The Relationship Between Gratitude and Loneliness: The Potential Benefits of Gratitude for Promoting Social Bonds Research Reports The Relationship Between Gratitude and Loneliness: The Potential Benefits of Gratitude for Promoting Social Bonds Andrea Caputo*a [a] Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, University of Rome “Sapienza”, Rome, Italy. Abstract This paper explores the potential role of gratitude on the reduction of loneliness feelings, even controlling for several variables related to social desirability, well-being (subjective happiness and life satisfaction) and socio-demographic characteristics. Through a web-based survey a convenience sample of 197 participants completed an online questionnaire including these measures. Correlation analyses and four-step hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted. The results show a negative correlation between gratitude and loneliness; specifically, gratitude succeeds in accounting for up to almost one-fifth of the total variability of loneliness even controlling for further variables. Being female, not having a stable and consolidated relationship and not participating in the labor force represent some risk factors affecting loneliness which should be taken into account in further research. Keywords: gratitude, loneliness, social relationships, social desirability, well-being Europe's Journal of Psychology, 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334, doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 Received: 2014-06-17. Accepted: 2015-01-16. Published (VoR): 2015-05-29. Handling Editor: Izabela Lebuda, Academy of Special Education, Warsaw, Poland *Corresponding author at: Via Pescasseroli 6, 00177 Roma, Italy. E-mail: andrea.caputo@uniroma1.it This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Loneliness plays an important role in investigating the psychological process of human feelings and behaviors with regard to the formation and maintenance of social relationships (Wu & Yao, 2008). Loneliness is an emotionally unpleasant experience resulting from a discrepancy between the types of interpersonal relationships one wishes to have, and those that one perceives they presently have (Peplau & Perlman, 1982). It can be considered as a feeling of emptiness, isolation or unwanted solitude, clearly distinguishable from the objective state of solitude, social isolation, or being alone. Indeed, social contact does not necessarily buffer one against loneliness because the experience of loneliness seems to have more to do with individuals’ perceptions of the quality of social inter- actions (Asher & Paquette, 2003; Hawkley, Burleson, Berntson, & Cacioppo, 2003). Many studies (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010; Heinrich & Gullone, 2006) indicated that loneliness can negatively affect health, life satisfaction and well-being. In this regard, most of the recent research on subjective well-being has focused on the benefits of gratitude as a means to increase life satisfaction and positive affects and decrease negative ones from childhood to old age (Emmons & McCullough, 2003; Watkins, Woodward, Stone, & Kolts, 2003). Gratitude is recognized as “indispensable in the life of one individual who will face isolation and loneliness if the capacity to feel grateful is impaired” (Emmons & McCullough, 2004, p. 2010), because it motivates the re- Europe's Journal of Psychology ejop.psychopen.eu | 1841-0413 http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0 http://ejop.psychopen.eu/ http://ejop.psychopen.eu/ http://www.psychopen.eu/ ciprocation of aid (Bartlett & DeSteno, 2006; McCullough, Kilpatrick, Emmons, & Larson, 2001; Tsang, 2006). As stated by Melanie Klein (1963/1975) gratitude can mitigate loneliness because it deeply includes a very close connection between being able to accept and to give, and both are part of the relation with the good object. Indeed, loneliness relates to a deficiency of the needs of intimacy and meaning (McGraw, 1995). Expressing gratitude thus plays an important role in relationships because it can strengthen social bonds and friendships (Emmons & Shelton, 2002; McCullough & Tsang, 2004), as well as the characteristics needed for their development and maintenance (Algoe, Haidt, & Gable, 2008; Gordon, Arnette, & Smith, 2011; Kubacka, Finkenauer, Rusbult, & Keijsers, 2011; McCullough, Kimeldorf, & Cohen, 2008), such as peer, family and social support (Froh, Yurkewicz, & Kashdan, 2009), prosocial motivation (Michie, 2009; Naito, Wangwan, & Tani, 2005; Tsang, 2006), relationship connection and satisfaction (Algoe, Gable, & Maisel, 2010), willingness to forgive (DeShea, 2003), praise (Deutsch, Roksa, & Meeske, 2003) and trust (Dunn & Schweitzer, 2005). However, only few studies have specifically examined the potential role of gratitude for the reduction of loneliness feelings (Burcat, 2010; Feng, 2011). In addition, these studies show two main limitations: the specific type of samples selected (respectively, college students at a public California university in the study by Burcat and Korean American pastors and spouses in the study by Feng) and the correlation analyses conducted which did not take into account further confounding factors. Therefore, this paper aims at proposing a research study which consents to overcome these two limitations by extending findings to a wider range of population and using regression models to disentangle the relationship between gratitude and loneliness controlling for further variables. Theoretical Framework and Aim of the Study In this study loneliness is defined as an individual’s subjective experience of emptiness, isolation and lack of sat- isfying human relationships (Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2004; Victor, Scambler, Bond, & Bowling, 2000), thus causing negative feelings and distress. Loneliness “denotes the lack of intimate/meaningful solidarity with other beings and bespeaks an entitative-emotional longing for their plenitude and connectedness” (McGraw, 1995, p. 46). In this sense, gratitude is hypothesized to reduce the feeling of individual isolation from others res- ulting in insecurity and instability (McGraw, 1995), which impacts on how people interact, as well as how they in- terpret interpersonal situations (Murphy & Kupshik, 1992). The emotion of gratitude can influence the likelihood of their forming satisfying relationships, counteracting the distorted thinking which can lead to loneliness by causing deficits in sociability (Peplau & Perlman, 1982). By distorted thinking we mean irrational cognitions ac- cording to which, in explaining the causes of their loneliness, lonely people are likely to blame themselves, deriving uncontrollable, internal, and stable attributions (Solano, 1987). That is, they are likely to see their personalities as unchangeable (stable), view social situations as being beyond their control, and believe that they do not have friends because they are perhaps dull and boring (internal). This contributes to inappropriate patterns of self-dis- closure (Horowitz & French, 1979), less effective coping behavior and dysfunctional attitudes (e.g., fears of inter- personal rejection, feeling unsure of oneself, and social anxiety) (Wilbert & Rupert, 1986). Instead, grateful indi- viduals can have an expanded circle of attributions, because they attribute their success to others’ controllable actions (Weiner, 1985), and also take into account how they themselves have contributed to their own success (McCullough, Emmons, & Tsang, 2002). Therefore, the experience of gratitude may allow people to be more re- sponsive to others and take full advantage of available interpersonal opportunities (Jones, Hobbs, & Hockenbury, 1982), thus increasing self-perceived social competence and interrupting chronic loneliness which impede the future meeting of belongingness needs. Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 The Relationship Between Gratitude and Loneliness 324 http://www.psychopen.eu/ This study aims at investigating the potential role of gratitude on the reduction of loneliness feelings. In doing that, it takes into account some confounding factors which could interfere in this relationship, which mainly refer to social desirability bias and other positive emotions. On the one hand, social desirability may affect both gratitude and loneliness because it leads to report pro-social and altruistic dispositions which are culturally agreeable (Ly- ubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade, 2005; Wood, Froh, & Geraghty, 2010), as well as to hide undesirable feelings related to low emotional well-being (Lasgaard, Goossens, & Elklit, 2011). On the other hand, how much of the variance in loneliness is due to gratitude needs to be studied independently of how much it is due to well-being measures such as life satisfaction and subjective happiness. Method Participants and Procedure A web-based survey was promoted via social media (forums, blogs, social networks) to study the relationship between subjective well-being and other related psychological constructs. It was conducted according to online survey design, development and implementation guidelines suggested by Andrews, Nonnecke, and Preece (2003). Online survey was chosen because of its widespread use for quality of life, health-related and well-being research (Vereecken & Maes, 2006) and its easy access to geographically diverse respondent groups across the national context (Evans & Mathur, 2005). In addition, the validity and reliability of internet research for subjective well-being surveys were demonstrated to be comparable to those of the paper-based versions (Howell, Rodzon, Kurai, & Sanchez, 2010). A convenience sample of 197 participants was recruited (158 women and 39 men) whose mean age was 29.1 (SD = 10.4). A questionnaire was administered which included socio-demographic information and loneliness, gratitude, social desirability, subjective happiness and life satisfaction measures. Participants were guaranteed anonymity. For the present study 100% of the respondents filled in the complete questionnaire without missing data. Measures Gratitude — The Gratitude Questionnaire-Six-Item Form (GQ-6) (McCullough, Emmons, & Tsang, 2002) is a six- item self-report questionnaire designed to assess individual differences in the proneness to experience expressions of gratefulness and appreciation in daily life, as well as feelings about receiving from others. Respondents endorsed each item on a 7-point Likert-type scale and the score was calculated as the sum of items, ranging from 6 to 42. Higher scores mean higher proneness to experience gratitude in daily life. For the purpose of this study, the scale was adapted to the Italian language through translation, back translation and equivalence evaluation, and showed a satisfactory internal consistency (α = .750). Loneliness — The Three-Item Loneliness Scale, developed by Hughes et al. (2004) from the revised UCLA Loneliness Scale, was used to assess loneliness consisting of feelings of isolation, disconnectedness, and not belonging. The response categories were coded 1 (hardly ever), 2 (some of the time), and 3 (often) on a 3-point scale. Each person’s responses to the questions are summed, with higher scores indicating greater loneliness. For the purpose of this study, the three items were derived from the Italian version of the revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Solano & Coda, 1994). The 3-item scale showed a good internal consistency (α = .839). Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 Caputo 325 http://www.psychopen.eu/ Social desirability — The Italian adaptation (Manganelli Rattazzi, Canova, & Marcorin, 2000) of the short 9-item version of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MC-SDS) was used to measure social desirability. Participants were requested to respond to each item on a 7-point scale. A total score is derived from the sum of all items, ranging from 7 to 63. Higher scores indicate higher levels of social desirability. Internal consistency was sufficient (α = .611). The relatively low Cronbach’s alpha seems to be in agreement with other studies using the Italian short version of the MC-SDS (Maino & Aceti, 1997; Manganelli Rattazzi et al., 2000). Subjective happiness — The Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS) (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999) is a widely used four-item scale, measuring global subjective happiness. The scale required participants to use absolute ratings to characterize themselves as happy or unhappy individuals on a 7-point Likert scale, as well as it asked to what extent they identify themselves with description of happy and unhappy individuals. The score was calculated as the mean of items, ranging from 1 to 7. Higher scores mean greater perceived happiness. The Italian version of the scale was used (Duncan & Grazzani-Gavazzi, 2004) and showed a Cronbach’s alpha of .847. Life satisfaction — As a measure of global life satisfaction, a three-item scale was specifically developed and used for the purpose of the study. Subjects had to rate how much they were satisfied with three dimensions re- spectively regarding socio-economic status, general health status, and life style and conditions, using a 10-point agreement scale. The score was calculated as the sum of items, ranging from 3 to 30. Higher scores mean better life satisfaction. The scale showed good psychometric properties with a Cronbach’s alpha of .764. Statistical Procedures To explore the relationships among the different measures considered (gratitude, loneliness, social desirability, subjective happiness, life satisfaction), correlation analyses were performed. With regard to our research question, four-step hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted by using loneliness measure as dependent variable. The Model 1 includes only gratitude measure as explicative variable in order to test whether gratitude can explain variance of loneliness. Then other variables were progressively entered in the regression models in order to estimate the predictive value of gratitude in reducing loneliness, even controlling for several additional characteristics which refer to: social desirability bias (Model 2), well-being measures such as subjective happiness and life satisfaction (Model 3) and socio-demographic variables (Model 4). With regard to socio-demographic predictors, a dummy for gender (male, female) and three dummies respectively for marital/relationship status (married/cohabitant, in a relationship, single) and employment status (employed, unemployed, others) were created. Instead, age and education were inserted as continuous variables. All analyses were performed using SPSS 16.0. Results In Table 1 socio-demographic variables of our sample are reported, as well as descriptive characteristics of used measures. Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 The Relationship Between Gratitude and Loneliness 326 http://www.psychopen.eu/ Table 1 Socio-Demographic Variables and Descriptive Characteristics of Subjective Well-Being Measures of the Sample (N = 197) Social DesirabilityGratitudeLonelinessLife Satisfaction Subjective HappinessSocio-demographic variables SDMSDMSDMSDMSDM%nCategorical variables Gender (6.92)39Male .227.6438.5924.801.974.764.9718.161.014.819 158Female .667.5937.417.5425.092.735.165.5418.041.873.280 Marital/Relationship status 56Married-Cohabitant .208.2039.407.7027.781.544.974.4819.041.224.428 62In a relationship .796.2137.356.0824.981.745.564.9018.930.893.531 79Single .657.2837.797.1425.022.206.465.8117.131.683.140 Employment status 85Employed .867.2037.996.5126.861.095.784.2519.950.084.143 33Unemployed .677.1836.057.7321.232.795.545.7915.071.483.816 79Othersa .087.1636.077.6325.082.036.854.1519.131.883.140 SDMContinuous variables -----Age .4010.1029 -----Education (years) .523.4513 Total .577.8037.317.3625.052.585.085.6318.061.903 aOthers include homemakers, students or retired. As shown in Table 2, the negative correlation between loneliness and gratitude is confirmed and the other measures are associated accordingly with the theoretical framework: loneliness is negatively correlated with subjective happiness, life satisfaction and social desirability; while gratitude shows a positive association with them. However, the correlations are not so high to cause potential multicollinearity problems in regression analyses, thus indicating the presence of separate constructs. Table 2 Correlations for all Measures Social DesirabilityGratitudeLonelinessLife satisfaction Subjective Happiness 1Subjective Happiness 1Life satisfaction .601*** 1Loneliness .481***-.557***- 1Gratitude .438***-.463***.483*** 1Social Desirability .316***.253***-.235***.168** **p < .01. ***p < .001. The results of hierarchical multiple regression analyses (Table 3) indicate that gratitude can be considered as a valid predictor in all the regression models, accounting for up to 18.7% of the total variance of loneliness. Gratitude thus seems to contribute to reduce loneliness feelings. When entering additional controls, its coefficient tends to decrease because of its relationship with the other explicative variables, despite it still remains statistically signi- ficant. In the Model 2, social desirability does not show an incremental validity in explaining loneliness. Instead, in the Model 3, the well-being controls increase the overall explained variance thus suggesting the relevance of Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 Caputo 327 http://www.psychopen.eu/ individual positive emotional states in accounting loneliness. Then, in the Model 4 (explaining 44.4% of loneliness) the results about socio-demographic characteristics show that being male and married or cohabitant predict lower loneliness, while homemakers, students or retired (in terms of occupational status) generally feel more lonely. In other words, this means that being female, not having a stable and consolidated relationship and not participating in the labor force represent some risk factors affecting loneliness which should be taken into account. Table 3 Summary of Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analyses for Predicting Loneliness (Standardized Coefficients) Model 4Model 3Model 2Model 1 Constant .051***12.747***11.722***9.699***8 Gratitude .176**-0.150*-0.397***-0.438***-0 Social desirability .054-0.105-0.128-0 Subjective happiness .312***-0.367***-0 Life satisfaction .186*-0.166*-0 Gender (Male) .122*-0 Age .087-0 Education .063-0 Marital/Relationship status (ref: Single) Married-Cohabitant .201**-0 In a relationship .082-0 Employment status (ref: Unemployed) Employed .1430 Others .186*0 R .6890.6140.4540.4380 R2 (Adjusted) .444***0.364***0.198***0.187***0 ∆R2 .098**0.171***0.0150.192***0 *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Discussion The study results highlight that loneliness and gratitude are negatively correlated, consistently with previous research (Burcat, 2010; Feng, 2011). In addition, gratitude is demonstrated to be associated with increase in happiness, life satisfaction and social desirability; differently from loneliness which is generally related to negative outcomes in these measures. With regard to our research aim, gratitude seems to work as an influential moderator of loneliness feelings, accounting for up to almost one-fifth of its variability, thus suggesting the potential benefits of gratitude for promoting social bonds. Some gender differences in predicting loneliness emerge which indicate that women are more vulnerable to loneliness than men (Brackin, 2002; Briscoe, 2005; Victor, Scambler, Bowling, & Bond, 2005). Gilligan (1982) suggested that women may develop a way of thinking about the world that depends on a sense of being connected with others. Thus, women feel that to be alone is to be a failure. Men, by contrast, value independence, and believe that it is unmanly to need another to assuage loneliness. However, previous research findings have been quite ambiguous and controversial in this regard (Mahon, Yarcheski, Yarcheski, Cannella, & Hanks, 2006). Borys and Perlman (1985) found differentiated results of gender differences in loneliness using different measures. In detail, Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 The Relationship Between Gratitude and Loneliness 328 http://www.psychopen.eu/ they highlighted that when loneliness is measured using the direct self-labeling measurement (e.g., “do you often feel lonely?”) as in our study, females reported higher level of loneliness. Besides, higher levels of loneliness are perceived in people who are single than in married/cohabitant ones, consistently with recent literature (Drennan et al., 2008; Ernst & Cacioppo, 1999; Findlay, 2003; Victor et al., 2005). Indeed, data from respondents in more than 20 nations document that loneliness is less common among married than non-married individuals (Perlman & Peplau, 1998). Being married is considered as a protective factor in the experience of loneliness (Lauder, Sharkey, & Mummery, 2004), probably because of the socially integrating function of a stable partner (Dykstra, 1995). The presence of a partner facilitates social interaction and many activities are undertaken as a couple, with other couples; while the absence of an important relationship or the lost connection with a loved one can lead to loneliness (Mcinnis, 2000). Then, loneliness mainly characterizes homemakers, students or retired than other social categories probably because they do not participate in the labor force and may have more restricted social networks. Indeed, college students are more lonely than older adults (Rokach, 2000), homemakers may build fewer nonfamily social contacts due to the lack of opportunities to build such contacts through their employment (Owens & Swensen, 2000), and retired people may be characterized by reduced mobility as a precondition for seeking out contact partners (Dawson, Hendershot, & Fulton, 1987) and facilitating the establishment and maintenance of social contacts. This notwithstanding, the comparison between these categories and unemployed people (who don’t report higher loneliness in our study) should be further examined in future research in order to better understand the specific factors explaining these differences. Some limitations regarding this study need to be taken into account in order to put the findings into perspective. At first, this study used a convenience sample composed of internet users which was not a national representation and was not randomly chosen. Issues regarding self-selection bias may thus exist which do not allow generalization. In addition, internet users may be characterized by increased loneliness and a reduction in both the number of friends people have and the time they spend with them (Kraut et al., 1998). However, other studies found no such correlations (Katz & Aspden, 1997) or achieved opposite conclusions (Coget, Yamauchi, & Suman, 2002). Another limitation refers to the low generalizability of findings due to the small number of male respondents in the sample. However, the inclusion of gender as covariate in the regression analysis helped adjust estimates for this gender imbalance. Then, the nature of this research does not enable conclusions on causal relations between examined variables, because correlation analyses and self-report measures also have inherent limitations. Another limitation concerns the causal relationship between gratitude and loneliness. In this regard, loneliness might also be con- sidered as a moderating factor that can facilitate (or not) the opportunity to build social bond and the consequent feeling of gratitude. However, longitudinal data would be required to disentangle the pattern of these causal effects. In addition, experimental research could be carried out where participants could be induced to engage in various amounts of gratitude to examine what effects this would have on their feelings of loneliness. Funding The author has no funding to report. Competing Interests The author has declared that no competing interests exist. Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 Caputo 329 http://www.psychopen.eu/ Acknowledgments The author has no support to report. References Algoe, S. B., Gable, S. L., & Maisel, N. C. (2010). It’s the little things: Everyday gratitude as a booster shot for romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 17, 217-233. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01273.x Algoe, S. B., Haidt, J., & Gable, S. L. (2008). Beyond reciprocity: Gratitude and relationships in everyday life. Emotion, 8, 425-429. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.425 Andrews, D., Nonnecke, B., & Preece, J. (2003). Electronic survey methodology: A case study in reaching hard-to-involve Internet users. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 16(2), 185-210. doi:10.1207/S15327590IJHC1602_04 Asher, S. R., & Paquette, J. A. (2003). Loneliness and peer relations in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 75-78. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01233 Bartlett, M. Y., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behavior: Helping when it costs you. Psychological Science, 17, 319-325. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01705.x Borys, S., & Perlman, D. (1985). Gender differences in loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 63-74. doi:10.1177/0146167285111006 Brackin, L. A. (2002). Loneliness, depression, social support, marital satisfaction and spirituality as experienced by the Southern Baptist clergy wife. Dissertation Abstracts International: B. The Sciences and Engineering, 62(9), 4270. Briscoe, J. (2005). Renewal on the run. Birmingham, AL: New Hope. Burcat, E. (2010, August). Do grateful people feel less lonely? Paper presented at the 2010 Berkeley McNair Symposium, Berkeley, CA. Abstract retrieved from http://www.learningace.com/doc/314422/fc69ddafe8d4f5bcb53d3013ab1474fe/2010_mcnair_symposium_guide Coget, J.-F., Yamauchi, Y., & Suman, M. (2002). The Internet, social networks and loneliness. IT & Society, 1(1), 180-201. Dawson, D., Hendershot, G., & Fulton, J. (1987). Aging in the eighties: Functional limitations of individuals aged 65 years and over. Advance data, 133, 1-12. DeShea, L. (2003). A scenario-based scale of willingness to forgive. Individual Differences Research, 1, 201-217. Deutsch, F. M., Roksa, J., & Meeske, C. (2003). How gender counts when couples count their money. Sex Roles, 48, 291-304. doi:10.1023/A:1022982328840 Drennan, J., Treacy, M., Butler, M., Byrne, A., Fealy, G., Frazer, K., & Irving, K. (2008). The experience of social and emotional loneliness among older people in Ireland. Ageing and Society, 28(8), 1113-1132. doi:10.1017/S0144686X08007526 Duncan, E., & Grazzani-Gavazzi, I. (2004). Positive emotional experiences in Scottish and Italian young adults: A diary study. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5, 359-384. doi:10.1007/s10902-004-0666-8 Dunn, J. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing: The influence of emotion on trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 736-748. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.736 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 The Relationship Between Gratitude and Loneliness 330 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01273.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.425 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327590IJHC1602_04 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.01233 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01705.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167285111006 http://www.learningace.com/doc/314422/fc69ddafe8d4f5bcb53d3013ab1474fe/2010_mcnair_symposium_guide http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1022982328840 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X08007526 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-004-0666-8 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.736 http://www.psychopen.eu/ Dykstra, P. A. (1995). Loneliness among the never and formerly married: The importance of supportive friendships and a desire for independence. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 50B, S321-S329. doi:10.1093/geronb/50B.5.S321 Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 377-389. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377 Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2004). The psychology of gratitude. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Emmons, R. A., & Shelton, C. M. (2002). Gratitude and the science of positive psychology. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 459-471). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. Ernst, J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1999). Lonely hearts: Psychological perspectives on loneliness. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 8, 1-22. doi:10.1016/S0962-1849(99)80008-0 Evans, J., & Mathur, A. (2005). The value of on-line surveys. Internet Research, 15(2), 195-219. doi:10.1108/10662240510590360 Feng, E. (2011). The impact of spiritual well-being, gratitude, and loneliness on marital satisfaction among Korean American pastors and spouses (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database (UMI No. 1499156) Findlay, R. A. (2003). Interventions to reduce social isolation among older people: Where is the evidence? Ageing and Society, 23, 647-658. doi:10.1017/S0144686X03001296 Froh, J. J., Yurkewicz, C., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Gratitude and subjective well-being in early adolescence: Examining gender differences. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 633-650. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.06.006 Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Gordon, C. L., Arnette, R. A. M., & Smith, R. E. (2011). Have you thanked your spouse today? Felt and expressed gratitude among married couples. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 339-343. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.012 Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 40, 218-227. doi:10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8 Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Loneliness in everyday life: Cardiovascular activity, psychosocial context, and health behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 105-120. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.105 Heinrich, L. M., & Gullone, E. (2006). The clinical significance of loneliness: A literature review. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 695-718. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.04.002 Horowitz, L. M., & French, R. D. (1979). Interpersonal problems of people who describe themselves as lonely. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 762-764. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.47.4.762 Howell, R. T., Rodzon, K. S., Kurai, M., & Sanchez, A. H. (2010). A validation of well-being and happiness surveys for administration via the Internet. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 775-784. doi:10.3758/BRM.42.3.775 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 Caputo 331 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/50B.5.S321 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0962-1849(99)80008-0 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/10662240510590360 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X03001296 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.06.006 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.012 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.105 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.04.002 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.47.4.762 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.3.775 http://www.psychopen.eu/ Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: Results from two population-based studies. Research on Aging, 26, 655-672. doi:10.1177/0164027504268574 Jones, W. H., Hobbs, S. A., & Hockenbury, D. (1982). Loneliness and social skill deficits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 682-689. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.42.4.682 Katz, J. E., & Aspden, P. (1997). A nation of strangers? Communications of the ACM, 40(12), 81-86. doi:10.1145/265563.265575 Klein, M. (1975). On the sense of loneliness. In R. Money-Kyrle (Ed.), Envy and gratitude and other works (pp. 300-313). London, United Kingdom: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1963) Kraut, R., Patterson, M., Lundmark, V., Kiesler, S., Mukophadhyay, T., & Scherlis, W. (1998). Internet paradox: A social technology that reduces social involvement and psychological well-being? The American Psychologist, 53(9), 1017-1031. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.53.9.1017 Kubacka, K. E., Finkenauer, C., Rusbult, C. E., & Keijsers, L. (2011). Maintaining close relationships: Gratitude as a motivator and a detector of maintenance behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1362-1375. doi:10.1177/0146167211412196 Lasgaard, M., Goossens, L., & Elklit, A. (2011). Loneliness, depressive symptomatology, and suicide ideation in adolescence: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 137-150. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9442-x Lauder, W., Sharkey, S., & Mummery, K. (2004). A community survey of loneliness. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 46, 88-94. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2003.02968.x Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46, 137-155. doi:10.1023/A:1006824100041 Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9, 111-131. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111 Mahon, N. E., Yarcheski, A., Yarcheski, T. J., Cannella, B. L., & Hanks, M. M. (2006). A meta-analytic study of predictors for loneliness during adolescence. Nursing Research, 55(5), 308-315. doi:10.1097/00006199-200609000-00003 Maino, E., & Aceti, G. (1997). Contributo all’adattamento italiano della Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale [Contribution to the Italian adaptation of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale]. TPM – Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 4(2), 81-93. Manganelli Rattazzi, A. M., Canova, L., & Marcorin, R. (2000). La desiderabilità sociale: Un’analisi di forme brevi della scala di Marlowe e Crowne [Social desirability: An analysis of short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale]. TPM – Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 7(1), 5-17. McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J. (2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 112-127. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.112 McCullough, M. E., Kilpatrick, S. D., Emmons, R. A., & Larson, D. B. (2001). Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 249-266. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.249 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 The Relationship Between Gratitude and Loneliness 332 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0164027504268574 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.4.682 http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/265563.265575 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.9.1017 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211412196 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9442-x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2003.02968.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1006824100041 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111 http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006199-200609000-00003 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.112 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.249 http://www.psychopen.eu/ McCullough, M. E., Kimeldorf, M. B., & Cohen, A. D. (2008). An adaptation for altruism? The social causes, social effects, and social evolution of gratitude. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 281-285. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00590.x McCullough, M. E., & Tsang, J. (2004). Parent of the virtues? The prosocial contours of gratitude. In R. A. Emmons & M. E. McCullough (Eds.), The psychology of gratitude (pp. 123-141). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. McGraw, J. G. (1995). Loneliness, its nature and forms: An existential perspective. Man and World, 28(1), 43-64. doi:10.1007/BF01278458 Mcinnis, G. (2000). A phenomenological exploration of loneliness in the older adult. Dissertation Abstracts: Section B. The Sciences and Engineering, 61(4), 1873. Michie, S. (2009). Pride and gratitude: How positive emotions influence the prosocial behaviors of organizational leaders. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 15, 393-403. doi:10.1177/1548051809333338 Murphy, P. M., & Kupshik, G. A. (1992). Loneliness, stress and well-being: A helper’s guide. London, United Kingdom: Routledge. Naito, T., Wangwan, J., & Tani, M. (2005). Gratitude in university students in Japan and Thailand. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 247-263. doi:10.1177/0022022104272904 Owens, S. J., & Swensen, C. H. (2000, August). Loneliness in older adult spousal caregivers. Paper presented at the 108th Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Peplau, L., & Perlman, D. (1982). Perspectives on loneliness. In L. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 1-20). New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons. Perlman, D., & Peplau, L. A. (1998). Loneliness. In H. Friedman (Ed.), Encyclopedia of mental health (Vol. 2, pp. 571-581). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Rokach, A. (2000). Loneliness and the life cycle. Psychological Reports, 86(2), 629-642. doi:10.2466/pr0.2000.86.2.629 Solano, C. H. (1987). Loneliness and perceptions of control: General traits versus specific attributions. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2(2, Pt 2), 201-214. Solano, L., & Coda, R. (1994). Relazioni, emozioni, salute: Introduzione alla psicoimmunologia [Relationships, emotions and health: Introduction to psychoimmunology]. Padua, Italy: Piccin. Tsang, J. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behaviour: An experimental test of gratitude. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 138-148. doi:10.1080/02699930500172341 Vereecken, C. A., & Maes, L. (2006). Comparison of a computer administered and paper-and pencil-administered questionnaire on health and lifestyle behaviors. The Journal of Adolescent Health, 38(4), 426-432. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.10.010 Victor, C., Scambler, S., Bond, J., & Bowling, A. (2000). Being alone in later life: Loneliness, social isolation and living alone. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 10, 407-417. doi:10.1017/S0959259800104101 Victor, C. R., Scambler, S. J., Bowling, A., & Bond, J. (2005). The prevalence of, and risk factors for, loneliness in later life: A survey of older people in Great Britain. Ageing and Society, 25, 357-375. doi:10.1017/S0144686X04003332 Watkins, P. C., Woodward, K., Stone, T., & Kolts, R. L. (2003). Gratitude and happiness: Development of a measure of gratitude, and relationships with subjective well-being. Social Behavior and Personality, 31, 431-451. doi:10.2224/sbp.2003.31.5.431 Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 Caputo 333 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00590.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01278458 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1548051809333338 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022104272904 http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2000.86.2.629 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930500172341 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.10.010 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0959259800104101 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X04003332 http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2003.31.5.431 http://www.psychopen.eu/ Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548-573. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548 Wilbert, J. R., & Rupert, P. A. (1986). Dysfunctional attitudes, loneliness, and depression in college students. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 71-77. doi:10.1007/BF01173384 Wood, A. M., Froh, J. J., & Geraghty, A. W. A. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 890-905. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005 Wu, C.-h., & Yao, G. (2008). Psychometric analysis of the short-form UCLA loneliness scale (ULS-8) in Taiwanese undergraduate students. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1762-1771. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.02.003 About the Author Andrea Caputo is a clinical psychologist, psychotherapist, specialist in Health Psychology. He is currently a Ph.D. student at the Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, University of Rome “Sapienza”, Italy. His research interests have mainly been focusing on the individual-context relationship, health and quality of life, cultural studies and techniques for text analysis based on social representations theory, in both clinical and psychosocial field. PsychOpen is a publishing service by Leibniz Institute for Psychology Information (ZPID), Trier, Germany. www.zpid.de/en Europe's Journal of Psychology 2015, Vol. 11(2), 323–334 doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i2.826 The Relationship Between Gratitude and Loneliness 334 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01173384 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.02.003 http://www.psychopen.eu/ http://www.zpid.de/en The Relationship Between Gratitude and Loneliness (Introduction) Theoretical Framework and Aim of the Study Method Participants and Procedure Measures Statistical Procedures Results Discussion (Additional Information) Funding Competing Interests Acknowledgments References About the Author