Technostress, Coping, and Anxious and Depressive Symptomatology in University Students During the COVID-19 Pandemic


Research Reports

Technostress, Coping, and Anxious and Depressive Symptomatology in 
University Students During the COVID-19 Pandemic

John Galvin 1, Michael Scott Evans 2, Kenisha Nelson 3, Gareth Richards 4, Eirini Mavritsaki 1, Theodoros Giovazolias 5, 

Katerina Koutra 5, Ben Mellor 4, Maria Clelia Zurlo 6, Andrew Paul Smith 2, Federica Vallone 6

[1] Birmingham City University, Birmingham, United Kingdom. [2] Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom. [3] University of Technology, Kingston, Jamaica. 
[4] Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom. [5] University of Crete, Rethymno, 74100, Greece. [6] University of Naples Federico II, Naples, 
Italy. 

Europe's Journal of Psychology, 2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318, https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

Received: 2020-11-22 • Accepted: 2021-05-16 • Published (VoR): 2022-08-31

Handling Editor: Michelle E. Roley-Roberts, Creighton University, Omaha, NE, USA

Corresponding Author: John Galvin, Department of Psychology, Curzon Building, Cardigan Street, Birmingham City University, B4 7BD, Birmingham, 
United Kingdom. E-mail: john.galvin@bcu.ac.uk

Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic raised many challenges for university staff and students, including the need to work from home, which 
resulted in a greater reliance on technology. We collected questionnaire data from university students (N = 894) in three European 
countries: Greece, Italy, and the United Kingdom. Data were collected between 7th April 2020 and 19th June 2020, representing a 
period covering the first lockdown and university closures in these countries and across Europe generally. We tested the hypotheses 
that technology-related stressors (techno-overload, work-home conflict, techno-ease, techno-reliability, techno-sociality, and pace of 
change) would be associated with anxiety and depressive symptoms, and that coping styles (problem-focused, emotion-focused, and 
avoidance) would mediate these relationships. Results showed significant positive associations between techno-overload, work-home 
conflict and anxiety and depressive symptoms, and significant negative associations between techno-reliability, techno-ease and 
anxiety and depressive symptoms. A significant negative association was found between techno-sociality and depressive symptoms 
but not anxiety symptoms. No evidence was found for an association between pace of change and anxiety or depressive symptoms. 
Multiple mediation analyses revealed significant direct effects of techno-overload, work-home conflict and techno-ease on anxiety 
symptoms, and of work-home conflict and techno-ease on depressive symptoms. Work-home conflict had significant indirect effects 
on anxiety and depressive symptoms through avoidance coping. Techno-overload and techno-ease both had significant indirect 
effects on anxiety symptoms through problem- and emotion-focused coping. Techno-ease also had a significant indirect effect on 
depressive symptoms through problem-focused coping. The findings add to the body of evidence on technostress amongst university 
students and provide knowledge on how technostress translates through coping strategies into anxious and depressive symptoms 
during the disruption caused by the outbreak of a pandemic disease.

Keywords
university students, technostress, coping, anxiety, depression, COVID-19

In response to the novel coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19), the World Health Organisation declared a global pandemic on 
11th March 2020. As of 15th May 2021, the Coronavirus Resource Centre at Johns Hopkins University and Medicine 
(2021) reported 161,566,026 confirmed cases and 3,353,630 deaths worldwide. Government officials and public health 
experts have taken several steps to control the spread of the virus, including imposing special measures on their 

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons 
Attribution 4.0 International License, CC BY 4.0, which permits unrestricted use, 
distribution, and reproduction, provided the original work is properly cited.

https://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.5964/ejop.4725&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2022-08-31
https://www.psychopen.eu/
https://ejop.psychopen.eu/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/


populations, such as self-isolation and restriction of movement and assembly, which have led to a high number of 
individuals having to work from home.

The higher education (HE) sector has been severely impacted by the COVID-19 crisis. The pandemic necessitated 
a rapid transition from a predominantly face-to-face teaching model to an online only or heavily blended learning 
model for many academic courses (Watermeyer et al., 2021). Although online teaching and learning is not new for 
many universities, a predominantly online model is new to many staff and students. This transition to a purely digital 
teaching and learning experience has, by its very nature, an intrinsic expectation that staff and students are able to use 
technology for all intents and purposes, that the technology is reliable, and that they have workspaces at home which 
would mirror the workplace environment, i.e., without distractions or conflicting home demands (Sahu, 2020).

Technostress was first defined by Brod (1984) as the inability to adapt or cope with information and communication 
technologies (ICTs) in a healthy manner. This definition is in line with Lazarus and Folkman’s (1984) suggestion that 
stress refers to any demand, event or situation that disturbs the adaptive state and threatens to exceed the individual’s 
resources and skills. If the individual’s adaptive state is altered by an event, this may provoke a coping response 
(Lazarus & Folkman, 1991). If people maintain adaptive coping responses, they are less likely to appraise a situation as 
threatening and have improved mental health outcomes (Freire et al., 2016; Taylor & Stanton, 2007).

Previous research has shown that technostress in university students is associated with a range of psychopathologi­
cal outcomes including higher anxiety, depression, burnout, and suicidality (Kim et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2020). Several 
factors have been identified as determinants of technostress (hereinafter referred to as techno-stressors) (Ayyagari et 
al., 2011; DeLone & McLean, 2003; Jiang et al., 2002; Kreiner, 2006; Moore, 2000; Moore & Benbasat, 1991; Netemeyer 
et al., 1996; Weiss & Heide, 1993). Techno-overload refers to the situation in which individuals feel forced by ICTs to 
work faster and longer. Work-home conflict is when work and private life merge due to ICT usage. Pace of change refers 
to an individual’s perception of frequent ICT-related changes and upgrades. Techno-ease refers to whether or not the 
user feels competent enough to use ICTs and to achieve the desired results. Techno-sociality refers to ICT as a social 
communication tool by which individuals can contact, or be contacted by other people. Finally, techno-reliability is the 
perception of the consistency or dependability of ICTs.

An ability to cope with techno-stressors will depend on individual resources (e.g., coping competencies) as well as 
environmental factors (e.g., circumstances). Coping can be defined as acts of adaptation that an individual performs 
in response to events that occur in his/her environment (Folkman & Lazarus, 1988; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Coping 
responses are commonly categorised into three broader themes: problem-focused, emotion-focused, and avoidance 
coping (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Roth & Cohen, 1986). Problem-focused coping involves handling the stressor by 
taking action to solve the problem, facing it head-on, and making attempts to resolve the underlying cause. Examples 
of problem-focused coping include planning and taking active steps to address the problem. Emotion-focused coping 
involves the regulation of feelings and emotional responses that arise, as opposed to directly addressing the problem. 
Examples of emotion-focused coping include accessing social support networks and venting about the problem. Finally, 
avoidance coping is characterised by coping efforts aimed at avoiding the stressor, and examples include disengagement, 
denial, and substance use. While problem-focused coping is often considered the most effective coping strategy and 
avoidance coping the least effective, research shows that the most effective strategy can depend on the type of stressor 
encountered and/or other environmental circumstances (Bonanno & Burton, 2013; Lee-Baggley et al., 2005). Therefore, 
individuals might not differ only in their choice of coping strategies, but also in the extent and context in which they 
engage in any single strategy.

The digitalisation of society and the labour-market has been on a constant rise over the last decades (Vasilescu et 
al., 2020), and this shift has been exacerbated by the COVID-19 crisis. Although digitalisation has some advantages, it 
also results in important challenges, including a rise in the phenomenon of technostress in distance education. It has 
thus never been more pertinent to investigate techno-stressors and their relationships with mental health outcomes 
in the student population. In this study, we explored the relationships between techno-stressors, coping strategies and 
anxious/depressive symptoms in a sample of students from three European countries: Greece, Italy and UK. It was 
hypothesised that:

Galvin, Evans, Nelson et al. 303

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


H1: Techno-overload, work-home conflict and pace of change would be positively associated with 
anxiety and depressive symptoms.

H2: Techno-ease, techno-reliability and techno-sociality would be negatively associated with anx­
ious and depressive symptoms.

H3: Coping style would mediate the association between techno-stressors and anxious/depressive 
symptoms.

M e t h o d

Procedure and Participants
Participants were given an information sheet that provided basic details of the study, and were required to complete a 
consent form before taking part. An online cross-sectional survey (hosted by Qualtrics) was distributed to university 
students in Greece, Italy and the UK between 7th April 2020 and 19th June 2020. This period covered a timeframe in 
which the first lockdown was implemented and included full closures of universities in all three countries. Participants 
were studying at undergraduate or masters level, and were recruited from the affiliative institutions of the authors 
through research participation databases and student learning forums. In Greece, a link to the survey was sent by 
e-mail to faculty members in universities across different cities and regions of the country (Crete, Athens, Thessaloniki, 
Thessaly, Epirus, Thrace) who then forwarded it to their students using either academic mailing lists or student social 
media groups. In Italy, a link to the survey was sent via academic mailing lists and social media groups for three univer­
sities in the southwestern region of Campania (Naples and Benevento). In the UK, the Psychology Department Research 
Participation Schemes (RPS) at Birmingham City University and Newcastle University were used. The questionnaire 
link was also sent to student social media groups at Birmingham City University, Newcastle University, Northumbria 
University, and the University of Liverpool. Students recruited in the UK through RPS were awarded participation 
credits. All other participants did not receive any reward for completing the study.

Materials
After demographic questions (sex, age, relationship status, course status, level of study and employment status), a series 
of questions on technology usage was presented. This included a question asking the participants to provide detail on 
the technological device(s) they have in their home, as well as a question on the device(s) that they personally own. 
Participants were also asked how many people (including themselves) live in their household, and whether they have 
their own personal space to use technological device(s).

Technostress Scale

Techno-stressors were measured with validated survey items from prior studies. The constructs, items, and internal 
reliability coefficients for the present study are detailed in Table 1. Participants responded to 17 items on a seven-point 
Likert scale (1 = Strongly Disagree to 7 = Strongly Agree).

Coping Style

The 60-item version of the COPE inventory (Carver et al., 1989) was used to measure coping style. It comprises 
15 subscales: positive reinterpretation and growth, mental disengagement, focus on and venting of emotions, use 
of instrumental social support, active coping, denial, religious coping, suppression of competing activities, humour, 
behavioural disengagement, restraint, use of emotional social support, substance abuse, acceptance, and planning. 
Although the original scale has 15 subscales, Carver et al. (1989) suggest three higher order factors (problem-focused, 
emotion-focused, and avoidance coping) based on factor analysis. Overall internal consistency for the COPE factors in 
the present study were as follows: problem-focused coping α = .908; emotion-focused coping α = .850; avoidance coping 
α = .702. In Greece, problem-focused coping α = .878, emotion-focused coping α = .841, and avoidance coping α = .716. 

Technostress, Coping and Mental Health in Students 304

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


In Italy, problem-focused coping α = .844, emotion-focused coping α = .821, and avoidance coping α = .669. In the UK, 
problem-focused coping α = .941, emotion-focused coping α = .861, and avoidance coping α = .711.

Table 1

Technostress Constructs, Items, and Cronbach’s Alpha Scores for the Present Study

Technostress Factors and Items Reference(s) Overall Sample Greece Italy UK

Techno-Overload
• ICTs create many more requests and problems than I would otherwise experience
• I feel busy or rushed due to ICTs
• I feel pressured due to ICTs

Moore (2000) α = .82 α = .83 α = .78 α = .84

Work-Home Conflict
• Using ICTs blurs boundaries between my work and my home life
• Using ICTs for work related responsibilities creates conflicts with my home 

responsibilities
• I do not get everything done at home because I find myself completing work due 

to ICTs

Kreiner (2006) α = .77 α = .74 α = .78 α = .80

Techno-Ease
• Learning to use ICTs is easy for me
• ICTs are easy to use
• It is easy to get results that I desire from ICTs

Moore and Benbasat (1991) α = .85 α = .86 α = .82 α = .85

Techno-Reliability
• The features provided by ICTs are dependable
• The capabilities provided by ICTs are reliable
• ICTs behave in a highly consistent way

DeLone and McLean (2003)

Jiang et al. (2002)

α = .85 α = .86 α = .82 α = .85

Techno-Sociality
• The use of ICTs enables others to have access to me
• The use of ICTs enables me to be in touch with others

Ayyagari et al. (2011) α = .74 α = .59 α = .90 α = .86

Pace of Change
• I feel that there are frequent changes in the features of ICTs
• I feel that the capabilities of ICTs change often
• I feel that the way ICTs work changes often

Weiss and Heide (1993) α = .84 α = .85 α = .88 α = .83

Anxious and Depressive Symptoms

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (Zigmond & Snaith, 1983) consists of 14 items, with seven measuring 
anxiety symptoms, and seven measuring depressive symptoms. Participants’ responses are coded on a scale of 0–3 for 
each item. The questionnaire is designed to assess an individual’s mental state over the previous two weeks. Overall 
internal consistency was α = .822 for anxiety symptoms and α = .688 for depressive symptoms. In Greece, α = .797 
for anxiety symptoms and α = .673 for depressive symptoms. In Italy, α = .818 for anxiety symptoms and α = .653 for 
depressive symptoms. In the UK, α = .843 for anxiety symptoms and α = .697 for depressive symptoms.

Translation of Scales Into Greek and Italian

The UK sample completed the questionnaire in English, including the original English versions of the COPE (Carver 
et al., 1989) and HADS (Zigmond & Snaith, 1983). For distribution in Italy and Greece, the information sheet, consent 
form, debrief form, demographic and technostress items were translated into Greek by authors TG, KK, and EM, and 
into Italian by author FV. The scales were then back-translated into English by the same authors. We used the Italian 
versions of the COPE (Sica et al., 2008) and HADS (Costantini et al., 1999) in Italy, and the Greek versions of the COPE 
(Roussi, 2001) and HADS (Michopoulos et al., 2008) in Greece.

Data Analysis
Data were analysed using JASP software version 0.14.1 (JASP Team, 2020) and statistical significance was set at 
5% (two-tailed). Differences between countries on demographic and study variables were examined with ANOVA 
(Bonferroni corrected) and with chi-square test for categorical variables. Because the utilisation and effectiveness of 

Galvin, Evans, Nelson et al. 305

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


coping strategies can rely on specific environmental contexts (Bonanno & Burton, 2013; Lee-Baggley et al., 2005), and 
given the uniqueness of the pandemic situation, we identified coping factors with an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) 
using principal components extraction and promax oblique rotation. As the technostress scale has not previously been 
validated in Greek or Italian, we conducted confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) on the scale followed by multi-group 
confirmatory factor analysis (MGCFA) to examine measurement invariance.

Measurement invariance comprises configural, metric and scalar invariance. Configural invariance examines wheth­
er the measurement scale has a similar factor structure across the different countries. It is tested by imposing the same 
structure across groups and allowing all model estimated parameters to differ. Metric invariance examines whether 
the rating scales are used similarly in the different countries. It is tested by examining whether the different countries 
have the same factor loadings for the same item. Finally, scalar invariance examines whether the different countries 
have the same item intercepts. It is achieved by constraining intercepts to be equal across groups. Establishing scalar 
invariance would enable meaningful comparison of the means across the countries (Little, 1997). The goodness of fit 
indices for CFA and MGCFA models include the chi-square (χ2) statistic, root mean square error of approximation 
(RMSEA), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lews index (TLI), and Standardised Root Mean Squared Residual (SRMR). 
The common guidelines for an acceptable model fit are: χ2, p > .05, RMSEA < .08; CFI > .90; TLI > .90; SRMR < .09. As the 
chi-square test is strongly influenced by sample size (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002), we relied on the RMSEA, CFI, TLI and 
SRMR to assess model fit. The assessment of measurement invariance involved testing the deterioration of the model fit 
between the configural, metric and scalar model. Changes in CFI, TLI, and RMSEA of < .01 are considered acceptable 
(Rutkowski & Svetina, 2014).

We examined the associations between all variables using Pearson’s correlation. This was followed by four multiple 
linear regression analyses (enter method). The first two regressions included the technostress factors as predictors 
and HADS anxiety and depression subscale scores as outcomes. The remaining two regressions included COPE 
factors as predictors and anxiety/depression as outcomes. The independent errors assumption was checked with the 
Durbin-Watson statistic, and the multicollinearity assumption was tested with Variance Inflation Factor (VIF). Mediation 
analysis was then performed (bootstrap 5000 iterations and bias-corrected). The predictor variables included in the 
analysis were each of the significant techno-stressors from the multiple regression step. Mediators were each of the 
significant COPE factors from the multiple regression step. Outcome variables were the HADS anxiety and depression 
subscale scores. The maximum likelihood estimation was used to estimate the direct and indirect effects. Background 
confounders included age, sex (female), relationship status (single), level of study (masters), international student (yes) 
and employment status (employed). The full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation was used to deal with 
the missing values (< 10%) in the final sample.

R e s u l t s

Demographics
The questionnaire was accessed by N = 963 participants. Forty were removed from the analysis as they did not respond 
to any of the study variables. A further 17 were removed because they reported that they were not students and 12 
were removed as they were doctoral level students. This resulted in a total sample size of N = 894 (Greece, n = 343; 
Italy, n = 120; UK, n = 431). Participants were studying a range of subjects, including psychology (n = 262), core sciences 
(biology, chemistry or physics) (n = 142), engineering (n = 88), medicine (n = 83), social studies (n = 45), business (n = 
66), languages (n = 42), education (n = 36), history (n = 36), art or media studies (n = 15), geography (n = 15), maths (n = 
15), nursing (n = 13), law (n = 11), philosophy (n = 10), architecture (n = 6), and archaeology (n = 6). Participants differed 
significantly across countries on all demographic variables except for sex, ownership of a mobile phone, and having a 
desktop or mobile phone at home (Table 2).

Technostress, Coping and Mental Health in Students 306

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Table 2

Demographic Information For the Overall Sample and Stratified By Nation

Sample Characteristic Total Sample Greece Italy United Kingdom Statistic

Sex n (%)
Females 686(77) 267(78) 96(80) 323(75)
Males 206(23) 74(21) 24(20) 108(25) χ2 = 5.178

Other 0(0) 0(0) 0(0) 0(0) p = .270
Prefer not to say 2(0) 2(1) 0(0) 0(0)

Age in years
M, SD 21.58, 4.29 22.99, 5. 58 22.47, 4.10 20.20, 2.21 F = 46.371
(Range) (18–56) (18–56) (19–38) (18–44) p < .001

Relationship n (%)
Single 747(84) 58(17) 47(39) 42(10) χ2 = 59.229

Relationship 147(16) 285(83) 73(61) 389(90) p < .001

Course n (%)
Full-time 824(96) 318(98) 92(80) 414(99) χ2 = 100.917

Part-time 33(4) 6(2) 23(20) 4(1) p < .001

Study level n (%)
Bachelors 727(89) 264(93) 101(87) 362(87) χ2 = 7.353

Masters 91(11) 20(7) 15(13) 56(13) p = .025

Employment n (%)
Full-time 68(7) 34(11) 8(7) 21(5)
Part-time 228(27) 27(9) 27(23) 174(40) χ2 = 99.036

Not employed 538(63) 229(77) 83(68) 226(53) p < .001
Prefer not to say 22(3) 10(3) 2(2) 10(2)

Technology devices at home n (%)
Laptop

Yes 840(94) 302(88) 110(92) 428(99) χ2 = 43.932

No 54(6) 41(12) 10(8) 3(1) p < .001
Desktop

Yes 340(38) 118(34) 53(44) 169(39) χ2 = 4.088

No 554(62) 225(66) >67(56) 262(61) p = .130
Tablet

Yes 472(53) 142(41) 56(47) 274(64) χ2 = 39.771

No 422(47) 201(59) 64(53) 157(36) p < .001
Mobile

Yes 841(99) 295(99) 117(98) 429(99) χ2 = 43.932

No 8(1) 3(1) 3(2) 2(1) p = .123
Othera 37(4) 8(2) 7(5) 22(5) —

Technology devices personally owned n (%)
Laptop

Yes 722(86) 268(78) 95(80) 409(95) χ2 = 51.611

No 122(14) 75(22) 25(20) 22(5) p < .001
Desktop

Yes 106(12) 50(15) 18(15) 38(9) χ2 = 7.375/

No 788(88) 293(85) 102(85) 393(91) p = .003
Tablet

Yes 278(31) 97(28) 25(20) 156(36) χ2 = 12.398

No 616(69) 246(72) 95(80) 275(64) p = .002
Mobile

Yes 842(99) 295(99) 119(99) 428(99) χ2 = 0.208

No 7(1) 3(1) 1(1) 3(1) p = .901
Othera 20(2) 4(1) 3(2) 13(3) —

Galvin, Evans, Nelson et al. 307

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Sample Characteristic Total Sample Greece Italy United Kingdom Statistic

Personal space? n (%)
Yes 698(79) 254(75) 100(85) 344(80) χ2 = 6.929

No 187(21) 87(25) 19(15) 81(20) p = .031

Number of people living in household M 3.58(1.51) 2.87(1.58) 3.69(1.11) 3.85(1.58) F = 40.364
(SD) p < .001

a Responses to “other” included: Game Consoles (n = 18), Smart TVs (n = 10), Home Hubs (n =7), and Smartwatch (n = 2).

EFA on the COPE Scale
Principal components analysis (PCA) was conducted on the COPE scale. The PCA confirmed three factors with 
eigenvalues greater than 1, which together explained 62% of the variance (Table 3). The factors were aligned closely 
with the findings of Carver et al. (1989) and represented problem-focused, emotion-focused, and avoidance coping. 
The first factor represented problem-focused coping, with high loadings from the following COPE subscales: positive 
reinterpretation and growth, active coping, restraint, acceptance, humour, suppression of competing activities, and planning. 
The second factor represented emotion-focused coping, with high loadings from the subscales: focusing on and venting 
of emotions, instrumental social support, and use of social support. The third factor represented avoidance coping, with 
high loadings from the subscales: denial, substance use, behavioural disengagement, and mental disengagement. Religious 
coping did not load highly on any of the three factors. As religion is not a specific focus of our study, the decision was 
made to exclude this subscale from further analysis.

Table 3

Exploratory Factor Analysis of the COPE Subscales

EFA COPE subscales Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3

Planning .924

Positive reinterpretation and growth .859

Active coping .849

Acceptance .733

Suppression of competing activities .720

Restraint .626

Humour .430

Use of emotional support .943

Instrumental social support .780

Focus on and venting of emotions .749

Behavioural disengagement .856

Denial .677

Substance use .550

Mental disengagement .352

Religious coping

Note. Factor loadings below 0.3 are excluded.

CFA and MGCFA on Technostress Scale
Next, we conducted a series of confirmatory factor analyses on the technostress scale (Table 4). RMSEA, CFI and SRMR 
values indicate acceptable model fit for the Greek and the UK samples. TLI indicated acceptable fit for the UK sample 
and was very close to the acceptable threshold for the Greek sample (.893). RMSEA, CFI, TLI and SRMR indicated an 
insufficient fit for the Italian sample.

To see if the model-data fit could be improved we inspected the modification indices for each country separately. We 
based a selected model on the UK data, since English is the source language of the scales. Further estimations indicated 
that deleting the third item on the techno-ease scale ‘it is easy to get results that I desire from ICTs’ increased the fit in 

Technostress, Coping and Mental Health in Students 308

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


all countries. The item was not distinctive enough and cross-loaded with items on the techno-reliability scale. RMSEA, 
CFI, TLI and SRMR values indicated acceptable fit for the overall sample as well as for each country in the revised model 
(Table 5). These results provided a good starting point for the subsequent multi-group confirmatory factor analyses.

Table 4

Fit Indices For Technostress

Sample RMSEA CFI TLI SRMR

Overall .071 [.065-.077] .935 .915 .058

Greece .081 [.070-.091] .918 .893 .062

Italy .124 [.107-.142] .835 .785 .097

UK .071 [.061-.081] .940 .921 .062

Note. Sub-scales from the confirmatory factor analyses.

Table 5

Revised CFA

Sample RMSEA CFI TLI SRMR

Overall .047 [.040-.055] .972 .963 .038

Greece .067 [.055-.079] .944 .925 .053

Italy .074 [.051-.096] .941 .921 .064

UK .045 [.032-.058] .977 .968 .041

Note. With the removal of Item 3 from the Techno-Ease Scale.

The MGCFA consisted of three steps. The configural equivalent model was estimated first, in which we imposed 
the same factor structure on the scores in each country. A sufficiently good fit was found (Table 6), suggesting the 
measurement scale has a similar factor structure across the three countries. Next, we imposed the factor loadings to 
be the same across countries (Table 6). We expected a slight decrease in fit, which was confirmed, with a RMSEA of 
.062 and SRMR of .057. However, these are both still above the acceptable thresholds. Finally, we tested the full scalar 
invariant model and found this was acceptable with ΔRMSEA, ΔCFI, and ΔTLI < .01 (Rutkowski & Svetina, 2014). The 
comparison of latent means for the techno-stress factors can therefore be justified (Table 6).

Table 6

Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis For the Techno-Stress Scale

MGCFA RMSEA CFI TLI SRMR
Model 

comparison ΔRMSEA ΔCFI ΔTLI ΔSRMR

M1: Configural 

invariance

.061 [.051-.070] .961 .947 .049

M2: Metric 

invariance

.062 [.053-.070] .956 .945 .057 M1 .001 .005 .002 .008

M3: Scalar 

invariance

.069 [.060-.077] .947 .937 .057 M2 .007 .009 .008 .000

Differences Between Countries on the Study Variables
Table 7 details the means, standard deviations, and results of the ANOVA and Bonferroni post hoc tests. Significantly 
higher levels of anxiety and depression were found in the UK sample compared to the other countries. Work-home 

Galvin, Evans, Nelson et al. 309

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


conflict was significantly higher in the UK compared to Italy. Techno-ease was lower in the Italian sample compared 
to the other countries, and pace of change was higher in Greece in comparison with Italy. Significantly higher levels of 
avoidance-focused coping and lower levels of problem-focused coping were found in the UK sample compared to the 
other countries. The Italian sample reported higher emotion-focused coping compared to the UK sample.

Table 7

Group Means and ANOVA Tests

Means and 
differences

Total Sample Greece (1) Italy (2) UK (3) F Statistic Post 
Hoc

Technostress Factors
Techno-overload 10.74 (4.56) 10.89 (4.65) 10.29 (4.41) 10.75 (4.53) 0.718 —

Work-home conflict 11.97 (4.94) 11.73 (5.05) 10.92 (4.72) 12.55 (4.85) 5.267* 3 > 2

Techno-ease 16.22 (3.76) 16.30 (3.76) 15.18 (3.67) 16.50 (3.79) 5.383* 1 > 2

3 > 2

Techno-reliability 14.01 (3.70) 13.78 (3.75) 13.52 (3.32) 14.37 (3.76) 3.148* —

Techno-sociality 12.07 (2.46) 11.82 (2.86) 12.06 (2.10) 12.29 (2.14) 2.909 —

Pace of change 14.14 (4.30) 14.62 (4.77) 13.19 (3.64) 14.04 (3.97) 4.922* 1 > 2

COPE Inventory
Problem-focused 

coping

56.61 (18.27) 58.32 (17.27) 59.31 (13.53) 54.41 (20.00) 5.726* 1 > 3

2 > 3

Avoidance-focused 

coping

35.93 (11.13) 35.97 (9.54) 32.49 (8.37) 37.77 (12.67) 12.727** 3 > 1

3 > 2

Emotion-focused 

coping

27.64 (10.77) 28.23 (11.49) 29.59 (9.15) 26.57 (10.52) 4.455* 2 > 3

HADS
Anxious symptoms 8.81 (6.96) 7.61 (4.37) 8.73 (4.76) 9.92 (4.69) 21.429** 3 > 1

3 > 2

Depressive 

symptoms

6.99 (3.94) 6.31 (3.69) 6.08 (3.56) 7.76 (4.10) 14.555** 3 > 1

3 > 2

Note. From left to right, Mean(SD), F statistic and Bonferroni Post Hoc. 1 = Greece, 2 = Italy, 3 = UK.
*p < .05. **p < .001.

Pearson’s Correlations and Regression Analyses
Table 8 shows the Pearson correlation coefficient matrix for the study variables. In regard to hypotheses 1 and 2, 
significant associations were found between techno-overload (r = .241, p < .001), work-home conflict (r = .350, p < 
.001), techno-ease (r = -.214, p < .001), techno-reliability (r = -.196, p < .001), techno-sociality (r = -.123, p = .001) 
and depressive symptoms, but no significant correlation was found between pace of change (r = -.010, p = .795) and 
depressive symptoms. Significant correlations were found between techno-overload (r = .307, p < .001), work-home 
conflict (r = .285, p < .001), techno-ease (r = -.199, p < .001), techno-reliability (r = -.160, p < .001) and anxiety symptoms, 
but not between techno-sociality (r = -.064, p = .087), pace of change (r = .057, p = .122) and anxiety symptoms.

Four multiple regression analyses (enter method) were then performed. Two of these included the six technostress 
factors and demographic variables as predictors and the HADS anxiety and depression subscale as outcomes. The other 
two included coping factors and demographics as predictors and the HADS subscales as outcomes (Table 9). Model 1 
explained 16.9% of the total variance (p < .001) in anxiety symptoms. Techno-overload (β = .187, p < .001), work-home 
conflict (β = .201, p < .001), techno-ease (β = -.116, p = .011) and age (β = -.100, p < .001) were significant predictors of 
anxiety symptoms. Model 2 explained 16.6% of the total variance (p < .001) in depressive symptoms. Work-home conflict 
(β = .290, p < .001), techno-ease (β = -.122, p = .008) and age (β = -.118, p = .004) were significant predictors of depressive 
symptoms. Model 3 explained 17.1% of the total variance in anxiety symptoms (p < .001), and problem-focused coping 
(β = -.290, p < .001), emotion-focused coping (β = .221, p < .001), avoidance-focused coping (β = .327, p < .001) and sex 

Technostress, Coping and Mental Health in Students 310

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


(female) (β = .089, p = .017) were significant predictors of anxiety symptoms. Model 4 explained 14.5% of the variance 
(p < .001) in depressive symptoms. Problem-focused coping (β = -.312, p < .001) and avoidance-focused coping (β = 
.317, p < .001) were significant predictors of depressive symptoms. All regression models met multicollinearity and error 
independence assumptions (Table 9).

Table 8

Pearson’s Correlations Between Study Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17

1. Age —
2. Sex (Female) -.078* —
3. International 
Student (Yes)

.060 .029 —

4. Level of study 
(Masters)

.230** -.090* -.035 —

5. Employment status 
(Employed)

.225** -.055 -.058 .099* —

6. Relationship status 
(Single)

-.327** -.028 -.050 -.121** -.062 —

7. Techno-overload .013 .101* .016 -.062 .047 .048 —
8. Work home conflict .004 .078* .012 .019 .128** .027 .545** —
9. Techno-ease .037 -.138** -.008 .106* .026 .001 -.278** -.189** —
10. Techno-reliability .085* -.133** -.040 .078* -.004 -.071 -.340** -.273** .554** —
11. Techno-sociality .021 .011 -.040 .092* .156** -.026 -.114* -.014 .238** .237** —
12. Pace of change -.042 .043 .014 -.029 .155** .038 .138** .092* -.032 -.054 .235** —
13. Problem-focused 
coping

.064 .106** .020 .037 -.118** -.036 .014 -.090* .100* .201** -.080* -.005 —

14. Avoidance-focused 
coping

-.093* .054 .025 -.009 -.044 .120** .095* .095* -.001 .030 -.070 .027 .494** —

15. Emotion-focused 
coping

.011 .247** .026 .013 -.180** -.072* .089* -.086* .023 .121* -.101* -.044 .656** .373** —

16. Anxiety symptoms -.132** .146** -.005 -.068 -.019 .037 .307** .285** -.199** -.160** -.064 .057 -.014 .280** .189** —
17. Depressive 
symptoms

-.128** .058 -.036 -.060 .022 .080* .241** .350** -.214** -.196** -.123* -.010 -.179** .231** -.045 .620** —

*p < .05. **p < .001.

Coping as a Mediator Between Technostress Factors and Anxiety Symptomatology
Multiple mediation analysis was used to test hypothesis 3. The first mediation analysis investigated coping as a 
mediator between techno-stress factors and anxiety symptoms (Table 10). The total effect of techno-overload on anxiety 
symptoms was significant, β = .179, 95% CI [.080, .272]. Techno-overload had a significant indirect effect through prob­
lem-focused coping, β = -.034, 95% CI [-.077, -.002], which accounted for 18.99% of the total effect of techno-overload 
on anxiety symptoms. In addition, techno-overload had a significant indirect effect through emotion-focused coping, β 
= .031, 95% CI [.011, .060], which accounted for 17.32% of the total effect of techno-overload on anxiety symptoms. No 
evidence for an indirect effect was found between techno-overload and anxiety symptoms through avoidance coping.

The total effect of work-home conflict on anxiety symptoms was significant, β = .207, 95% CI [.116, .307]. Work-
home conflict had a significant indirect effect through avoidance coping, β = .027, 95% CI [.001, .066] that accounted 
for 13.04% of the total effect of work-home conflict on anxiety symptoms. No evidence for an indirect effect was found 
between work-home conflict and anxiety symptoms through problem- or emotion-focused coping.

The total effect of techno-ease on anxiety symptoms was also significant, β = -.087, 95% CI [-.159, -.010], with 
indirect effects through problem-, β = -.043, 95% CI [-.079, -.016] and emotion-focused coping, β = .018, 95% CI [.003, 
.040] accounting for 49.43% and 20.69% of the total effect, respectively. No evidence for an indirect effect was found 
between techno-ease and anxiety symptoms through avoidance coping. The residual direct effects for techno-overload, 
β = .163, 95% CI [.069, .254], work-home conflict, β = .179, 95% CI [.089, .268] and techno-ease, β = -.078, 95% CI [-.150, 
-.007] on anxiety symptoms indicated partial mediation (Table 10).

Galvin, Evans, Nelson et al. 311

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Table 9

Regression Analyses For the HADS Anxiety and Depression Subscales

Anxiety Symptoms Depressive Symptoms

Predictor Statistic p value Statistic p value
Regression model Model 1 Model 2

R 2 = .169 < .001 R 2 = .166 < .001

D-W = 1.809 D-W = 1.987
VIF = 1.1 VIF = 1.1

Age β = -.100 < .001 β = -.118 .004

Sex (Female) β = .058 .124 β = -.019 .621

International student (Yes) β = .012 .726 β = .000 .001

Level of study (Masters) β = -.028 .464 β = -.034 .381

Employed (Yes) β = .030 .427 β =.049 .196

Relationship status (Single) β = -.028 .460 β =.008 .840

Techno-overload β = .187 < .001 β = .030 .517

Work-home conflict β = .201 < .001 β = .290 < .001

Techno-ease β = -.116 .011 β = -.122 .008

Techno-reliability β = .017 .714 β = -.025 .604

Techno-sociality β = .038 .346 β = -.043 .287

Pace of change β = -.006 .873 β = -.056 .143

Regression model Model 3 Model 4
R 2 = .171 < .001 R 2 = .145 < .001

D-W = 1.829 D-W = 1.956
VIF = 1.2 VIF = 1.3

Age β = -.047 .227 β = -.045 .265

Sex (Female) β = .089 .017 β = .059 .118

International student (Yes) β = .017 .640 β = -.007 .856

Level of study (Masters) β = -.052 .161 β = -.043 .250

Employed (Yes) β = .051 .160 β = .037 .324

Relationship status (Single) β = -.047 .211 β = -.015 .687

Problem focused coping β = -.290 < .001 β = -.312 < .001

Emotion-focused coping β = .221 < .001 β = .049 .279

Avoidance coping β = .327 < .001 β = .317 < .001

Note. β: standardised beta. D-W: Durbin-Watson value. VIF: Variance Inflation Factor value.

Coping as a Mediator Between Technostress Factors and Depressive Symptomatology
Multiple mediation analysis was performed to investigate coping style as a mediator between technostress factors and 
depressive symptoms (Table 10). The total effect of work-home conflict on depressive symptoms was significant, β = 
.317, 95% CI [.243, .390]. Work-home conflict had a significant indirect effect through avoidance coping, β = .034, 95% CI 
[.007, .066], which accounted for 10.73% of the total effect of work-home conflict on depressive symptoms. No evidence 
for an indirect effect was found between work-home conflict and depressive symptoms through problem-focused 
coping.

The total effect of techno-ease on depressive symptoms was significant, β = -.156, 95% CI [-.233, -.081], with an 
indirect effect through problem-focused coping, β = -.038, 95% CI [-.073, -.011] accounting for 24.36% of the total effect. 
No evidence for an indirect effect was found between techno-ease and depressive symptoms through avoidance coping. 
The residual direct effects for work-home conflict, β = .284, 95% CI [.211, .354] and techno-ease, β = -.131, 95% CI [-.204, 
-.055] on depressive symptoms indicated partial mediation (Table 10).

Technostress, Coping and Mental Health in Students 312

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


Table 10

Outcomes of Multiple Mediation Analyses (Bootstrapped 5000 Samples)

Total Effect Direct Effect Effect of 
IV on M

Effect of M 
on DV

Indirect Effect

Outcome Predictor Mediator β SE CI β SE CI β SE CI

Anxiety Techno-
overload

.179** .044 .080, .272 .163** .041 .069, .254

Problem-focused .111* -.252** -.034* .018 -.077, -.002
Avoidance coping .066 .264** .018 .017 -.015, .058
Emotion-focused .202** .232** .031* .012 .011, .060

Work-home 
conflict

.207** .043 .116, .307 .179** .041 .089, .268

Problem-focused -.131* -.244** .015 .017 -.024, .055
Avoidance coping .064 .251** .027* .017 .001, .066
Emotion-focused -.188** .277** -.014 .001 -.041, .007

Techno-ease -.087* .037 -.159, -.010 -.078* .035 -.150, -.007
Problem-focused .103* -.246** -.043* .016 -.079, -.016
Avoidance coping .026 .280** .016 .014 -.011, .049
Emotion-focused .041 .252** .018* .009 .003, .040

Depression Work-home 
conflict

.317** .036 .243, .390 .284** .035 .211, .354

Problem-focused -.074* -.244** -.001 .014 -.032, .028
Avoidance coping .097* .266** .034* .014 .007, .066

Techno-ease -.156** .037 -.233, -.081 -.131** .035 -.204, -.055
Problem-focused .084* -.244** -.038* .015 -.073, -.011
Avoidance coping .015 .298** .013 .014 -.013, .043

Note. β: standardised beta. SE: standard error. CI: bias corrected accelerated 95% confidence intervals.

D i s c u s s i o n
This study investigated the associations between techno-stressors, coping, and anxious and depressive symptoms in 
university students during an intensive period of technology usage. Universities across the globe had to adapt quickly to 
deliver their courses during the COVID-19 pandemic and it is anticipated that reliance on technology in HE will last for 
the foreseeable future (Bloomfield, 2020). Understanding how technostress translates into psychopathological outcomes 
in the student population is therefore important to support students in facing the heightened ICT challenges introduced 
by the pandemic.

The study found that work-home conflict was associated with greater anxiety and depressive symptoms. This has 
been found in previous research, which showed that greater work-home conflict exists when university work and 
personal life are integrated rather than separated (Adebayo, 2006; McCutcheon & Morrison, 2018). Stricter boundaries 
between technology, work, and personal life may allow students to mentally detach from their work and protect them 
against anxiety and depression.

A substantial body of research has investigated how workers cope with managing the boundaries between their 
work and home life, and how this relates to psychopathology (e.g., Bergs et al., 2018; McTernan et al., 2016). The results 
of the current study show a direct effect of work-home conflict on anxiety and depressive symptoms as well as an 
indirect effect through avoidance coping. Considering the specific context of the pandemic and lockdown, the use of 
avoidance coping to manage conflict between work/home life may have resulted in students closing themselves off 
and/or hiding into their ICT activities, which, in turn, increased their anxiety and depressive symptoms.

Previous research shows that dealing with the complexity of technology and/or the uncertainty that comes with 
constant changes, developments, and upgrades in ICT can lead to stress, anxiety, and depression (Dragano & Lunau, 
2020; Thomée, 2012). It is now more essential than ever that students renew their technical skills while dealing 
with the pressure of more complex systems and virtual learning environments. The findings of the present study 

Galvin, Evans, Nelson et al. 313

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


reveal a negative association between techno-ease and anxiety and depressive symptoms. Techno-ease had a protective 
direct effect on anxiety and depressive symptoms in addition to an indirect effect through problem-focused coping. 
Techno-ease and problem-focused resolution can be supported by institutions providing their students with accessible 
ICT services, training, and workshops, as well as clear online ICT instructions and resources.

Techno-overload was positively associated with anxiety and depressive symptoms, which is in line with previous 
research on general population samples (Gaudioso et al., 2017). The mediation analysis suggested that when techno-
overload is high, the indirect effect of problem-focused coping protected against anxiety, whereas the indirect effect of 
emotion-focused coping increased anxiety symptoms. This latter finding contradicts previous research, which suggests 
that emotion-focused resolution through social support, including chatting with friends/family online, translates into 
positive outcomes for wellbeing (Liu et al., 2018; Zhu et al., 2013). One explanation for our finding could be situational 
factors since access to support networks during the data collection period would likely have been through ICTs due to 
social restriction measures. Engaging in emotion-focused coping during this period could therefore have contributed to 
increased techno-overload, necessitated intensive screen time, and resulted in a bi-directional relationship between these 
variables that resulted in heightened student anxiety. This is supported by research on Facebook Addiction Disorder 
(FAD), which showed that individuals who received high levels of social support online were at risk for tendencies 
toward FAD and that this negatively influenced mental health (Brailovskaia et al., 2019). Furthermore, another aspect 
of ICT is that communication can occur via several channels simultaneously (e.g., webchats, mobiles, video calls, etc.), 
which can be mentally exhausting and potentially stressful since distractions and dual tasking are demanding on 
working memory (Nijboer et al., 2016). With this in mind, access to social support through ICT during a period in which 
reliance on ICT was already high may have contributed toward heightened anxiety symptoms in this sample. However, 
this is somewhat speculative given the cross-sectional nature of the current research, and longitudinal studies will be 
needed to confirm this hypothesis.

An interesting finding in the present study was that significantly higher levels of anxiety and depression were found 
in the UK sample compared to Italy and Greece. Higher levels of avoidance coping and lower levels of problem-focused 
coping were also found in the UK sample compared to the other countries, and work-home conflict was significantly 
higher in the UK compared to Italy. Techno-ease was significantly lower in the Italian sample compared to the other 
countries, and pace of change was significantly higher in Greece in comparison to Italy. Students in Italy reported 
significantly higher emotion-focused coping compared to the UK. These observed differences could be due to a wide va­
riety of factors, including individual differences in socio-cultural factors, pandemic specific responses within countries, 
or differences in the academic environment/demands among the participating countries. Although these differences 
between the countries are interesting, they should be interpreted with caution. We did not confirm measurement 
invariance on the COPE and HADS, limiting the conclusions that can be made regarding statistical differences on these 
variables. However, the instruments have previously been validated in the respective countries, which supports their 
use in a range of populations (Anastasiou et al., 2017; Coriale et al., 2012; Ferrandina et al., 2012) including students 
(Fradelos et al., 2019; Sagone & De Caroli, 2014). Further, more research is needed in order to specify the exact factors 
and underlying mechanisms that may account for these differences at a country-level.

The overall sample for the current study was relatively young (M = 21.58, SD = 4.29). Although this is reflective 
of the broader student population, it is difficult to generalise our findings to mature learners. Hauk et al. (2019) found 
that even though older people are more prone to techno-stressors, ageing is connected to development of coping skills 
that in turn help reduce negative outcomes of technostress. However, increased home/work conflict is more common 
for mature learners (Markle, 2015; van Rhijn et al., 2016), as these students often experience greater social and family 
responsibilities. Future research could therefore extend our paradigm to establish whether these relationships are also 
present in mature student samples. Another limitation is that the primary language of the study participants was not 
assessed. We worked on the assumption that students had sufficient proficiency in the language of the country in 
which they were studying. Although we did measure the status of international students in our design, which may have 
accounted for non-native speakers to some extent, this working assumption could have affected the results.

Finally, it should also be noted that technostress can act as an “enhancer” to one’s productivity (Hung et al., 
2015), therefore possibly giving some users the perception that while they are working faster and longer with their 
ICTs, they are also working more efficiently. It is possible that while technostress may have increased anxiety and 

Technostress, Coping and Mental Health in Students 314

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://www.psychopen.eu/


depressive symptoms in the students, perceived productivity could also have resulted in the experience of positive 
feelings, such as accomplishment, which may serve as a protective factor. Although we did include some positive effects 
of technology in our design (techno-sociality, techno-ease, techno-reliability), we did not account for other possible 
benefits of technology and acknowledge this as a further limitation of the study.

C o n c l u s i o n s
The current study investigated associations between technostress, coping, and anxiety/depressive symptoms in Europe­
an university students during disruption to the higher education sector caused by the COVID-19 pandemic. Further data 
and psychological interventions are needed to promote psychological health among students in the immediate future 
and also after the pandemic. The psychological consequences of the COVID-19 outbreak will unfortunately last. An 
understanding of how technostress translates through coping strategies into mental health outcomes can help student 
counselling centres target maladaptive coping strategies, thus providing appropriate support to students.

Funding: The authors have no funding to report.

Acknowledgments: Ethical approval was granted by the School of Psychology Research Ethics Committee at Newcastle University, UK (reference number: 

3393/2020).

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

R e f e r e n c e s

Adebayo, D. O. (2006). Workload, social support, and work-school conflict among Nigerian nontraditional students. Journal of Career 
Development, 33(2), 125–141. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894845306289674

Anastasiou, C. A., Yannakoulia, M., Kosmidis, M. H., Dardiotis, E., Hadjigeorgiou, G. M., Sakka, P., Arampatzi, X., Bougea, A., 
Labropoulos, I., & Scarmeas, N. (2017). Mediterranean diet and cognitive health: Initial results from the Hellenic Longitudinal 
Investigation of Ageing and Diet. PLoS One, 12(8), Article e0182048. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182048

Ayyagari, R., Grover, V., & Purvis, R. (2011). Technostress: Technological antecedents and implications. Management Information 
Systems Quarterly, 35(4), 831–858. https://doi.org/10.2307/41409963

Bergs, Y., Hoofs, H., Kant, I., Slangen, J. J. M., & Jansen, N. W. H. (2018). Work–family conflict and depressive complaints among 
Dutch employees: Examining reciprocal associations in a longitudinal study. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health, 
44(1), 69–79. https://doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.3658

Bloomfield, L. S. P. (2020). Global mapping of landscape fragmentation, human-animal interactions, and livelihood behaviors to 
prevent the next pandemic. Agriculture and Human Values, 37, 603–604. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10460-020-10104-x

Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: An individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation. 
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(6), 591–612. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613504116

Brailovskaia, J., Rohmann, E., Bierhoff, H. W., Schillack, H., & Margraf, J. (2019). The relationship between daily stress, social support 
and Facebook Addiction Disorder. Psychiatry Research, 276, 167–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.05.014

Brod, C. (1984). Technostress: The human cost of the computer revolution. Addison-Wesley.
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of 

Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2), 267–283. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation 

Modeling, 9(2), 233–255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5
Coriale, G., Bilotta, E., Leone, L., Cosimi, F., Porrari, R., De Rosa, F., & Ceccanti, M. (2012). Avoidance coping strategies, alexithymia 

and alcohol abuse: A mediation analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 37(11), 1224–1229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.05.018

Galvin, Evans, Nelson et al. 315

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://doi.org/10.1177/0894845306289674
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182048
https://doi.org/10.2307/41409963
https://doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.3658
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10460-020-10104-x
https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613504116
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.05.014
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.05.018
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Costantini, M., Musso, M., Viterbori, P., Bonci, F., Del Mastro, L., Garrone, O., Venturini, M., & Morasso, G. (1999). Detecting 
psychological distress in cancer patients: Validity of the Italian version of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Supportive 
Care in Cancer, 7, 121–127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s005200050241

DeLone, W. H., & McLean, E. R. (2003). The DeLone and McLean model of information systems success: A ten-year update. Journal of 
Management Information Systems, 19(4), 9–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/07421222.2003.11045748

Dragano, N., & Lunau, T. (2020). Technostress at work and mental health: Concepts and research results. Current Opinion in 
Psychiatry, 33(4), 407–413. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000613

Ferrandina, G., Mantegna, G., Petrillo, M., Fuoco, G., Venditti, L., Terzano, S., Moruzzi, C., Lorusso, D., Marcellusi, A., & Scambia, G. 
(2012). Quality of life and emotional distress in early stage and locally advanced cervical cancer patients: A prospective, 
longitudinal study. Gynecologic Oncology, 124(3), 389–394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygyno.2011.09.041

Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Coping as a mediator of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(3), 466–475. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.3.466

Fradelos, E. C., Kapsiocha, E., Tzavella, F., Kastanidou, S., Tsaras, K., Papagiannis, D., & Papathanasiou, I. V. (2019). Factors associated 
with psychological distress in university students and the relation to emotional intelligent and spirituality: A cross-sectional 
study. Materia Socio-Medica, 31(4), 262–267. https://doi.org/10.5455/msm.2019.31.262-267

Freire, C., Ferradás, M. D. M., Valle, A., Núñez, J. C., & Vallejo, G. (2016). Profiles of psychological well-being and coping strategies 
among university students. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article e1554. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01554

Gaudioso, F., Turel, O., & Galimberti, C. (2017). The mediating roles of strain facets and coping strategies in translating techno-
stressors into adverse job outcomes. Computers in Human Behavior, 69, 189–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.041

Hauk, N., Göritz, A. S., & Krumm, S. (2019). The mediating role of coping behavior on the age-technostress relationship: A 
longitudinal multilevel mediation model. PLoS One, 14(3), Article e0213349. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213349

Hung, W. H., Chen, K., & Lin, C. P. (2015). Does the proactive personality mitigate the adverse effect of technostress on productivity 
in the mobile environment? Telematics and Informatics, 32(1), 143–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2014.06.002

JASP Team. (2020). JASP (Version 0.14.1) [Computer software]. https://jasp-stats.org/
Jiang, P. J. J., Klein, G., & Carr, C. L. (2002). Measuring information system service quality: SERVQUAL from the other side. 

Management Information Systems Quarterly, 26(2), 145–166. https://doi.org/10.2307/4132324
Johns Hopkins University & Medicine. (2021). Johns Hopkins Coronavirus Resource Centre. https://coronavirus.jhu.edu/map.html
Kim, K., Ryu, E., Chon, M. Y., Yeun, E. J., Choi, S. Y., Seo, J. S., & Nam, B. W. (2006). Internet addiction in Korean adolescents and its 

relation to depression and suicidal ideation: A questionnaire survey. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 43(2), 185–192. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2005.02.005

Kreiner, G. E. (2006). Consequences of work-home segmentation or integration: A person-environment fit perspective. Journal of 
Organizational Behavior, 27(4), 485–507. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.386

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1991). The concept of coping. In A. Monat & R. S. Lazarus (Eds.), Stress and coping: An anthology (3rd ed., 

pp. 189–206). Columbia University Press.
Lee-Baggley, D., Preece, M., & DeLongis, A. (2005). Coping with interpersonal stress: Role of Big Five traits. Journal of Personality, 

73(5), 1141–1180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00345.x
Little, T. D. (1997). Mean and covariance structures (MACS) analyses of cross-cultural data: Practical and theoretical issues. 

Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32(1), 53–76. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3201_3
Liu, D., Wright, K. B., & Hu, B. (2018). A meta-analysis of social network site use and social support. Computers & Education, 127, 201–

213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2018.08.024
Markle, G. (2015). Factors influencing persistence among nontraditional university students. Adult Education Quarterly, 65(3), 267–285. 

https://doi.org/10.1177/0741713615583085
McCutcheon, J. M., & Morrison, M. A. (2018). It’s “like walking on broken glass”: Pan-Canadian reflections on work–family conflict 

from psychology women faculty and graduate students. Feminism & Psychology, 28(2), 231–252. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/0959353517739641

McTernan, W. P., Dollard, M. F., Tuckey, M. R., & Vandenberg, R. J. (2016). Enhanced co-worker social support in isolated work groups 
and its mitigating role on thework-family conflict-depression loss spiral. International Journal of Environmental Research and 
Public Health, 13(4), Article e382. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph13040382

Technostress, Coping and Mental Health in Students 316

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://doi.org/10.1007/s005200050241
https://doi.org/10.1080/07421222.2003.11045748
https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000613
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygyno.2011.09.041
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.3.466
https://doi.org/10.5455/msm.2019.31.262-267
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01554
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.041
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213349
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2014.06.002
https://jasp-stats.org/
https://doi.org/10.2307/4132324
https://coronavirus.jhu.edu/map.html
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2005.02.005
https://doi.org/10.1002/job.386
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00345.x
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3201_3
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2018.08.024
https://doi.org/10.1177/0741713615583085
https://doi.org/10.1177/0959353517739641
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph13040382
https://www.psychopen.eu/


Michopoulos, I., Douzenis, A., Kalkavoura, C., Christodoulou, C., Michalopoulou, P., Kalemi, G., Fineti, K., Patapis, P., Protopapas, K., 
& Lykouras, L. (2008). Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS): Validation in a Greek general hospital sample. Annals of 
General Psychiatry, 7, Article e4. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-859X-7-4

Moore, J. E. (2000). One road to turnover: An examination of work exhaustion in technology professionals. Management Information 
Systems Quarterly, 24(1), 141–168. https://doi.org/10.2307/3250982

Moore, G. C., & Benbasat, I. (1991). Development of an instrument to measure the perceptions of adopting an information technology 
innovation. Information Systems Research, 2(3), 192–222. https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.2.3.192

Netemeyer, R. G., Boles, J. S., & McMurrian, R. (1996). Development and validation of work-family conflict and family-work conflict 
scales. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(4), 400–410. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.81.4.400

Nijboer, M., Borst, J., van Rijn, H., & Taatgen, N. (2016). Contrasting single and multi-component working-memory systems in dual 
tasking. Cognitive Psychology, 86, 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.01.003

Roth, S., & Cohen, L. J. (1986). Approach, avoidance, and coping with stress. American Psychologist, 41(7), 813–819. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.7.813

Roussi, P. (2001). The adaptation of COPE scale. In E. Vassilaki, S. Triliva, & E. Bezevengis (Eds.), Coping with stress and anxiety. 
Ellinika Grammata.

Rutkowski, L., & Svetina, D. (2014). Assessing the hypothesis of measurement invariance in the context of large-scale international 
surveys. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 74(1), 31–57. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164413498257

Sagone, E., & De Caroli, M. E. (2014). A correlational study on dispositional resilience, psychological well-being, and coping strategies 
in university students. American Journal of Educational Research, 2(7), 463–471. https://doi.org/10.12691/education-2-7-5

Sahu, P. (2020). Closure of universities due to Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19): Impact on education and mental health of 
students and academic staff. Cureus, 12(4), Article e7541. https://doi.org/10.7759/cureus.7541

Sica, C., Magni, C., Ghisi, M., Altoè, G., Sighinolfi, C., Chiri, L. R., & Franceschini, S. (2008). Coping Orientation to Problems 
Experienced-Nuova Versione Italiana (COPE-NVI): Uno strumento per la misura degli stili di coping [Coping Orientation to 
Problems Experienced-New Italian Version (COPE-NVI): A tool for measuring coping styles]. Psicoterapia Cognitiva e 
Comportamentale, 14(1), 27–53. 

Taylor, S. E., & Stanton, A. L. (2007). Coping resources, coping processes, and mental health. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 
377–401. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091520

Thomée, S. (2012). ICT use and mental health in young adults. Effects of computer and mobile phone use on stress, sleep disturbances, and 
symptoms of depression [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Department of Public Health and Community Medicine, Institute of 
Medicine, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg.

van Rhijn, T. M., Lero, D., Bridge, K., & Fritz, V. (2016). Unmet needs: Challenges to success from the perspectives of mature university 
students. Canadian Journal for the Study of Adult Education, 28(1), 29–47. 

Vasilescu, M. D., Serban, A. C., Dimian, G. C., Aceleanu, M. I., & Picatoste, X. (2020). Digital divide, skills and perceptions on 
digitalisation in the European Union—Towards a smart labour market. PLoS One, 15(4), Article e0232032. 
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0232032

Wang, X., Tan, S. C., & Li, L. (2020). Technostress in university students’ technology-enhanced learning: An investigation from 
multidimensional person-environment misfit. Computers in Human Behavior, 105, Article e106208. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.106208

Watermeyer, R., Crick, T., Knight, C., & Goodall, J. (2021). COVID-19 and digital disruption in UK universities: Afflictions and 
affordances of emergency online migration. Higher Education, 81, 623–641. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10734-020-00561-y

Weiss, A. M., & Heide, J. B. (1993). The nature of organizational search in high technology markets. Journal of Marketing Research, 
30(2), 220–233. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224379303000207

Zhu, X., Woo, S. E., Porter, C., & Brzezinski, M. (2013). Pathways to happiness: From personality to social networks and perceived 
support. Social Networks, 35(3), 382–393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2013.04.005

Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67(6), 361–370. 
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

Galvin, Evans, Nelson et al. 317

Europe's Journal of Psychology
2022, Vol. 18(3), 302–318
https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.4725

https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-859X-7-4
https://doi.org/10.2307/3250982
https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.2.3.192
https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.81.4.400
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.01.003
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.7.813
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164413498257
https://doi.org/10.12691/education-2-7-5
https://doi.org/10.7759/cureus.7541
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091520
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0232032
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.106208
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10734-020-00561-y
https://doi.org/10.1177/002224379303000207
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2013.04.005
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x
https://www.psychopen.eu/


A b o u t  t h e  A u t h o r s
John Galvin, PhD, is a Senior Lecturer in the Department of Psychology, Birmingham City University, UK. His research interests 
include stress and wellbeing, autism and autistic traits, and general population mental health.

Michael Scott Evans, PhD, C.Psychol, AFBPsS, FRSA, FIIRSM, FInstLM works within the Contracts Management team at 
Transport for Wales leading the infrastructure contractual delivery of a £5 billion contract on behalf of the Welsh Government. Dr 
Michael Scott Evans is a Chartered Psychologist and Associate Fellow of the British Psychological Society. He is also a Fellow of the 
Institute of Leadership & Management (FInstLM), Fellow of the International Institute of Risk & Safety Management (FIIRSM), and 
Fellow of the Royal Society of Arts (FRSA).

Kenisha Nelson, PhD, is a full-time lecturer in the Faculty of Education and Liberal Studies (FELS) at the University of Technology, 
Jamaica and a research assistant with the None-in-Three Research Centre, Jamaica. Her research interests include topics on occupa­
tional health, stress and well-being, gender-based violence, and help seeking behaviours for mental health related problems.

Gareth Richards, PhD, is a Lecturer in the School of Psychology, Newcastle University. His research interests include evolutionary 
approaches to mind and behaviour, autism and autistic traits, testosterone, sex differences, and cerebral lateralisation and handedness.

Eirini Mavritsaki, PhD, is a Professor and Director of Research for the School of Social Sciences at Birmingham City University. 
Eirini has been working for 16 years in psychology and published extensively in reputable journals and books in the topics of cogni­
tion, neuropsychology and disorders, and cross-cultural differences. She has contributed to the International research community 
as Associate Editor in Frontiers in Psychology and Special issue editor in Frontiers in Psychology and Frontiers in Computational 
Neuroscience and a member of the Board of Directors of the Organisation for Computational Neuroscience (OCNS). Eirini was 
awarded by the British Psychological Society the Cognitive Section Award in 2012.

Theodoros Giovazolias, PsychD, is Professor of Counselling Psychology at the Department of Psychology, University of Crete, 
Greece. His research interests focus on parental and intimate acceptance-rejection and its correlates on the psychological adjustment 
of children and young adults. He also conducts research on students’ mental health. He is Honorary Editor-in-Chief of European 
Journal of Counselling Psychology and serves as Editorial member and ad-hoc reviewer in more than 30 international peer-reviewed 
journals.

Katerina Koutra, PhD, is Assistant Professor of Clinical Psychology at the Department of Psychology, University of Crete, 
Greece. Her research interests focus on intrafamilial relationships in severe mental disorders, psychosocial determinants of child 
neuropsychological and behavioural/emotional development from childhood to adolescence, and students’ mental health.

Ben Mellor, BSc, MSc, is a Higher Research Assistant within Cumbria, Northumberland, Tyne and Wear NHS Foundation Trust. 
He is a Researcher affiliated with the School of Psychology at Newcastle University with interests in the efficacy of mental health 
formulations, participatory research methodologies, assortative mating, autism and autistic traits.

Maria Clelia Zurlo, PhD, Full Professor and Head of the Dynamic Psychology Laboratory at University of Naples Federico II, Italy. 
Her research covers the areas of Health Psychology and focuses on the development and applications of models and tools for the 
evaluation of stress dimensions and psychological and physical health conditions with reference to a wide range of target populations 
i.e., students, teachers, immigrant workers, nurses, partners of infertile couples, clinical patients. She is the author of more than 70 
scientific publications in these areas of expertise, with several international co-authors.

Andrew Paul Smith, BSc, PhD, FBPsS, C.Psychol, FRSM is Professor of Psychology and Director, Centre for Occupational and 
Health Psychology, Cardiff University. He has been at Cardiff since 1999 and has conducted research on: occupational stress and 
fatigue; seafarers’ fatigue; driver fatigue; nutrition and behaviour; caffeine; chewing gum; well-being at work; and factors influencing 
children’s well-being and performance. He has published over 500 papers and given over 200 invited talks and conference papers.

Federica Vallone, PhD, Researcher at the University of Naples Federico II. She received her Doctorate in Human Mind and 
Gender Studies at University of Naples Federico II in collaboration with the Centre for Occupational and Health Psychology, Cardiff 
University. Her research covers the areas of Occupational and Health Psychology in students, teachers, nurses, and partners of 
infertile couples.

Technostress, Coping and Mental Health in Students 318

PsychOpen GOLD is a publishing service by
Leibniz Institute for Psychology (ZPID), Germany.
www.leibniz-psychology.org

https://www.leibniz-psychology.org/
https://www.psychopen.eu/

	Technostress, Coping and Mental Health in Students
	(Introduction)
	Method
	Procedure and Participants
	Materials
	Data Analysis

	Results
	Demographics
	EFA on the COPE Scale
	CFA and MGCFA on Technostress Scale
	Differences Between Countries on the Study Variables
	Pearson’s Correlations and Regression Analyses
	Coping as a Mediator Between Technostress Factors and Anxiety Symptomatology
	Coping as a Mediator Between Technostress Factors and Depressive Symptomatology

	Discussion
	Conclusions
	(Additional Information)
	Funding
	Acknowledgments
	Competing Interests

	References
	About the Authors