Parenting Styles and Child’s Well-Being: The Mediating Role of the Perceived Parental Stress


Research Reports

Parenting Styles and Child’s Well-Being: The Mediating Role of the
Perceived Parental Stress

Elisa Delvecchio* a, Alessandro Germani a, Veronica Raspa a, Adriana Lis b,
Claudia Mazzeschi a

[a] Department of Philosophy, Social Sciences and Education, University of Perugia, Perugia, Italy. [b] Department of Developmental
Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, Padova, Italy.

Abstract
In the last decades, consensus from laymen, scholars, and policy-makers has emphasized the role of child-parent relationships to promote
child’s development and positive well-being. Parenting style was claimed as one of the crucial factors for the child’s positive adjustment.
The main aim of the present study was to investigate the relationships between authoritative and authoritarian parenting styles and child’s
difficulties. The mediational role of parent’s perception of a difficult child on the above mentioned relation was taken into account. The study
was carried out on a sample of 459 couples including mothers (n = 459) and fathers (n = 459) of children aged 2 to 10 years old who filled
in the Parenting Styles & Dimensions Questionnaire short version, the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, and the Parenting Stress
Index-short form. Main findings indicated that authoritative style was associated with less child’s maladjustment, while the authoritarian one
showed the opposite association. These relationships were partially mediated by the perception of a difficult child, which partially explained
the link between parenting style and child’s problems. Above and beyond the role of parent’s perception as a difficult child, parenting styles
had an important effect on child’s difficulties. Future studies should replicate these results with other samples, use the spouse version of the
parenting styles, control the effect of socio-economic status and other variables related to family functioning, as well as to consider the
child’s perception regarding parents’ parenting style.

Keywords: authoritarian parenting style, authoritative parenting style, parenting styles, parenting stress, well-being

Europe's Journal of Psychology, 2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531, https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

Received: 2019-06-02. Accepted: 2020-01-03. Published (VoR): 2020-08-31.

Handling Editor: Iris Zezelj, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia

*Corresponding author at: Department of Philosophy, Social Sciences and Education, University of Perugia, Piazza Ermini 1, Perugia, Italy. Phone:
+390755854943, Fax: +390755854918, E-mail: elisa.delvecchio@unipg.it

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, CC BY 4.0
(https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited.

Parenting is a complex construct that involves several and multifaceted variables (Bornstein, 2002). It was
defined as a normal variation in the parents' attempts of controlling their children and making them socialize
(Baumrind, 1967). Parenting includes many specific attitudes which work individually and cumulatively to
influence the child's behavior. Schaefer (1959, 1965) proposed a circumplex parenting model using three
dimensions of acceptance versus rejection, psychological autonomy versus psychological control, and firm be-
havioral control versus lax behavioral control. On these dimensions Baumrind (Baumrind 1966, 1968; Baumrind
& Thompson, 2002) proposed three parental prototypes: authoritative, authoritarian and permissive, to describe
models of parental control and child socialization. For the purposes of the current paper, authoritative and
authoritarian styles were taken into account.

Europe's Journal of Psychology
ejop.psychopen.eu | 1841-0413

https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
https://ejop.psychopen.eu/
https://ejop.psychopen.eu/
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Authoritative parents tend to show high acceptance and behavioral control, low psychological control, high
responsiveness and warmth to their children. They are conceptualized as affectionate, encouraging, and con-
trolling in a way that promotes child autonomy (Baumrind, 1966, 2013a; Baumrind, Larzelere, & Owens, 2010).
They direct the child in a rational situation-based way in which both autonomy and conformity are important,
they clearly set rules and use reasoning to enforce them for encouraging open communication, supporting
children’s independence, and expressing love and affection (Maccoby & Martin, 1983). Authoritative style was
related to positive child outcomes such as self-reliance (Baumrind, 1968), social responsibility (Baumrind,
1971), and adjustment (Baumrind et al., 2010; Braza et al., 2015; García & Gracia, 2009; Luyckx et al., 2011).
A large number of studies indicated that children of authoritative parents are shown as being cooperative,
friendly, emotionally stable, and with good academic performance (García & Gracia, 2009; Maccoby, 1992;
Steinberg, Elmen, & Mounts, 1989).

Authoritarian parents combine high control with low levels of warmth involvement, support, and emotional
commitment to their child; they are rejecting, highly demanding, strongly commanding, psychologically and
domineeringly controlling (Baumrind, 2013b; Baumrind et al., 2010). They are often punitive and forceful in
order to adhere to an absolute standard of behavior (Baumrind, 2012), leaving very little space to the child’s
decision making or rationales (Baumrind, 1991). Parents believe that the child should do what they say, as a
consequence child’s behaviors and attitudes are guided, formed and based on a certain standard of conduct.
This parenting style was related to less optimal child outcomes, including lower self-efficacy (Baumrind et al.,
2010), more externalizing and internalizing problems (Braza et al., 2015; Maccoby & Martin, 1983; Rinaldi &
Howe, 2012), and rebellion (Baumrind, 1968).

Authoritative parenting style was defined as the optimal parenting style (e.g., Baumrind, 1966, 2013a; García
& Gracia, 2009; Luyckx et al., 2011; Maccoby & Martin, 1983) whereas the authoritarian style was proved to
be the most negative form of parenting (Baumrind & Black, 1967; Luyckx et al., 2011; Olivari, Tagliabue, &
Confalonieri, 2013). Research in the last decades supported Baumrind’s prediction about authoritative parents
(Bornstein & Lamb, 2015; Larzelere, Morris, & Harrist, 2013) and the adverse effects of authoritarian parenting.

In order to assess parenting styles, researchers mainly used questionnaires (e.g., Steinberg, Mounts, Lamborn,
& Dornbusch, 1991). One of the most known tools is the Parenting Practices Questionnaire (PPQ; Robinson,
Mandleco, Olsen, & Hart, 1995). This 62-item self-report measure aimed at identifying continuous scales of
authoritative, authoritarian and permissive parenting styles (Robinson et al., 1995), although greater importance
was given to authoritative and authoritarian styles. Several variations of this measure have been developed
in recent years (e.g., Coolahan, McWayne, Fantuzzo, & Grim, 2002; Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen, & Hart,
2001; Robinson et al., 1995; Russell, Londhe, & Britner, 2013; Wu et al., 2002). Among them, the most
used version is the Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire - short form (PSDQ; Robinson et al.,
2001), an abbreviated self-report measure of the PPQ in which each parent independently reports his/her own
parenting style and his/her spouse’s parenting style. The PSDQ measures continuous scales of authoritative,
authoritarian, and permissive parenting styles, although only some of the original items and dimensions from
the PPQ were retained.

PSDQ was widely used in large samples (Padilla-Walker & Coyne, 2011; Topham et al., 2011; Williams et al.,
2009) and in different cultures (Kern & Jonyniene, 2012; Önder & Gülay, 2009; Porter at al., 2005). PSDQ psy-
chometric characteristics were reviewed by Olivari et al. (2013). They showed that PSDQ was most frequently

Delvecchio, Germani, Raspa et al. 515

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://www.psychopen.eu/


used (83.02%) among school-age or pre-school children (Olivari et al., 2013). Little attention was given to the
comparison of PSDQ among different ages. A need for further studies was claimed after having found mixed
results in the conduction of three studies which included parents of children from nursery to primary school
(Olivari et al., 2013). Moreover, none of them were carried out in Italy. Notwithstanding the increasing interest
on parenting styles (Olivari et al., 2013), there are still many issues that need to be addressed.

The present study had two main goals. The first aim was to contribute to the investigation of the authoritative
and authoritarian parenting styles by using the PSDQ in a large sample of Italian parents of children aged 2-11
years old. The second purpose was to focus and expand the investigation on the relationship between the
authoritative and authoritarian parenting styles and child's adaptation. The possible mediator role of parents’
perception about having a difficult child was also examined. Mother and father’s contributions were taken into
account separately.

Issues About the PSDQ

Previous research comparing parenting styles in different cultures showed that the original assumption that
authoritative style would be the “best protective” style of parenting, is not always true. For instance, in African
American families, authoritarian parenting, if associated with physical punishment, did not increase the level of
behavior problems, suggesting that the pattern of findings reported by Baumrind is culture-specific (Baumrind,
1972). A large body of evidence supports the importance of extending investigation on parenting and its
related contribution to child development, as well as to collect data in countries that can be culturally different
concerning parenting practices (Bornstein et al., 1998; Russell, Hart, Robinson, & Olsen, 2003).

In Italy, the parent-child relationship is characterized by basic emotional bonding and support from parents.
Moreover, although parents encourage their children to be autonomous and independent (Casiglia, Lo Coco, &
Zappulla, 1998; Confalonieri, 2010; Laudani, Guzzo, Cascio, Pace, & Cacioppo, 2014; Scabini & Cigoli, 2000),
attention is also paid to the rights of children in a two-way parent-child relationship (Scabini, Marta, & Lanz,
2006; Yeh & Bedford, 2004). However, a parental change involving fathers (Bacchini, Galiani, Guerriera, &
Sbandi, 2003) was observed to occur among Italian culture in the last 20 years (Confalonieri, 2010). From
a family where the parents’ roles were differentiated, with the mother being in charge of caring for the child
whereas the father providing rules and transmitting values (Confalonieri et al., 2010), there was a shift toward a
family where both mother and father identify their main function as to ensuring care to their children. However,
just one Italian study (Confalonieri, Giuliani, & Tagliabue, 2009) was run on parenting styles of parents with
children aged 3 to 10.

Attempts to compare mothers’ and fathers’ approach to parenting styles yielded inconclusive findings.
Baumrind (1991, 2013a) focused on both parents rather than on single mothers or fathers, but she didn’t
explore why parents adopted these strategies. Studying parents of Australian and US pre-schoolers, Russell
and colleagues (2003) found that mothers were more likely to identify with the authoritative style of parenting,
whereas fathers were more likely to describe themselves as either authoritarian or permissive. Based on these
results, we expected the highest average score for authoritative style in mothers and on the contrary the
highest average score for authoritarian style for fathers (Hypothesis 2). However, because of the shift occurred
in Italy where both parents identify their main function as to ensuring warm care to their child, both mothers and
fathers were expected to report higher scores in authoritative than authoritarian style (Hypothesis 1).

Parenting Style in Mothers and Fathers 516

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Another limitation in the existing literature is the investigation of possible differences in parenting styles related
to the child’s gender. Few papers were devoted to this issue. Russell et al. (2003) found that more authoritative
parenting was reported by parents of girls rather than by parents of boys, while more authoritarian parenting
was found in parents of boys rather than of girls. The same results were hypothesized for the present study
(Hypothesis 3). Finally, although many papers were focused on children of different ages, few studies com-
pared possible variation of parental styles with the child’s developmental changes with regard to age. Bornstein
and Lamb (2015) reviewed and stressed the importance of different roles played by parents in their involvement
with children in pre-school and school years, respectively taking into account the different goals of development
the child needs to reach in these two important age groups. Thus, the comparison of parenting styles in these
two periods needs more investigation and no specific hypothesis was given (Hypothesis 4).

PSDQ and Related Variables

A considerable research body examined how variations in parental styles are related to a wide range of child
outcomes (Baumrind et al., 2010; Braza et al., 2015; García & Gracia, 2009; Luyckx et al., 2011). Relation-
ship between parenting styles and child’s behavioral problems were examined. For example, as said before,
authoritative style was related to psychological adjustment (Baumrind et al., 2010), whereas the authoritarian
one was positively related to externalizing problems (Braza et al., 2015; Maccoby & Martin, 1983; Rinaldi &
Howe, 2012). Researchers described the relationship between parenting styles and some negative outcomes
concerning academic adjustment, success, moral development, emotion regulation and social competence
(Davidov & Grusec, 2006; Denham et al., 2000; Laible, 2004; Spera, 2005). However, Darling and Steinberg
(1993), Bornstein and Lamb (2015), and Russell et al. (2003) pointed out that the influence of parenting
style on behavioral outcomes across ethnic groups and cultures was not adequately studied, despite research
findings had suggested that differences exist.

Previous research reported greater parenting stress regarding parents with an authoritarian parenting style
(Deater-Deckard & Scarr, 1996; McBride & Mills, 1993), whereas other studies showed no differences in
parenting distress among authoritarian and authoritative styles (Woolfson & Grant, 2006). Parenting stress
is a universal experience for parents in all sociodemographic groups and contexts (Crnic & Low, 2002).
Parents who experience moderate amounts of parenting stress may engage in lesser optimal parenting (Bonds,
Gondoli, Sturge-Apple, & Salem, 2002; Seginer, Vermulst, & Gerris, 2002) and the children of stressed parents
may be adversely affected, though indirectly, through parenting behaviors (Magill-Evans & Harrison, 2001;
Putnick et al., 2008, 2010; Seginer et al., 2002). Within parenting stress, Abidin (1995) and Deater‐Deckard
(1998) identified a specific dimension: stress that can be attributed to the parent’s perception of the child as
being difficult. More specifically, it deals with the child’s behavioral characteristics that make him or her easy or
difficult to manage, due to either temperament and/or noncompliant, defiant, or demanding behavior. However,
no previous studies on the link between parenting styles and parent’s perception of a difficult child were found.

Based on the above-mentioned findings, we hypothesized that the authoritative style was negatively related
to behavioral problems in children, whereas the authoritarian one was positively related to them (Hypothesis
5). Moreover, in this study, we explored how a perception of the child as being difficult would mediate the
relationship between authoritative and authoritarian parenting style and the child’s behavior difficulties. Since a
negative relation between authoritative style and the perception of a difficult child, as well as a positive relation
between the authoritarian style and the perception of a difficult child were expected (Hypothesis 6), negative

Delvecchio, Germani, Raspa et al. 517

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://www.psychopen.eu/


and positive indirect effects of parenting styles on child’s behavioral difficulties were hypothesized respectively
(Hypothesis 7).

Thus, to summarize, the current study is aimed to investigate in mothers and fathers respectively (a) levels of
authoritative versus authoritarian parenting style and the role of parent’s gender; (b) the role of child’s gender
and age on parenting styles and (c) the relation between parenting styles and child’s psychological well-being
mediated by parents’ perception of having a difficult child.

Method

Participants

Participants were 459 Caucasian parent couples (n = 459 fathers, Mage = 37.13, SD = 5.39; and n = 459
mothers, Mage = 35.08, SD = 4.95) living in urban and suburban areas of Central Italy. They were married
or living together heterosexual parents of children from infancy to childhood (2-11 years old). Considering
the large life-span included, parents were assessed considering child’s developmental stage: (a) pre-school
children (from 2 to 5 years old) and (b) school aged children (from 6 to 11 years old). 239 (n = 129 boys
(54.0%), Mage = 3.21, SD = 1.39) were parents of pre-school age children; 220 (n = 108 boys (49.1%); Mage
= 7.91, SD = 1.33) were parents of school age children. More than half (58.5%) were parents at their first
parenthood experience. Parents’ socio-economic level measured by SES (Hollingshead, 1975), was primarily
middle to upper for 90% of families. Only 7% had a low socio-economic status and only 3% reported a very
high level.

Procedure

All procedures were conducted in compliance with the ethical standards for research outlined in the 1964
Helsinki declaration. Both parents gave written informed consent to participate in the study. Confidentiality
was assured in all phases of the study using a numerical code instead of participants’ names. Families were
recruited through day-cares, nurseries and schools. Parents who participated in the study were given at their
children’s school - with the teachers’ help - a booklet containing the tools that they needed to fill in at home
and bring back to school. No incentives were awarded and voluntary participation was encouraged. Families of
the final sample met the following criteria: (a) both mothers and fathers agreed to participate, (b) all participants
completed the entire assessment phase (c) parents and children did not meet criteria for psychiatric diagnosis
and were not under psychological treatment. 5% of the initial participants did not meet all the criteria.

Measures
Parenting Styles & Dimensions Questionnaire- Short Version (PSDQ)
The PSDQ-short version (Robinson et al., 2001) is a 32 self-report questionnaire measuring continuous dimen-
sions of parenting styles using authoritative (15 items), authoritarian (12 items) and permissive (5 items) scales
(Russell et al., 2013). It comprehends a self and spouse version (i.e., self-evaluation of her/his-own parenting
style and evaluation of his/her spouse’s parenting style). With regard to the current study, the authoritarian and
authoritative scales of the self-version were considered. Mothers and fathers independently rated themselves
by assessing how often they exhibited parenting behaviors as described in each item by using a five-point

Parenting Style in Mothers and Fathers 518

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://www.psychopen.eu/


scale from “never” to “always” (coded 1 to 5). Higher mean scores indicated a prevalence on that style.
Confalonieri, Giuliani, and Tagliabue (2009) adapted the measure with Italian parents. The alpha value related
to the authoritarian scale was .70 (.65-.74) for fathers, and .72 (.69-.76) for mothers, whereas referring to the
authoritative scale it was .78 (.75-.81) for fathers and .75 (.72-.78) for mothers.

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)
The SDQ (Goodman, 1999, 2001) is a brief questionnaire of 25 attributes, some positive and some negative,
for assessing strengths and difficulties in the psychological adjustment of children and adolescents. Each item
uses a 3-point ordinal Likert scale (0 “not true”; 1 “somewhat true”; 2 “certainly true”). In the present paper,
the Total Difficulties Score (TDS; 20 items, range 0-40) was used. Higher scores indicated more difficultiesi.
Cronbach’s coefficients for SDQ TDS were .79 (.76-.81) for fathers and .78 (.74-.80) for mothers.

The Difficult Child Subscale (PSI_DC) of the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form (PSI-SF)
The PSI-SF (Abidin, 1995) is a 36-item measure of overall levels of stress experienced by parents. The current
study is focused on the Difficult Child Scale which includes 12 items scored on a 5-point Likert scale from 1
(Strongly agree) to 5 (Strongly disagree). It measures the parent’s perception of his/her child’s self-regulatory
abilities. The higher the score is, the higher the perceived difficulties will be. The measure was validated in sev-
eral countries, among which Italy (Guarino, Di Blasio, D’Alessio, Camisasca, & Serantoni, 2008). Cronbach’s
alpha coefficients for the present study were .88 (.86-.89) for fathers and .86 (.84-.88) for mothers.

Data Analysis

Firstly, in order to test Hypotheses 1-4, descriptive statistics and a MANOVA were performed with parental role,
child’s gender, and age-group as factors, and the authoritative and authoritarian parenting styles as dependent
variables. Effect size was measured by using partial eta-squared, in which small, medium, and large effects
were .0099, .0588, and .1379, respectively (Cohen, 1988, p. 283). Second, before testing the mediational
model, Pearson’s correlations were run to explore the associations between the selected variables. Effect
sizes were interpreted according to Cohen (1992), with correlation coefficient of .10, .30, and .50 representing
low, medium and high effect sizes, respectively. Third, the mediational models split for mothers and fathers
were carried out. Specifically, in order to test the total effects of parenting styles on children’s maladjustment
(Hypothesis 5), simple regression analyses were used. Then, to test their effects on difficult child variable (i.e.,
the mediator; Hypothesis 6), simple regressions were analyzed. Finally, in order to assess direct and indirect
effects of parental styles on child’s behavior problems mediated by the perception of a difficult child, SPSS
Process Macro (Model 4) developed by Preacher and Hayes (2008) was used (Hypothesis 7). This allowed
the authors to observe the change of the total effects and the influence of the mediator (i.e., the mediated
effect). Bootstrapping method (Preacher & Hayes, 2008) was applied to all the mediation models. Specifically,
5000 bootstrap samples were used from the full data and 95% confidence interval was utilized to determine
the significance of the mediating effect. Significant mediating effect was identified if the confidence interval
excluded 0. In order to calculate the ratio of the indirect effect to the total effect, proportion mediated (PM =
indirect effect/total effect) was calculated. Regression and moderation analysis were carried out separately for
mothers and fathers. Analyses were carried out using SPSS (Version 18.0) and Process Macro for SPSS.

Delvecchio, Germani, Raspa et al. 519

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Results

The Effect of Parental Role, Child’s Age, and Age-Group on Authoritative and
Authoritarian Parenting Styles

Means and standard deviations of PSDQ subscales for mothers and fathers, and according to child’s gender
and age-group are shown in Table 1.

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics of PSDQ Variables

PSDQ

Mothers Fathers Preschool School Boys Girls

n = 459 n = 459 n = 239 n = 220 n = 237 n = 222

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Authoritative 3.88 0.51 4.11 0.39 3.98 0.46 4.01 0.47 4.02 0.46 3.97 0.47
Authoritarian 2.04 0.45 2.15 0.45 2.07 0.46 2.12 0.44 2.11 0.47 2.09 0.43

Results of MANOVA indicated that there was a significant main effect of parental role (Wilks’ Lambda = .914,
F(2, 909) = 43.00, p < .001, η2 = .086) on dependent variables (parenting styles), but there were not significant
main effects of child’s gender (Wilks’ Lambda = .996, F(2, 909) = 2.04, p = .131, η2 = .004) and age-group
(Wilks’ Lambda = .994, F(2, 909) = 2.62, p = .074, η2 = .006) on parenting styles. ANOVA indicated that there
was a significant effect of parental role both for authoritative (F = 57.96, p < .001, η2 = .060) and authoritarian
(F = 2.83, p < .001, η2 = .015) styles. As shown in Table 1, fathers referred both a higher authoritative and
authoritarian style than mothers, with a medium and small effect size, respectively.

Correlations Between Selected Variables by Parental Role

Pearsons’ correlations are reported in Table 2. Authoritative and authoritarian styles were significantly and
negatively correlated to each other with small effect size, independently of parental role. Moreover, on one
hand, authoritative style was significantly and negatively correlated to the perception of a difficult child and
child’s total difficulties with small effect sizes, irrespective of parental role. On the other hand, authoritarian style
was significantly and positively associated to parents’ perception of a difficult child and child’s total difficulties,
with small effect size, both in mothers and fathers. Finally, the perception of having a difficult child and child’s
total difficulties were significantly and positively associated both in fathers and mothers, with medium and
nearly large effect size, respectively.

Parenting Style in Mothers and Fathers 520

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Table 2

Pearson’s Correlations Between the Selected Variables, Split for Parental Role

Variable 1 2 3 4

1. Authoritative style -.19* -.18* -.13*
2. Authoritarian style -.19* .29* .22*
3. Difficult child -.13* .18* .49*
4. Child’s total difficulties -.15* .22* .35*
Note. Coefficients above the diagonal referred to mothers, coefficients under the diagonal referred to fathers.
*p < .01.

Parenting Styles, Parenting Stress and Child’s Psychological Well-Being

Since results for mothers and fathers showed similar paths, they are described together, although the statistics
are illustrated separately in Figure 1 and Figure 2 respectively.

Figure 1. Regression and mediational model for mothers. Total effects and mediational models examining the direct and
indirect effects of authoritative (A) and authoritarian (B) parenting styles of mothers on child’s TDS, mediated by the
perception of having a difficult child.

Note. Slope coefficients are standardized; numbers in parenthesis represent significant indirect effects mediated by Difficult
Child (5.000 bootstrap samples, 95% confidence interval).

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Delvecchio, Germani, Raspa et al. 521

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Referring to authoritative style (Figure 1A and 2A): i. Total effect. The increase in authoritative style significantly
predicted a decrease in TDS; ii. Variation in mediator. The rise in authoritative style predicted a decrease in
the score of the perception of a difficult child; iii. Mediation model. The increase in the difficult child variable
predicted a rise in the child’s TDS. Moreover, authoritative style showed both negative direct (except for
mothers) and indirect effect on the child’s total difficulties as mediated through the perception of a difficult child
variable. More specifically, the proportion mediated by the difficult child variable was .65 (65%) for mothers
and .31 (31%) for fathers.

Regarding the authoritarian style (Figure 1B and 2B): i. Total effect. There was a positive relation between
authoritarian style and TDS score; ii. Variation in mediator. Moreover, the rise in authoritarian style predicted
an increase of the difficult child variable; iii. Mediation model. The perception of a difficult child had a positive
relation with the child’s TDS. Moreover, the authoritarian style showed both positive direct and indirect effects

Figure 2. Regression and mediational model for fathers. Total effects and mediational models examining the direct and
indirect effects of authoritative (A) and authoritarian (B) parenting styles of fathers on child’s TDS, mediated by difficult child
variable.

Note. Slope coefficients are standardized; numbers in parenthesis represent significant indirect effects mediated by Difficult
Child (5.000 bootstrap samples, 95% confidence interval).

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Parenting Style in Mothers and Fathers 522

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://www.psychopen.eu/


on the child’s TDS. The proportion mediated by difficult child variable was .61 (61%) for mothers and .26 (26%)
for fathers.

Discussion

The first aim of this paper was to investigate the authoritative and authoritarian parenting styles using the
PSDQ in a large sample of Italian parents of children aged 2-11 years old. Bornstein and Lamb (2015) stressed
how greatly research enriched understanding of socialization processes with family system scholars, docu-
menting the processes by which parents and children shape one another’s behavior. The authors highlighted
the developmental processes by which authoritative parents facilitate their children’s socialization (Bornstein
& Lamb, 2015). Jacobson and Crockett (2000) and Smetana (2008) reported that parental involvement plays
an important role in the development of both social and cognitive competences in children, whereas a lack
of it is associated with the risk of delinquent behavior. Bornstein and Lamb (2015) according to these results,
suggested to consider both the children’s impact on the parents’ child rearing beliefs and the parental role in
giving care to the child. They stressed that this assertion needs to be explored empirically in a wide variety of
cultural and subcultural settings.

The interest in Italian parents originated from many different issues. First of all, the importance to extend
investigation of parenting styles in order to advance the understanding of factors shaping parenting as well
as processes that influence child development (Russell, 2003; Wu et al., 2002). Italy is a country where
parenting is characterized by intertwining emotional bonding and supportive parent-child relationship with en-
couragement for the child’s autonomy. Moreover, both mothers and fathers are called to ensure the care for the
child. The complexity of the present parenting “environment” confirmed the hypothesis that both mothers and
fathers reported higher score on authoritative than on authoritarian parenting style. However, for both styles,
fathers scored higher than mothers. Following the general agreement in literature as to the positive effect
of authoritative versus authoritarian styles, Italian parents seem to offer their children a healthy environment
characterized by warmth, affection, open communication and supportive proximity (Maccoby & Martin, 1983).
Although gender and age-group differences were expected, mothers and fathers appeared to be homogenous
in their score of authoritative and authoritarian parenting styles.

A further aim of this study was to focus and expand the investigation of the relationship regarding the authorita-
tive and authoritarian parenting styles with the child's adaptation. More specifically, to investigate the possible
relationship between these two parenting styles in mothers and fathers and their perception of the child’s
well-being. Stressful perception of a difficult child was also examined as a mediator. The significant total effects
of parenting styles on the child’s behavioral problems confirmed this well-established relation. More precisely,
the authoritative parenting style had a positive influence in diminishing the parent’s perception on the child’s
maladjustment, and the reverse for the authoritarian style. These results were regardless of parent’s gender.
In line with literature, these results confirmed that the authoritative style is the optimal parenting style while
the authoritarian style is a less desirable form of parenting (Baumrind, 1966, 2013a; Bornstein & Lamb, 2015;
Larzelere et al., 2013). The current study allowed confirming this trend also for Italian mothers and fathers.
Moreover, being the fathers more authoritative than authoritarian, just like mothers, they appeared to contribute
in their protecting role against child’s maladjustment.

Delvecchio, Germani, Raspa et al. 523

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://www.psychopen.eu/


The potential role of parents’ perception of a difficult child in mediating the aforementioned relation was
investigated. As expected, the higher the authoritative style was, the lower the parents’ perception of a difficult
child and referred problems. On the contrary, parents who reported higher score in the authoritarian style,
referred that their children were particularly difficult, and they reported higher difficulties in their children. Both
mothers and fathers showed the same pattern. In other words, both parents were significantly less stressed by
their perception of the child as difficult when their style was authoritative and the reverse when their style was
authoritarian. Moreover, the higher the parental distress related to their perception of a difficult child, the more
problems in children arose. Therefore, being authoritative has indirect protective effect on child’s behavioral
problems, because it is related to less perception of a difficult child. On the contrary, being authoritarian
increases the child’s problems, also because parents consider more difficult their children. This is true for
mothers and fathers, although, mothers were significantly more influenced by the perception of their child as
difficult. In other words, this component of parenting stress seemed to explain more variance in child’s total
difficulties in mothers than in fathers. A possible explanation may be that mothers, as primary caretakers, have
to deal more closely with everyday life and routine with the child than fathers, even if the latter in the last few
years have been more involved in family life. This makes mothers more sensitive to stress linked with child
care. Finally, it is important to highlight that the present study allowed to measure the relation between parental
styles and child’s behavioral problems taking into account and controlling for the role of a relevant aspect of
parenting stress (i.e., parents’ perception of a difficult child). In other words, the current work emphasized the
link between parenting styles and child’s behavioral problems above and beyond the parents’ evaluation of the
child as difficult.

This study has some limitations. Firstly, this study used a specific measure of parenting with respect to other
versions quoted in literature. Moreover, although in the current study the self-report version of the PSDQ was
administered to mother and father of each child, the spouse version of the PSDQ was not considered. Since
the self-report and the spouse version could offer different results, future studies are encouraged to use both
versions to delve into these issues. Furthermore, the categories selected (i.e., pre-school versus school age
children) may be too ample to make the differences of parenting styles linked to child’s age emerge. Greater
and more detailed samples need to be taken into consideration in order to make more generalizable and
conclusive results. Hoff, Laursen, and Tardif (2002) described the strong association between parenting and
socioeconomic status. Although the current paper assessed parents’ SES, it did not consider such variable
for data analysis. Neither the role of parental burden was taken into account, although it may play a role on
the selected variables (e.g., Oyserman, Bybee, Mowbray, & Kahng, 2004). Future studies should deepen into
those topics, due to their meaningful potential role in explaining such complex relationships. Furthermore, the
current study used only parents’ perceptions. Further studies could be carried out to analyze child’s perception
regarding parents’ parenting style (according to child’s developmental stage).

Notwithstanding, the present study contributes to the cross-cultural literature on parenting styles in pre-school
and school children, as well as to the association with the child’s difficulties. The results of the mediational
models highlight that stress connected to the perception of the child as difficult, is one of the mechanisms
explaining how parenting styles are related to psychological difficulties in Italian pre-school and school age
children.

Notes
i) The Italian version was retrieved from https://sdqinfo.org.

Parenting Style in Mothers and Fathers 524

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://sdqinfo.org
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Funding
The authors have no funding to report.

Competing Interests
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Acknowledgments
The authors have no support to report.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the insti-
tutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable
ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Re fer en ces

Abidin, R. R. (1995). The parenting stress index professional manual. Odessa, FL, USA: Psychological Assessment
Resources.

Bacchini, D., Galiani, R., Guerriera, C., & Sbandi, M. (2003). Adolescenti che raccontano e si raccontano: un’indagine sulla
rappresentazione delle relazioni con i genitori. In M. Sbandi (Ed.), La narrazione come ricerca del significato (pp. 43-70).
Naples, Italy: Liguori Editore.

Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child Development, 37(4), 887-907.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1126611

Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology
Monographs, 75(1), 43-88.

Baumrind, D. (1968). Authoritarian vs. authoritative parental control. Adolescence, 3(11), 255-272.

Baumrind, D. (1971). Harmonious parents and their preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 4(1, Pt.1), 99-102.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030373

Baumrind, D. (1972). An exploratory study of socialization effects of Black children: Some Black-White comparisons. Child
Development, 43, 261-267. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127891

Baumrind, D. (1991). Parenting styles and adolescent development. In J. Brooks-Gunn, R. M. Lerner, & A. C. Petersen
(Eds.), The encyclopedia on adolescence (pp. 746-758). New York, NY, USA: Garland Publishing.

Baumrind, D. (2012). Differentiating between confrontive and coercive kinds of parental power-assertive disciplinary
practices. Human Development, 55(2), 35-51. https://doi.org/10.1159/000337962

Baumrind, D. (2013a). Authoritative parenting revisited: History and current status. In R. E. Larzelere, A. S. Morris, & A. W.
Harrist (Eds.), Authoritative parenting: Synthesizing nurturance and discipline for optimal child development (pp. 11-34).
Washington, DC, USA: American Psychological Association.

Delvecchio, Germani, Raspa et al. 525

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://doi.org/10.2307%2F1126611
https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fh0030373
https://doi.org/10.2307%2F1127891
https://doi.org/10.1159%2F000337962
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Baumrind, D. (2013b). Is a pejorative view of power assertion in the socialization process justified? Review of General
Psychology, 17(4), 420-427. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033480

Baumrind, D., & Black, A. E. (1967). Socialization practices associated with dimensions of competence in preschool boys
and girls. Child Development, 38(2), 291-327. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127295

Baumrind, D., Larzelere, R. E., & Owens, E. B. (2010). Effects of preschool parents’ power assertive patterns and practices
on adolescent development. Parenting, Science and Practice, 10(3), 157-201.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15295190903290790

Baumrind, D., & Thompson, R. A. (2002). The ethics of parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting: Vol. 5.
Practical Issues in Parenting (2nd ed., pp. 3-34). Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Bonds, D. D., Gondoli, D. M., Sturge-Apple, M. L., & Salem, L. N. (2002). Parenting stress as a mediator of the relation
between parenting support and optimal parenting. Parenting, Science and Practice, 2(4), 409-435.
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327922PAR0204_04

Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Parenting infants. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting
(2nd ed., pp. 3-43). Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Azuma, H., Galperín, C., Maital, S., Ogino, M., . . . Wright, B. (1998). A cross-national
study of self-evaluations and attributions in parenting: Argentina, Belgium, France, Israel, Italy, Japan, and the United
States. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 662-676. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.4.662

Bornstein, M. H., & Lamb, M. E. (2015). Developmental science: An advanced textbook (7th ed.). New York, NY, USA:
Psychology Press.

Braza, P., Carreras, R., Muñoz, J. M., Braza, F., Azurmendi, A., Pascual-Sagastizábal, E., . . . Sánchez-Martín, J. R. (2015).
Negative maternal and paternal parenting styles as predictors of children’s behavioral problems: Moderating effects of
the child’s sex. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(4), 847-856. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9893-0

Casiglia, A. C., LoCoco, A., & Zappulla, C. (1998). Aspects of social reputation and peer relationships in Italian children: A
cross-cultural perspective. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 723-730. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.4.723

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Erlbaum.

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer: Quantitative methods in psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

Confalonieri, E. (2010). Psicologia scolastica oggi fra teoria ed esperienza [School psychology today between theory and
experience]. In E. Bittanti (Ed.), Scuola e psicologia: un'alleanza possibile?: Insegnanti, psicologi e genitori in un
progetto di psicologia scolastica (pp. 21-26). Milan, Italy: Franco Angeli.

Confalonieri, E., Bacchini, D., Olivari, M. G., Affuso, G., Tagliabue, S., & Miranda, M. C. (2010). Adolescenti e stili educativi
genitoriali: Quale percezione? [Adolescents and parenting educational styles: What perception?]. Psicologia
dell’Educazione, 4, 9-27.

Confalonieri, E., Giuliani, C., & Tagliabue, S. (2009). Stili genitoriali autorevoli e autoritari e loro dimensioni: Primo contributo
all’adattamento italiano di uno strumento self-report [Authorithative and authoritarian parenting style and their

Parenting Style in Mothers and Fathers 526

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fa0033480
https://doi.org/10.2307%2F1127295
https://doi.org/10.1080%2F15295190903290790
https://doi.org/10.1207%2FS15327922PAR0204_04
https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0012-1649.34.4.662
https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs10826-013-9893-0
https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0012-1649.34.4.723
https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0033-2909.112.1.155
https://www.psychopen.eu/


dimensions: First contribution to the Italian adaptation of a self-report instrument]. Bollettino di Psicologia Applicata,
2009(258), 51-61.

Coolahan, K., McWayne, C., Fantuzzo, J., & Grim, S. (2002). Validation of a multidimensional assessment of parenting
styles for low-income African-American families with preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 17(3),
356-373. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2006(02)00169-2

Crnic, K., & Low, C. (2002). Everyday stresses and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 5:
Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed., pp. 243-267). Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An integrative model. Psychological Bulletin, 113(3),
487-496. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.487

Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. E. (2006). Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to distress and warmth to child
outcomes. Child Development, 77(1), 44-58. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00855.x

Deater‐Deckard, K. (1998). Parenting stress and child adjustment: Some old hypotheses and new questions. Clinical
Psychology: Science and Practice, 5(3), 314-332. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1998.tb00152.x

Deater-Deckard, K., & Scarr, S. (1996). Parenting stress among dual-earner mothers and fathers: Are there gender
differences? Journal of Family Psychology, 10(1), 45-59. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.10.1.45

Denham, S. A., Workman, E., Cole, P. M., Weissbrod, C., Kendziora, K. T., & Zahn–Waxler, C. (2000). Prediction of
externalizing behavior problems from early to middle childhood: The role of parental socialization and emotion
expression. Development and Psychopathology, 12(1), 23-45. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400001024

García, F., & Gracia, E. (2009). Is always authoritative the optimum parenting style? Evidence from Spanish families.
Adolescence, 44(173), 101-131.

Goodman, R. (1999). The extended version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a guide to child psychiatric
caseness and consequent burden. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(5), 791-799.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00494

Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337-1345. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015

Guarino, A., Di Blasio, P., D’Alessio, M., Camisasca, E., & Serantoni, G. (2008). Parenting stress index SF. Florence, Italy:
Organizzazioni Speciali.

Hoff, E., Laursen, B., & Tardif, T. (2002). Socioeconomic status and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and ecology of parenting, (2nd ed., pp. 231-252). Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.

Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT, USA: Yale University.

Jacobson, K. C., & Crockett, L. J. (2000). Parental monitoring and adolescent adjustment: An ecological perspective.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 10(1), 65-97. https://doi.org/10.1207/SJRA1001_4

Delvecchio, Germani, Raspa et al. 527

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://doi.org/10.1016%2FS0885-2006%2802%2900169-2
https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0033-2909.113.3.487
https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-8624.2006.00855.x
https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1468-2850.1998.tb00152.x
https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0893-3200.10.1.45
https://doi.org/10.1017%2FS0954579400001024
https://doi.org/10.1111%2F1469-7610.00494
https://doi.org/10.1097%2F00004583-200111000-00015
https://doi.org/10.1207%2FSJRA1001_4
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Kern, R. M., & Jonyniene, J. (2012). Psychometric properties of the Lithuanian version of the Parenting Styles and
Dimensions Questionnaire (PSDQ): Pilot study. The Family Journal, 20(2), 205-214.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1066480712439845

Laible, D. (2004). Mother-child discourse surrounding a child’s past behavior at 30 months: Links to emotional
understanding and early conscience development at 36 months. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50(2), 159-180.
https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2004.0013

Larzelere, R. E., Morris, A. S. E., & Harrist, A. W. (2013). Authoritative parenting: Synthesizing nurturance and discipline for
optimal child development. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychological Association.

Laudani, C., Guzzo, G., Cascio, V. L., Pace, U., & Cacioppo, M. (2014). Does a Mediterranean model of family functioning
in the perception of Italian and Spanish adolescents exist? A cross-national study. Mediterranean Journal of Social
Sciences, 5(1), 377-385. https://doi.org/10.5901/mjss.2014.v5n1p377

Luyckx, K., Tildesley, E. A., Soenens, B., Andrews, J. A., Hampson, S. E., Peterson, M., & Duriez, B. (2011). Parenting and
trajectories of children’s maladaptive behaviors: A 12-year prospective community study. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 40(3), 468-478. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2011.563470

Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview. Developmental
Psychology, 28(6), 1006-1017. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1006

Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M.
Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality and social development (4th ed., pp.
1-101). New York, NY, USA: John Wiley.

Magill-Evans, J., & Harrison, M. J. (2001). Parent-child interactions, parenting stress, and developmental outcomes at 4
years. Children’s Health Care, 30(2), 135-150. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326888CHC3002_4

McBride, B. A., & Mills, G. (1993). A comparison of mother and father involvement with their preschool age children. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 457-477. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2006(05)80080-8

Olivari, M. G., Tagliabue, S., & Confalonieri, E. (2013). Parenting style and dimensions questionnaire: A review of reliability
and validity. Marriage & Family Review, 49(6), 465-490. https://doi.org/10.1080/01494929.2013.770812

Önder, A., & Gülay, H. (2009). Reliability and validity of parenting styles & dimensions questionnaire. Procedia: Social and
Behavioral Sciences, 1(1), 508-514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2009.01.092

Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., Mowbray, C., & Kahng, S. K. (2004). Parenting self‐construals of mothers with a serious mental
illness: Efficacy, burden, and personal growth. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(12), 2503-2523.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb01989.x

Padilla-Walker, L. M., & Coyne, S. M. (2011). “Turn that thing off!” parent and adolescent predictors of proactive media
monitoring. Journal of Adolescence, 34(4), 705-715. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.09.002

Porter, C. L., Hart, C. H., Yang, C., Robinson, C. C., Olsen, S. F., Zeng, Q., . . . Jin, S. (2005). A comparative study of child
temperament and parenting in Beijing, China and the western United States. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 29(6), 541-551. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650250500147402

Parenting Style in Mothers and Fathers 528

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1066480712439845
https://doi.org/10.1353%2Fmpq.2004.0013
https://doi.org/10.5901%2Fmjss.2014.v5n1p377
https://doi.org/10.1080%2F15374416.2011.563470
https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0012-1649.28.6.1006
https://doi.org/10.1207%2FS15326888CHC3002_4
https://doi.org/10.1016%2FS0885-2006%2805%2980080-8
https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01494929.2013.770812
https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.sbspro.2009.01.092
https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1559-1816.2004.tb01989.x
https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.adolescence.2010.09.002
https://doi.org/10.1177%2F01650250500147402
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in
multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879-891. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

Putnick, D. L., Bornstein, M. H., Hendricks, C., Painter, K. M., Suwalsky, J. T., & Collins, W. A. (2008). Parenting stress,
perceived parenting behaviors, and adolescent self-concept in European American families. Journal of Family
Psychology, 22(5), 752-762. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013177

Putnick, D. L., Bornstein, M. H., Hendricks, C., Painter, K. M., Suwalsky, J. T., & Collins, W. A. (2010). Stability, continuity,
and similarity of parenting stress in European American mothers and fathers across their child’s transition to
adolescence. Parenting, Science and Practice, 10(1), 60-77. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295190903014638

Rinaldi, C. M., & Howe, N. (2012). Mothers’ and fathers’ parenting styles and associations with toddlers’ externalizing,
internalizing, and adaptive behaviors. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(2), 266-273.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.001

Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., & Hart, C. H. (1995). Authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting
practices: Development of a new measure. Psychological Reports, 77(3), 819-830.
https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.819

Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., & Hart, C. H. (2001). The parenting styles and dimensions questionnaire. In B.
F. Perlmutter, J. Touliatos, & G. W. Holden (Eds.), Handbook of family measurement techniques: Vol. 3. instruments &
index (pp. 319–321). Thousand Oaks, CA, USA: Sage.

Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145-172.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145

Russell, A., Hart, C., Robinson, C., & Olsen, S. (2003). Children’s sociable and aggressive behaviour with peers: A
comparison of the US and Australia, and contributions of temperament and parenting styles. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 27(1), 74-86. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250244000038

Russell, B. S., Londhe, R., & Britner, P. A. (2013). Parental contributions to the delay of gratification in preschool-aged
children. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22(4), 471-478. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-012-9599-8

Scabini, E., & Cigoli, V. (2000). Il famigliare. Legami, simboli e transizioni. Milan, Italy: Raffaello Cortina Editore.

Scabini, E., Marta, E., & Lanz, M. (2006). The transition to adulthood and family relations: An intergenerational perspective.
London, United Kingdom: Psychology Press.

Schaefer, E. S. (1959). A circumplex model for maternal behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59(2),
226-235. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0041114

Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children’s reports of parental behavior: An inventory. Child Development, 36(2), 413-424.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1126465

Seginer, R., Vermulst, A., & Gerris, J. (2002). Bringing up adolescent children: A longitudinal study of parents’ child-rearing
stress. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(5), 410-422. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250143000355

Delvecchio, Germani, Raspa et al. 529

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://doi.org/10.3758%2FBRM.40.3.879
https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fa0013177
https://doi.org/10.1080%2F15295190903014638
https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.ecresq.2011.08.001
https://doi.org/10.2466%2Fpr0.1995.77.3.819
https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0033-295X.110.1.145
https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01650250244000038
https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs10826-012-9599-8
https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fh0041114
https://doi.org/10.2307%2F1126465
https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01650250143000355
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Smetana, J. G. (2008). “It’s 10 o’clock: Do you know where your children are?” Recent advances in understanding parental
monitoring and adolescents’ information management. Child Development Perspectives, 2(1), 19-25.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00036.x

Spera, C. (2005). A review of the relationship among parenting practices, parenting styles, and adolescent school
achievement. Educational Psychology Review, 17(2), 125-146. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-005-3950-1

Steinberg, L., Elmen, J. D., & Mounts, N. S. (1989). Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity, and academic success
among adolescents. Child Development, 60(6), 1424-1436. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130932

Steinberg, L., Mounts, N. S., Lamborn, S. D., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Authoritative parenting and adolescent adjustment
across varied ecological niches. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 1(1), 19-36.

Topham, G. L., Hubbs-Tait, L., Rutledge, J. R., Page, M. C., Kennedy, T. S., Shriver, L. H., & Harrist, A. W. (2011). Parenting
styles, parental response to child emotion, and family emotional responsiveness are related to child emotional eating.
Appetite, 56, 261-264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.007

Williams, L. R., Degnan, K. A., Perez-Edgar, K. E., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Pine, D. S., . . . Fox, N. A. (2009).
Impact of behavioral inhibition and parenting style on internalizing and externalizing problems from early childhood
through adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(8), 1063-1075.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9331-3

Woolfson, L., & Grant, E. (2006). Authoritative parenting and parental stress in parents of pre‐school and older children with
developmental disabilities. Child Care, Health and Development, 32(2), 177-184.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00603.x

Wu, P., Robinson, C. C., Yang, C., Hart, C. H., Olsen, S. F., Porter, C. L., . . . Wu, X. (2002). Similarities and differences in
mothers’ parenting of preschoolers in China and the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
26(6), 481-491. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250143000436

Yeh, K. H., & Bedford, O. (2004). Filial belief and parent-child conflict. International Journal of Psychology, 39(2), 132-144.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000312

A bout  the  A uthor s

Elisa Delvecchio, Ph.D., is assistant professor in Dynamic Psychology. She is the representative of the international
relationships for the psychological field at the University of Perugia. She is the coordinator of the “Psychology and Cultures
Lab” at the International Human-being Research Centre (IHRC). Her main research interests focus on the role of personal
and interpersonal variables for the prevention and treatment of anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescence adopting a
cross-cultural perspective. She is currently running a project aimed to assess the link between psychological well-being and
cultural beliefs in adolescents and emerging adults across the world. She has authored or coauthored more than 30 articles
in international peer reviewed scientific journals.

Alessandro Germani is a Ph.D candidate in Human Sciences. His research interests concern Clinical and Dynamic
Psychology, Cross-Cultural Psychology, as well as Cognitive Neuroscience.

Veronica Raspa, Ph.D., is an expert in the subject of Dynamic Psychology. She is a contract professor of Theory and
Techniques of Interview (University of Perugia), and General Psychology (University of Siena). She collaborates with the

Parenting Style in Mothers and Fathers 530

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1750-8606.2008.00036.x
https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs10648-005-3950-1
https://doi.org/10.2307%2F1130932
https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.appet.2011.01.007
https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs10802-009-9331-3
https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1365-2214.2006.00603.x
https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01650250143000436
https://doi.org/10.1080%2F00207590344000312
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Center for Documentation, Update and Experimentation on childhood in the Region of Umbria. She participates in research
projects related to early childhood, specific educational services, parenting and the reflective function.

Adriana Lis, Senior Prof. at the University of Padova from 1/10/2014. She was Full Professor at the same University
and she taught Psychology of Family Relation in 2014/2015, 2015/2016, 2016/2017. Moreover, she was Professor of
Psychology of family relation, Psychodynamic diagnosis, Intervention models for family relationships, Evaluation of psy-
chotherapeutic interventions. She published over 50 national and international papers as well as several national and
international books.

Claudia Mazzeschi, Ph.D, is full professor in Dynamic Psychology and Head of the Department of Philosophy, Social Sci-
ences and Education (University of Perugia). She is the coordinator of the clinical service at the University of Perugia and
the scientific coordinator for the psychological area of CURIAMO, a clinical and research centre for obesity and diabetes
affiliated to the University of Perugia. Her main research interests focus on psychological assessment and intervention
throughout the life span. She has authored or coauthored more than 50 articles in international peer reviewed scientific
journals.

Delvecchio, Germani, Raspa et al. 531

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2020, Vol. 16(3), 514–531
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v16i3.2013

PsychOpen GOLD is a publishing service by
Leibniz Institute for Psychology Information (ZPID),
Trier, Germany. www.leibniz-psychology.org

https://www.leibniz-psychology.org/
https://www.psychopen.eu/

	Parenting Style in Mothers and Fathers
	(Introduction)
	Issues About the PSDQ
	PSDQ and Related Variables

	Method
	Participants
	Procedure
	Measures
	Data Analysis

	Results
	The Effect of Parental Role, Child’s Age, and Age-Group on Authoritative and Authoritarian Parenting Styles
	Correlations Between Selected Variables by Parental Role
	Parenting Styles, Parenting Stress and Child’s Psychological Well-Being

	Discussion
	Notes
	(Additional Information)
	Funding
	Competing Interests
	Acknowledgments
	Ethical Approval

	References
	About the Authors