have been deleted. Design and Measures Unless otherwise indicated, participants reported their responses on 5-point scales (from 1 = “I strongly disa- gree” to 5 = “I strongly agree”). Belief in unique in-group victimhood was assessed with the question: “Do you agree that no other nation suffered as much as Poles did.” In order to measure group-related conspiracy beliefs, we constructed a scale of Belief in the Smolensk con- spiracy (α = .74). The scale consisted of four items that were based on the current public discourse about the Smolensk catastrophe, e.g. “Polish and Russian authorities jointly conceal the truth about the catastrophe,” and “The Smolensk catastrophe was just an accident, the result of a combination of many independent factors” (re- versed). Social Distance towards conspiracy non-believers (α = .86) concerned attitudes towards people who did not endorse the conspiratorial explanation of the Smolensk catastrophe. This construct was captured by three Bilewicz, Witkowska, Pantazi et al. 85 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 https://www.psychopen.eu/ items on a social distance scale (Bogardus, 1925). Participants had to indicate on a scale (from 1 = “I would highly accept” to 4 = “I would highly oppose”) how they would react if a person who thinks that Lech Kaczyński [the Polish President] died due to an accident moved in next door, if a close relative of theirs married such a person, or if such a person was hired by the same company as the one in which the participant works. The scores were reversed, so that higher scores on the scale meant greater social distance towards the target group. Social Distance towards conspiracy believers (α = .89) concerned attitudes towards people who believe that a conspiracy was the cause of the Smolensk crash. It was captured by adapting the items of the measurement of distance towards conspiracy non-believers. Results Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of all the variables and their correlations. The average belief in the Smolensk conspiracy was lower than the scale’s midpoint, t(894)= -11.93, p < .001. The same applied to the belief about uniqueness of in-group suffering, t(913) = -5.35, p < .001. With respect to social distance, partici- pants’ average answers expressed general closeness to, rather than distance from, both target groups. Social distance towards conspiracy non-believers was positively related to the belief in the uniqueness of in- group victimhood and to the endorsement of the Smolensk Conspiracy, while only the belief in the Smolensk Conspiracy correlated with social distance towards conspiracy believers, as Table 1 shows. Table 1 Correlations and Descriptive Statistics Variable 1 2 3 4 1. Unique VC (1-5) 2.76 (1.36) 2. Smolensk Consp (1-5) .09* 2.56 (1.11) 3. Dist Non-Believers (1-4) .15*** .12** 1.48 (0.59) 4. Dist Believers (1-4) .06+ -.15*** .64*** 1.57 (0.69) Note. N = 965. Means and standard deviations (parenthesized) reported in the diagonal. The other values represent Pearson correlation coefficients. The parentheses next to the scales’ name represent the scale’s range. Unique VC = Belief in unique in-group victimhood; Smolensk Consp = Belief in Smolensk conspiracy; Dist Non-Believers = Social Distance towards conspiracy non-believers; Dist Believers = Social distance towards conspiracy believers. †p < .06. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. In order to deepen our understanding of differences between individuals endorsing and not endorsing the Smo- leńsk conspiracy theory, we dichotomized the Smolensk conspiracy variable into people below and above the median. This technique allowed us to directly compare groups of conspiracy believers and non-believers in terms of their desired social distance and convictions about in-group victimhood. A series of t-test revealed sig- nificant differences between conspiracy believers and conspiracy non-believers (see Table 2). Individuals en- dorsing Smoleńsk conspiracy, proved more prone to claim uniqueness of in-group victimhood and to distance themselves from conspiracy non-believers, compared to those not endorsing this belief, but express less dis- tance towards conspiracy believers. The longest social distance was observed among conspiracy non-believers toward conspiracy believers, followed by the distance of conspiracy believers toward conspiracy non-believers. Conspiracy Beliefs and Societal Trauma 86 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Table 2 Mean Differences Between Individuals Endorsing and Not-Endorsing Belief in Smoleńsk Conspiracy Variable Conspiracy non-believers Conspiracy believers t df pM SD M SD Unique VC 2.63 1.38 2.89 1.33 -2.86 858 .004 Dist Non-Believers 1.39 0.48 1.58 0.66 -4.94 822.06 < .001 Dist Believers 1.64 0.72 1.52 0.64 2.64 851.62 .008 Note. Unique VC = Belief in unique in-group victimhood; Dist Non-Believers = Social Distance towards conspiracy non-believers; Dist Be- lievers = Social distance towards conspiracy believers. To integrate the analyses for the two indices of social distance we specified a structural equation model using MPlus 6 software (see: Figure 1). In this model we used the four items tapping into Smoleńsk conspiracy scale as indicators of a latent conspiracy beliefs variable. Social distance towards conspiracy believers and social distance towards conspiracy non-believers were introduced as latent variables with three indicators each. Belief in unique in-group victimhood was treated as a manifest variable. Figure 1. Structural equation model: Indirect effects of unique in-group suffering beliefs on distance towards Smoleńsk conspiracy believers and non-believers through the endorsement of conspiracy beliefs of the air crash. Standardized regression coefficients. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. We estimated a model in which beliefs in unique in-group victimhood was regressed on latent conspiracy be- liefs and both latent social distance variables. The latent social distance constructs were also directly predicted Bilewicz, Witkowska, Pantazi et al. 87 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 https://www.psychopen.eu/ by latent conspiracy beliefs. The model included covariance between the two latent social distance variables. The model also reflected patterns of question format as we allowed for covariance between series of indicators variables (i.e. in the Smoleńsk conspiracy we allowed for a correlation between two positively formulated items and between two negatively formulated items; in the social distance scales, items capturing the same level of social distance like neighborhood or common working place were correlated). The model exhibited acceptable fit, χ2(34) = 121.52, p < .001, CFI = .98, RMSEA = .05, SRMS = .042. Endorsement of unique in-group victim- hood was associated with the latent conspiracy belief construct which in turn predicted the two social distance latent variables. The direct link between unique victimhood belief and both latent constructs of social distance was also significant. We then tested for indirect effects of belief in unique victimhood on latent social distance variables. We found a relatively weak but significant indirect effect of IE = 0.003 for distance towards conspira- cy nonbelievers, z = 2.47, p = .01, 95% CIs [0.00, 0.005]. The indirect effect on distance towards conspiracy believers was not significant, estimate = -0.003, z = -1.81, p = .07, 95% BCIs [-0.01, 0.001] (see Figure 1). The test of the causal structure between in-group victimhood (unique in-group suffering beliefs), conspiracy be- liefs about Smoleńsk air crash and social distance towards people endorsing and not endorsing conspiracy be- liefs, suggests that the general sense of victimhood affects distancing from the conspiracy non-believers through higher endorsement of conspiracy theories among those who consider their in-group suffering to be unique. At the same time the conspiracy theories of societal trauma lead to greater societal distancing between believers and non-believers. Discussion The present survey study performed in the aftermath of the Smoleńsk catastrophe suggests that conspiratorial explanations of this collectively traumatic incident undermined social cohesion: People endorsing conspiratorial accounts of the Smoleńsk catastrophe expressed the desire to distance themselves from conspiracy non-be- lievers, whereas people opposing conspiracy explanations preferred greater distance to conspiracy believers. The analyses of social distance between conspiracy believers and conspiracy non-believers suggest that those who do not endorse conspiratorial explanations are more willing to distance themselves from their opponents, compared to the distance that conspiracy believers desire. In other words, the results of belief vs non-belief in conspiracy theories may have disproportional effects on social distancing from out-group (skeptics vs. conspira- cists) members, potentially because skeptics consider conspiracy thinking contagious and more dangerous, than conspiracists consider skepticism. This interpretation is supported by a recent study showing that con- spiracy beliefs may be socially stigmatized (Lantian et al., 2018). Thus, there seem to be alternative reasons for non-believers to distance from the proponents of conspiracy theories, one of them could be the fear of contami- nation (Haidt, Rozin, McCauley, & Imada, 1997) supported by the stigmatizing nature of conspiracy belief. As expected, higher belief in unique in-group suffering was found to account for conspiracy thinking and subse- quent hostility towards the outgroup, as defined by belief in conspiracies or not. This result is consistent with previous research showing that beliefs concerning the uniqueness of in-group trauma are related to greater suspiciousness in intergroup relations (Bar-Tal, Chernyak-Hai, Schori, & Gundar, 2009). Therefore, historical charters that position the group in the position of the victim create an interpretative framework for subsequent socially shared traumatic events. People who believe in the unique victimization of their nation are more likely to endorse conspiracy beliefs and this is likely to enhance their social distance to non-believers. To sum up, Conspiracy Beliefs and Societal Trauma 88 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 https://www.psychopen.eu/ three broader statements underpin our findings: Firstly, views about national history affect contemporary re- sponses to traumatic events. Second, people focused on historical victimhood interpret current traumas in a conspirational way and distance themselves from skeptics. Third, people skeptical about conspiracy explana- tions of trauma distance themselves from conspiracy believers even more strongly than those who do endorse such explanations distance themselves from skeptics. The phenomenon of social distance (Bogardus, 1925) is often treated as an indication of prejudice and discrimi- natory intention. However, it might also indicate a more general tendency to isolate oneself from other people, regardless of their views and group memberships. The positive relationship between social distance toward conspiracy believers and non-believers at least partly suggests such a possibility. In order to overcome this is- sue, we decided to treat the errors of social distance scales as correlated, which allowed us to directly assess the effects of conspiracy endorsement on distance toward believers vs. non-believers (as opposed to a general tendency to isolate oneself). There are some inherent limitations to the present investigation. Fist, the effect sizes we obtained for all the observed effects suggest that there is still a vast amount of investigation needed in order to fully understand the interplay of conspiracy theories and social cohesion. The correlational effects are relatively low, as is the varia- tion of the key dependent variable. This might be due to the fact that traditional social distance measures are imperfect measures of distancing intentions in politicized conflicts (as opposed to ethnic or religious ones). In such conflicts, it might be unwillingness to accept someone as a boss, priest or a representative that is more representative of distance, rather than unwillingness to accept such person as a neighbor. Second, due to the correlational nature of the data collected, these results are obviously restricted and should be treated with caution as for the causal structure of the phenomenon. Given the inability of mediation analysis to identify unique mediators (Fiedler, Schott, & Meiser, 2011), our analysis cannot be seen as statistically prov- ing the accuracy of our mediation hypothesis. It is worth highlighting, however that the proposed casual path, whereby beliefs about the uniqueness of in-group suffering facilitate the endorsement of conspiracy explana- tions of current events, is based on extended theoretical elaboration and also supported by other experimental research (Witkowska et al., 2018). Further research is needed to address this controversy by means of experi- mental tests of mediation analysis, or a longitudinal design (Bullock, Green, & Ha, 2010). In any event, the present study has the advantage of offering an insight into a unique period of Polish history when the country seemed to be split over the explanation of a socially shared traumatic event. Overall, the findings reported in this article provide a novel explanation for the negative behavioral consequen- ces of victimhood beliefs for intragroup attitudes that occur in the situation of a large scale societal trauma. Specifically, they showed that historical group charters may induce conspiracy believers to distance themselves from the non-believers when they interpret traumatic events. This might be due to the specificity of societal traumas in political domain that seems substantially different from individual traumas or natural disasters that often bring social cohesion rather than divide. Future empirical studies, potentially based on computer simula- tions using agent-based models (Eberlen, Scholz, & Gagliolo, 2017), could further examine the dynamics of so- cietal divisions after political traumatic events. Bilewicz, Witkowska, Pantazi et al. 89 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Funding This work was supported by the Polish National Science Center (NCN) BEETHOVEN Grant DEC-2014/15/G/HS6/04589 awarded to the first author and collaboration has been facilitated by the COST Action IS1205 “Social psychological dynamics of historical representations in the enlarged European Union”. Competing Interests The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. Acknowledgments The authors have no support to report. R ef er enc es Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (1999). Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20, 637-647. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00160 Bar-Tal, D., Chernyak-Hai, L., Schori, N., & Gundar, A. (2009). A sense of self-perceived collective victimhood in intractable conflicts. International Review of the Red Cross, 91, 229-258. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1816383109990221 Bilewicz, M., & Krzeminski, I. (2010). Anti-Semitism in Poland and Ukraine: The belief in Jewish control as a mechanism of scapegoating. International Journal of Conflict and Violence, 4, 234-243. Bogardus, E. S. (1925). Measuring social distance. Journal of Applied Sociology, 9, 299-308. Borger, J., & Pidd, H. (2011, April 8). Smolensk air crash – A year on and the scars are yet to heal. The Guardian. Retrieved from http://www.theguardian.com/world/2011/apr/08/poland-smolensk-air-crash-conspiracy Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 225. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225 Bullock, J. G., Green, D. P., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Yes, but what’s the mechanism? (Don’t expect an easy answer). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 550-558. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018933 Cohn, M. A., Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Linguistic markers of psychological change surrounding September 11, 2001. Psychological Science, 15, 687-693. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00741.x Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influenced by personal willingness to conspire. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 544-552. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02018.x Eberlen, J., Scholz, G., & Gagliolo, M. (2017). Simulate this! An introduction to agent-based models and their power to improve your research practice. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 30, 149-160. https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.115 Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731-742. https://doi.org/10.2307/3791630 Conspiracy Beliefs and Societal Trauma 90 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00160 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1816383109990221 http://www.theguardian.com/world/2011/apr/08/poland-smolensk-air-crash-conspiracy https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018933 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00741.x https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02018.x https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.115 https://doi.org/10.2307/3791630 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Grzesiak-Feldman, M., & Haska, A. (2012, November 22). Conspiracy theories surrounding the Polish presidential plane crash near Smolensk, Russia. Retrieved from http://deconspirator.com/2012/11/22/conspiracy-theories-surrounding-the-polish-presidential-plane-crash-near- smolensk-russia/ Fiedler, K., Freytag, P., & Unkelbach, C. (2011). Great oaks from giant acorns grow: How causal-impact judgments depend on the strength of a cause. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 162-172. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.750 Fiedler, K., Schott, M., & Meiser, T. (2011). What mediation analysis can (not) do. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 1231-1236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.007 Fritsche, I., Jonas, E., & Kessler, T. (2011). Collective reactions to threat: Implications for intergroup conflict and for solving societal crises. Social Issues and Policy Review, 5, 101-136. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01027.x Haidt, J., Rozin, P., McCauley, C., & Imada, S. (1997). Body, psyche, and culture: The relationship between disgust and morality. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9, 107-131. https://doi.org/10.1177/097133369700900105 Harding, L. (2010, April 10). Polish president Lech Kaczynski killed in plane crash. The Guardian. Retrieved from http://www.theguardian.com/world/2010/apr/10/poland-president-lech-kaczynski-killed Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014a). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 35-56. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12018 Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014b). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS One, 9, Article e89177. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177 Klein, O., Van der Linden, N., Pantazi, M., & Kissine, M. (2015). Behind the screen conspirators: Paranoid social cognition in an online age. In M. Bilewicz, A. Cichocka, & W. Soral (Eds.), The psychology of conspiracy (pp. 162-182). London, United Kingdom: Routledge. Lantian, A., Muller, D., Cécile, N., Klein, O., Berjot, S., & Pantazi, M. (2018). Stigmatized beliefs: Conspiracy theories, anticipated negative evaluation of the self, and fear of social exclusion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 939-954. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2498 Littman, R., & Paluck, E. L. (2015). The cycle of violence: Understanding individual participation in collective violence. Political Psychology, 36(S1), 79-99. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12239 Liu, J. H., & Hilton, D. J. (2005). How the past weighs on the present: Social representations of history and their role in identity politics. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 537-556. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466605X27162 Noor, M., Brown, R., & Prentice, G. (2008). Prospects for intergroup reconciliation: Social psychological predictors of intergroup forgiveness and reparation in Northern Ireland and Chile. In A. Nadler, T. E. Malloy, & J. D. Fisher (Eds.), Social psychology of intergroup reconciliation: From violent conflict to peaceful co-existence – Going beyond victimization, guilt and distrust (pp. 97-114). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. Noor, M., Shnabel, N., Halabi, S., & Nadler, A. (2012). When suffering begets suffering: The psychology of competitive victimhood between adversarial groups in violent conflicts. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 351-374. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312440048 Bilewicz, Witkowska, Pantazi et al. 91 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 http://deconspirator.com/2012/11/22/conspiracy-theories-surrounding-the-polish-presidential-plane-crash-near-smolensk-russia/ http://deconspirator.com/2012/11/22/conspiracy-theories-surrounding-the-polish-presidential-plane-crash-near-smolensk-russia/ https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.750 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.007 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01027.x https://doi.org/10.1177/097133369700900105 http://www.theguardian.com/world/2010/apr/10/poland-president-lech-kaczynski-killed https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12018 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177 https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2498 https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12239 https://doi.org/10.1348/014466605X27162 https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312440048 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Schori-Eyal, N., Klar, Y., & Ben-Ami, Y. (2017). Perpetual in-group victimhood as a distorted lens: Effects on attribution and categorization. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 180-194. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2250 Schori-Eyal, N., Klar, Y., Roccas, S., & McNeill, A. (2017). The shadows of the past: Effects of historical group trauma on current intergroup conflicts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43, 538-554. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216689063 Skitka, L. J. (2005). Patriotism or nationalism? Understanding post-September 11, 2001, flag-display behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 1995-2011. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02206.x Staub, E., & Vollhardt, J. (2008). Altruism born of suffering: The roots of caring and helping after victimization and other trauma. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 78, 267-280. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014223 Suedfeld, P. (1997). Reactions to societal trauma: Distress and/or eustress. Political Psychology, 18, 849-861. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00082 Swami, V. (2012). Social psychological origins of conspiracy theories: The case of the Jewish conspiracy theory in Malaysia. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, Article 280. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00280 van Prooijen, J.-W., & Jostmann, N. B. (2013). Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 109-115. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1922 Vollhardt, J. R. (2009). Altruism born of suffering and prosocial behavior following adverse life events: A review and conceptualization. Social Justice Research, 22, 53-97. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-009-0088-1 Vollhardt, J. R. (2015). Inclusive victim consciousness in advocacy, social movements, and intergroup relations: Promises and pitfalls. Social Issues and Policy Review, 9, 89-120. https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12011 Vollhardt, J. R., & Bilali, R. (2015). The role of inclusive and exclusive victim consciousness in predicting intergroup attitudes: Findings from Rwanda, Burundi, and DRC. Political Psychology, 36, 489-506. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12174 Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322, 115-117. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1159845 Witkowska, M., Bilewicz, M., Pantazi, M., Gkinopoulos, T., & Klein, O. (2018). “Historia est Magistra Vitae”? The impact of historical victimhood on current conspiracy beliefs. Unpublished manuscript. A bout the Au thor s Michał Bilewicz is an associate professor of psychology at the University of Warsaw. His research is focused on the social psychology of intergroup relations, including collective moral emotions, derogatory language and reconciliation processes. Marta Witkowska is a PhD candidate at the Center for Research on Prejudice at the University of Warsaw. Her main scien- tific interests are focused on motivated biases in history perception and reconciliation between groups with a violent past. Myrto Pantazi is a postdoctoral researcher at the Oxford Internet Institute. Her research focuses on the socio-cognitive de- terminants of beliefs, attitude formation and decision-making. Conspiracy Beliefs and Societal Trauma 92 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2250 https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216689063 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02206.x https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014223 https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00082 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00280 https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1922 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-009-0088-1 https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12011 https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12174 https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1159845 https://www.psychopen.eu/ Theofilos Gkinopoulos is a teaching assistant in School of Psychology at the University of Surrey and Research Associate in the Department of Higher Education at the University of Surrey. His research is focused on social identity and intergroup relations, leadership, and perception of time in victimhood experiences. Olivier Klein is a professor of social psychology at the Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium. His research is focused on intergroup relations, collective memory and gender. He is co-editor-in-chief of the International Review of Social Psychology. Bilewicz, Witkowska, Pantazi et al. 93 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2019, Vol. 15(1), 82–93 https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1699 PsychOpen GOLD is a publishing service by Leibniz Institute for Psychology Information (ZPID), Trier, Germany. www.leibniz-psychology.org https://www.leibniz-psychology.org/ https://www.psychopen.eu/ Conspiracy Beliefs and Societal Trauma (Introduction) Conspiracy Theories and Their Consequences Exclusive Victimhood as an Interpretative Framework Current Study: The Smoleńsk Plane Crash as a Societal Trauma Method Participants and Procedure Design and Measures Results Discussion (Additional Information) Funding Competing Interests Acknowledgments References About the Authors