CodePen - Strohmaier et al


    
    

     Frontline Learning Research Vol.8 No. 1 (2020) 16
      - 32 

      ISSN 2295-3159 

     A comparison of self-reports and electrodermal activity as
        indicators of mathematics state anxiety. An application of the
        control-value theory 

     Anselm R. Strohmaiera, Anja
        Schiepe-Tiskab & Kristina M. Reiss a b 
      

     aHeinz Nixdorf Chair of
      Mathematics Education, TUM School of Education, Technical
      University of Munich, Munich, Germany 

      bCentre for International Student Assessment, Technical
      University of Munich, Munich, Germany 

     Article received 11 November 2018 / revised 13
        December 2019/ accepted 24 January 2020 / available online 19
        February 

     Abstract 

     Abstract: In the present study with 86
        undergraduate students, we related trait Mathematics Anxiety
        (MA) with two indicators of state anxiety: self-reported state
        anxiety and electrodermal activity (EDA). Extending existing
        research, we included appraisals of control and perceived value
        in hierarchical multiple regression analyses in accordance with
        the control-value theory of achievement emotions (Pekrun, 2006).
        Results showed that trait MA predicted self-reported state
        anxiety, while no additional variance was explained by including
        control and value. In contrast, we found no significant relation
        between trait MA and physiological state anxiety, but a
        significant, negative three-way interaction effect with control
        and value. Regression coefficients indicated that trait MA
        predicted physiological state anxiety, but only in the presence
        of negative perceived control and positive perceived value.
        Thus, our results support the control-value theory for
        physiological state anxiety, but not for self-reports. They
        emphasize the need to distinguish between trait and state MA,
        the advantages of adopting the control-value theory, and the
        benefits of using EDA recording as a supplemental assessment
        method for state anxiety. 

     Keywords: mathematics anxiety,
      electrodermal activity, galvanic skin response, control-value
      theory, state anxiety. 

     Info corresponding author  anselm.strohmaier@tum.de 
      DOI  10.14786/flr.v8i1.427 
    

     

     1. Introduction 

     Mathematics Anxiety (MA) has a substantial impact on many
      students’ academic and personal lives. It influences achievement
      in mathematics tests and classes (Hembree, 1990; Ma, 1999;
      Namkung, Peng, & Lin, 2019). Moreover, students with high MA
      avoid mathematics in everyday life as well as in career and
      academic choices (Dowker, Sarkar, & Looi, 2016; Ma, 1999). MA
      is common across countries, cultures, and ages (Dowker et al.,
      2016; Lee, 2009). In the 2012 study of the Programme for
      International Student Assessment (PISA), 30% of students reported
      that they felt helpless when doing a mathematic problem (OECD,
      2013b). At the same time, MA is a problem of increasing relevance.
      On average across OECD countries, MA increased significantly from
      PISA 2003 to PISA 2012 (OECD, 2013b). Thus, for educational
      research, it is important to understand how MA affects students
      when doing mathematics. Research has elaborated the distinction
      between (momentary) state anxiety (MAstate) and (habitual) trait
      Mathematics Anxiety (MAtrait), assessed through separate
      self-reports, but the findings left their relationship ambiguous
      (Goetz, Bieg, Lüdtke, Pekrun, & Hall, 2013). Hence, merely
      assessing MAtrait cannot exhaustively explain how MA affects
      mathematical activities momentarily. Then again, directly
      assessing MAstate provides a challenge, because self-reports of
      state emotions might be unreliable (Pekrun & Bühner, 2014).
      Among other physiological measures, electrodermal activity (EDA;
      also referred to as galvanic skin response; GSR) had sporadically
      been used as an indicator for MAstate in the 1980s, but its
      relationship with self-reports of MAstate or to MAtrait remained
      unclear. In this paper, we addressed this research gap by
      combining two novel approaches. First, we included and compared
      both self-reports and EDA as measures of MAstate. Second, we used
      the control-value theory of achievement emotions (Pekrun, 2006) as
      a framework to test their relation to MAtrait. Accordingly, we
      included appraisals of control and perceived value as moderators
      of the relation between MAtrait and MAstate. 

     1.2 Mathematics Anxiety 

     MA “involves feelings of tension and anxiety that interfere with
      the manipulation of numbers and the solving of mathematical
      problems in a wide variety of ordinary life and academic
      situations” (Richardson & Suinn, 1972, p. 551). MA has an
      adverse effect on cognitive resources, independent of actual
      abilities (Ashcraft, 2007; Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001; Maloney et
      al., 2013). Ashcraft and Kirk (2001) found that in a mental
      addition task, undergraduates with high MA showed a smaller
      working memory capacity that led to an increase in reaction time
      and errors. This first finding started an intensive line of
      research, largely confirming direct effects of MA on performance
      (for overviews, see Dowker et al., 2016; Suárez-Pellicioni,
      Núñez-Peña, & Colomé, 2016). This influence is not limited to
      working memory capacity. For example, Maloney, Ansari, and
      Fugelsang (2011) found that high MA students suffer from low-level
      numerical deficits, like a less precise representation of
      numerical magnitude. Although most studies refer to MA as a
      unidimensional construct, a number of studies reported evidence
      that it consists of more than one factor, most prominently a
      cognitive component (“worry”) and an affective component
      (“emotionality”; e.g., Ho et al., 2000; Lukowski et al., 2016;
      Wigfield & Meece, 1988). These studies typically analyzed the
      factorial structure of questionnaires and related the dimensions
      to cognitive outcomes like mathematical achievement (e.g.,
      Ashcraft & Ridley, 2005; Lukowski et al., 2016). 

     1.3 Trait and state Mathematics Anxiety 

     While there are a large number of studies on MA, very few of
      them differentiate between MAstate and MAtrait (Goetz, Bieg,
      Lüdtke, Pekrun, & Hall, 2013; Goldin, 2014). However, this
      distinction arguably is important when focusing on the effects of
      MA during mathematical activities. Self-reports of MAtrait refer
      to multiple, generalized mathematical situations (Bieg, Goetz,
      Wolter, & Hall, 2015). In contrast, MAstate refers to the
      specific, current situation. Therefore, reports of MAtrait might
      be a good predictor for long-term effects of MA on learning or
      career and course choices (Dowker et al., 2016) but do not
      necessarily accurately predict MAstate during specific
      mathematical activities like tests or classes. When investigating
      the effects of MA during such activities, directly addressing
      MAstate seems to be more appropriate. 

     Studies investigating the role of emotions in mathematics and of
      MA in particular predominantly focus on trait emotions rather than
      state emotions (Goetz et al, 2013; Goldin, 2014). Accordingly, an
      extensive number of findings have been gathered on effects,
      individual differences, and precursors of MAtrait (Dowker et al.,
      2016). In contrast, there are fewer studies on MAstate, often
      using qualitative analyses (Goldin, 2014). Yet, specific
      mechanisms explaining the impact of MAstate have rarely been
      reported for mathematics (Dowker, 2016). 

     The relationship between MAstate and MAtrait is ambiguous. On
      the one hand, a number of studies indicate a strong positive
      relation. For high MAtrait students, MAstate is considered a key
      explanation for a lower working memory capacity (Ashcraft &
      Moore, 2009; Beilock, 2008). In his meta-analysis, Hembree (1990)
      reports a mean correlation of r = .42 between MAtrait and state
      anxiety. However, state anxiety was not necessarily assessed
      during specific mathematical activities in the four reported
      studies (e.g. Plake & Parker, 1982). On the other hand, some
      studies indicate that there is a notable discrepancy between
      MAtrait and MAstate. Goetz et al. (2013) found that girls
      systematically report higher levels of MAtrait, but that this
      difference is not present in reports of MAstate during mathematics
      tests or classes. This difference between reports of MAtrait and
      MAstate is largely explained by individual beliefs and perceptions
      of competence (Bieg et al., 2015; Goetz et al., 2013). Another
      reason for differences between MAtrait and MAstate might be that
      MA negatively affects achievement in mathematics through long-term
      avoidance behavior, but not during mathematical activities per se
      (Dowker et al., 2016): To avoid aversive consequences, MAstate can
      even enhance motivation momentarily and lead to an increase in
      effort and strategy use during mathematics tests (Eysenck &
      Calvo, 1992; Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, 2007). This
      indicates that MAtrait does not necessarily induce MAstate. 

     In general, state anxiety can have various cognitive and
      motivational-affective effects on learning and performance.
      Zeidner (2014) lists 15 specific deficits in information
      processing during learning caused by anxiety, which are likely to
      be transferable to MAstate. This includes cognitive deficits in
      areas like information encoding, information storage and
      processing, and information retrieval and production. Moreover,
      state anxiety is associated with physiological reactions. However,
      this has not been described for MAstate in particular, but only
      for state anxiety in general. Per definition, state anxiety is a
      “transitory emotional state consisting of feelings of
      apprehension, nervousness, and physiological sequelae such as an
      increased heart rate or respiration” (Wiedemann, 2015, p. 808).
      Among other aspects, state anxiety is thus characterized by
      increased arousal and activation of the autonomic nervous system
      (Steimer, 2002; Wiedemann, 2015). Accordingly, state anxiety does
      not only cause cognitive deficits, but also a physiological
      reaction. 

     In sum, existing studies mostly focus on MAtrait, while its
      relation to MAstate is left ambiguous. Thus, to better understand
      how MA affects learning not only over a longer period of time but
      also momentarily, additional research is needed. This refers both
      to the question of the relation between MAtrait and MAstate, as
      well as to the mechanisms and precursors of MAstate in particular.
      In the following, we propose a theoretical framework for
      investigating these questions. 

     1.4 The control-value theory 

     The control-value theory of achievement emotions (Pekrun, 2006)
      characterizes predictors of achievement emotions, including state
      anxiety. It states that appraisals of control and the perceived
      subjective value of an achievement situation are the most proximal
      predictors of achievement emotions. A low appraisal of control and
      a simultaneous high perceived value of the task are key
      determinants of state anxiety. In contrast, trait emotions,
      environmental factors, or former achievement are considered distal
      factors and are assumed to have a mostly indirect effect on state
      emotions. According to the control-value theory, MAtrait should
      therefore predict MAstate mostly indirectly, in association with
      low appraisals of control and a high subjective value. Several
      empirical studies support aspects of the control-value theory in
      mathematics (e.g., Niculescu, Tempelaar, Dailey-Hebert, Segers,
      & Gijselaers, 2015). Frenzel, Pekrun, and Goetz (2007) found
      that MAtrait is associated with a pattern of low competence
      beliefs paired with high achievement values in mathematics.
      Extending the scope, research about attitudes and beliefs about
      competence in mathematics offers plenty of evidence supporting the
      control-value theory for other mathematical achievement emotions
      (for an overview, see Goldin et al., 2016). However, to our
      knowledge, no study implemented both MAtrait and MAstate as well
      as appraisals of control and perceived value in one model. 

     1.5 Assessing mathematics state anxiety 

     To assess MAstate, research has mostly focused on qualitative
      research (see Goldin, 2014, for an overview). These approaches
      included retrospective interviews and videotaping, but the
      reliability of these methods has been questioned (Goldin, 2014).
      Using a more quantitative approach, Goetz et al. (2013) proposed
      short self-reports that could be used both for measuring anxiety
      during tests as well as during classes. The advantage of
      self-reports is that they can be used conveniently for
      experience-sampling and might be more reliable than observations.
      However, self-reports about achievement emotions might disrupt the
      current activity (Goldin, 2014). Moreover, it is questionable if
      self-reports can reflect an accurate evaluation of current
      emotions. In general, self-reports can only cover aspects of
      emotions that a person is aware of, depend on the use of language,
      and are subject to systematic biases, e.g. social desirability
      (Pekrun & Bühner, 2014). 

     Consequently, other researchers have attempted to use
      physiological measures to directly investigate MA in performance
      situations (Dowker et al., 2016; Hannula, 2016), predominantly
      using neuropsychological methods (e.g., Lyons & Beilock, 2012;
      Pletzer, Kronbichler, Nuerk, & Kerschbaum, 2015). These
      studies revealed that MA activates brain areas linked to fear
      processing, disgust and pain processing, but they did not
      distinguish between MAtrait and MAstate (Artemenko, Daroczy,
      Nuerk, 2015; Suárez-Pellicioni et al., 2016). 

     State anxiety in general is associated with arousal and stress
      and with physiological reactions due to the activation of the
      autonomic nervous system. This leads to an increased heart rate
      and respiration, among other physiological reactions (Steimer,
      2002; Wiedemann, 2015). This also holds for state anxiety in the
      context of education (Zeidner, 2014). Therefore, these specific
      physiological reactions can be assumed to be an indicator for
      MAstate. Some studies have assessed heart rate or cortisol
      secretion to monitor stress levels during mathematical tests (Dew,
      Galassi, & Galassi, 1984; Faust 1992, as cited in Ashcraft,
      2002; Mattarella-Micke, Mateo, Kozak, Foster, & Beilock, 2011;
      Pletzer, Wood, Moeller, Nuerk, & Kerschbaum, 2010; Sarkar,
      Dowker, & Cohen Kadosh, 2014). These studies produced mixed
      results. Mattarella-Micke et al. (2011) showed that cortisol
      secretion can be associated with high performance (for low MA
      students) or with low performance (for high MA students), probably
      associated with a working memory overload. In contrast, Pletzer et
      al. (2010) did not find a correlation between cortisol secretion
      and reports of MA, but used self-reports of MAtrait, not MAstate.
      A relation between heart rate and state anxiety has been shown in
      various fields (e.g. Kantor, Endler, Heslegrave, & Kocovski,
      2001), but has rarely been used in mathematics. Faust (as cited in
      Ashcraft, 2002) reported changes in heart rate when a highly
      math-anxious group performed mathematics tests of increasing
      difficulty. In contrast, Dew, Galassi, and Galassi (1984) found no
      substantial relation between heart rate and MAtrait or MAstate. 

     In addition to heart rate, Dew, Galassi, and Galassi (1984)
      observed physiological arousal during a timed mathematics test
      assessing participants’ EDA. EDA are fluctuations in skin
      conductance due to an increase in sweat gland activity. Since
      sweat gland activity is associated with the autonomic nervous
      system activity, EDA is an established method to assess
      physiological reactions to arousal, concerns, or stress (Boucsein,
      2012; Naveteur & Freixa i Baqué, 1987; Nikula, 1991). In his
      overview of the method, Boucsein (2012) extensively reviewed
      applications and correlates of various measures of EDA. He
      concludes that EDA “can be regarded as a valid indicator for the
      strength of – mostly negative – emotions, for observing the course
      of psychological stress, and for objectively determining coping
      efficacy” (p. 521). Therefore, EDA can indicate state anxiety by
      detecting associated physiological reactions (Boucsein, 2012). 

     EDA has recently been used to observe emotions during
      educational processes like self-regulated and multimedia learning
      (Dindar et al., 2019; Mudrick, Taub, Azevedo, Price, & Lester,
      2017) and reading (Meer, Breznitz, & Katzir, 2016). Dew et al.
      (1984) used various measures of EDA and different scales to assess
      MAtrait and MAstate, but found no relation between EDA and
      MAstate, and only a small relation between EDA and MAstate for one
      of their measures of EDA. As a possible explanation, they
      acknowledge that the challenge of comparing cognitively
      experienced anxiety and physiologically experienced anxiety might
      need a larger sample than their 31 students. Moreover, their study
      design did not include a baseline measure, which is generally
      advisable for data quality (Boucsein, 2012) and could indicate if
      EDA is indeed influenced by a mathematical test context. Thus,
      while their theoretical assumptions seem well-founded, the authors
      argue that their data was not sufficient for a meaningful
      interpretation (Dew et al., 1984). 

     In conclusion, MAstate has been assessed through qualitative
      methods, self-reports, and physiological measures. Physiological
      reactions are a vital aspect of anxiety in general and arguably of
      MAstate in particular, but previous research has not provided
      clear results concerning the relation between self-reports and
      physiological measures of MAstate, or the relation between MAstate
      and MAtrait in general. 

     1.6 The present research 

     So far, we have discussed that the relation between MAstate and
      MAtrait is not yet fully understood. In performance situations,
      MAstate might be stronger related to processes influencing
      mathematical thinking, like a reduction of working memory
      capacity. Therefore, taking into account MAstate seems important
      when analyzing effects of MA, but it can be assessed in different
      ways. While self-reports of MAstate are easy to obtain, they might
      suffer from systematic biases. As an alternative, some studies
      used physiological measures of stress and arousal instead of
      self-reports to assess MAstate in mathematical performance
      situations. Yet, these studies did either not address both MAstate
      and MAtrait or, in the case of Dew et al. (1984), did not show
      clear results. Moreover, no study did yet include appraisals of
      control or value to describe the relation between MAstate and
      MAtrait in accordance with the control-value theory. We consider
      this a considerable gap in research on MA. We assume that the
      approach by Dew and colleagues (1984) to use EDA as an indicator
      for MAstate is more promising today, because the possibilities to
      record and analyze EDA have greatly improved. Particularly, the
      innovations in EDA recording offer better possibilities in
      observing the association between EDA and MAtrait, since they
      allow to assess MAstate more reliable and in an authentic
      environment. At the same time, using the control-value theory
      offers a better theoretical framework for the correlation between
      MAstate and individual antecedents. It has been supported by a
      number of studies using self-reports and other methods to assess
      state anxiety, but to our knowledge, the control-value theory has
      not yet been utilized to analyze precursors of EDA. 

     1.7 Hypotheses 

     In the present study, we investigated the relation of MAtrait
      with two indicators for MAstate, the physiological measure EDA and
      self-reported state anxiety. We assessed MAstate both in a
      baseline context (a relaxation exercise) and a mathematics test.
      First, we assumed that the mathematics test would lead to an
      increase in both measures (hypothesis 1) and thus indicate that
      anxiety is successfully induced by the mathematics test. Second,
      we assumed that there is a relation between self-reported MAstate
      and EDA (hypothesis 2). Moreover, we anticipated that our findings
      would replicate the direct association between self-reported
      MAstate and MAtrait (Goetz et al., 2013; hypothesis 3a). We
      expected to find a similar relation between EDA and MAtrait, since
      EDA should reveal physiological arousal, which in turn is an
      indicator of MAstate (hypothesis 3b). According to the
      control-value theory, appraisals of control and subjective value
      were included as predictors. We expected that this would confirm
      the relation between these appraisals and both measures of MAstate
      (hypotheses 4a and 4b). Finally, the relation between MAstate and
      MAtrait should be higher when students report low control and high
      perceived value. Thus, we expected a negative three-way
      interaction between MAtrait, appraisals of control, and perceived
      value, on both measures of MAstate, respectively (hypotheses 5a
      and b). 

     2. Method 

     2.1 Sample and procedure 

     95 undergraduate students participated in the study. They gave
      written informed consent before participation. The study was
      conducted according to the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and
      Code of Conduct of the American Psychological Association from
      2017. An ethics approval was not required by institutional
      guidelines or national regulations, in line with the guidelines of
      the German Research Foundation. Due to technical difficulties, 5
      participants had to be excluded from the sample. Additionally, we
      excluded 4 students because of deviations of more than 3 SD in one
      of the assessed measures. The remaining participants were 86
      undergraduate students (53 female) from programs other than
      mathematics, ranging from engineering to nutritional science.
      Mathematics students were not recruited as participants to avoid a
      bias in their beliefs and attitudes towards mathematics, as well
      as in their mathematical skills. The mean age was 23.2 years (SD =
      4.07). Participants were recruited on campus and were paid 15 EUR
      for participation. During recruitment and before the experiment
      any indication of a mathematical content of the study was avoided.
      The study was described as a study investigating EDA during
      various tasks. 

     The individual sessions of the experiment took place in an
      office at the university containing only two tables, two chairs,
      and a closed closet. At the beginning of the experiment, the
      experimenter made participants familiar with the wristband
      assessing EDA. She then put the device on the wrist of the
      participant’s non-dominant hand and fitted it comfortably. After
      recording had started, participants were presented a 5-minute
      relaxation exercise via headphones. The exercise facilitated
      relaxation through breathing exercises, accompanied by an audio
      track that included sounds from nature to help promote a relaxing
      environment for the participant. 

     When the participant removed the headphones after the exercise,
      the experimenter immediately presented the first questionnaire
      assessing state-anxiety. After the participant finished the
      questionnaire, a first mathematical test was presented. The
      participant was asked to read the instruction carefully and then
      wait for the signal to start. All participants had 10 minutes to
      solve the test and received a short notice after 8 minutes. After
      the test, the participant answered the second state-questionnaire.
      The procedure was repeated for a second mathematics test. At the
      end of the experiment, trait and demographic data were assessed. 

     2.2 Mathematics tests 

     Both mathematics tests consisted of six items. Eleven items were
      taken from a pool of released items from the PISA-Study (OECD,
      2013a); one item was adopted from the Trends in International
      Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS, International Association
      for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement [IEA], 2013). Since
      research suggests that anxiety might have a larger influence for
      cognitively demanding tasks (Ching, 2017; Faust, Ashcraft, &
      Fleck, 1996), we composed both tests to be fairly difficult. The
      overall solution rate of 42% (SD = 21%) suggests that the tests
      were appropriately demanding. 

     The items covered a broad range of mathematical problems,
      ranging from geometry to statistics. They were based on the
      concept of mathematical literacy and therefore covered
      mathematical competencies beyond mere factual knowledge. The tasks
      required knowledge that all students should have achieved by the
      end of their compulsory education. For an overall achievement
      score, we coded each item according to the coding instructions
      from PISA and TIMSS (0 = incorrect, 0.5 = partially correct, 1 =
      correct; OECD, 2013a; IEA, 2013) and calculated a sum score for
      all 12 items. 

     2.3 Study measures 

     We assessed MAtrait using the ANXMAT-Scale developed for the
      PISA-studies (five items, e.g. “I feel helpless when doing a
      mathematics problem”,  α  = .87; OECD, 2005).
      Participants answered on a 4-point Likert scale from 1, strongly
      disagree to 4, strongly agree. We assessed self-reported MAstate
      twice during the experiment according to Goetz et al., 2013,
      asking if participants felt anxious in the previous situation (1,
      definitely not to 4, definitely). Appraisals of control and
      perceived values were assessed after both tests and were
      task-specific. For appraisals of control, we used two items
      accounting for the controllability and probability of outcomes
      (e.g. “I think my competence in this area is …”,  α 
      = .78) on a 7-point Likert-scale (1, low to 9, high; Engeser &
      Rheinberg, 2008; Pekrun & Perry, 2014). Appraisals of
      perceived value were assessed with the four-item cognitive
      preferences-scale by Kehr, Von Rosenstiel, and Bles (1997) on a
      7-point Likert-scale (e.g. “It is important to me to solve the
      exercises”; 1, not at all to 9, very much;  α  =
      .85). 

     For EDA data collection during the relaxation exercise and the
      tests, we used an Empatica E4 wristband. The wristband is worn
      like a watch and measures skin conductance with two stainless
      steel electrodes at the inner wrist. The exosomatic non-invasive
      sensor applies a very small, non-perceptible alternating current
      with a peak value of 100  μ A at 1V with an 8Hz
      frequency. The 4 Hz signal is recorded on an integrated flash
      memory. 

     2.4 EDA data analyses 

     EDA signals consist of two components. The tonic signal is
      influenced by medium-term factors like room temperature or
      physiological characteristics of the individual. It provides a
      level of skin conductance that is rather stable within some
      seconds. Even though the tonic signal can be an indicator for
      stress or anxiety, the phasic component of the signal is suited
      better to compare EDA between individuals and is commonly used as
      an indicator for state anxiety (Boucsein, 2012). Phasic components
      of the EDA signal are usually called responses, since they reflect
      a short peak in the signal. Responses can be specific responses to
      a stimulation, for example a bursting balloon. However, there are
      phasic responses that are not associated to any specific external
      stimulation, hence nonspecific. The frequency of these nonspecific
      responses in skin conductance is associated with stress and
      anxiety and is one of the most common measures for EDA (Boucsein,
      2012). The phasic and the tonic components of an EDA signal
      overlap and need to be decomposed for analyses. 

     Data processing was carried out using MATLAB (V9.2.0) and the
      MATLAB-based software Ledalab (V3.4.9). The software applies
      Continuous Decomposition Analysis to extract the phasic signal
      (Benedek & Kaernbach, 2010). After the extraction, any peak in
      the phasic signal bigger than .01  μ S is counted
      as a response (Boucsein, 2012). For both phases of the experiment
      (relaxation and test), the number of events is then summed up and
      divided by the duration of the phase in minutes. The result is the
      frequency of nonspecific skin conductance responses per minute
      (SCR.freq). SCR.freq served as the measure for physiological
      MAstate. 

     2.5 Analyses 

     For hypothesis 1, we conducted a repeated measures ANOVA to test
      for differences in state anxiety during the relaxation and the
      test. To assess the relation between the two measures of MAstate
      and their relation to MAtrait (hypothesis 2 and 3), we calculated
      the correlations controlling for gender, achievement, and the
      respective baseline measures (see Sect. 3.1). For hypotheses 4 and
      5, we adopted a 5-step hierarchical multiple regression model for
      both measures of MAstate as outcome variables (self-reported and
      physiological MAstate). All predictors except gender were
      z-standardized before the analyses. In step 1, we included the
      control variables as predictors. In step 2, we additionally
      included MAtrait. In accordance with the control-value theory,
      step 3 included appraisals of control and subjective value. In
      step 4, we included the interaction term between control and
      subjective value. Finally, step 5 included the interaction terms
      between MAtrait and appraisals of control and subjective value,
      respectively. Additionally, we included the three-way interaction
      between MAtrait, control, and subjective value. 

     3. Results 

     3.1 Control variables 

     Gender differences exist between self-reports of MA (Dowker et
      al., 2016). Moreover, because of physiological differences in the
      sweat gland density and activity, women tend to display a weaker
      EDA reactivity than men (Boucsein, 2012). Accordingly, our results
      revealed significant gender differences, with females showing
      weaker EDA, t(84) = 2.93, p = .004, reporting higher MAtrait,
      t(84) = -2.38, p = .020, and lower control, t(84) = 2.37, p =
      .020. No significant gender differences were found regarding
      self-reports of MAstate, t(84) = -0.49, p = .626, and perceived
      value t(84) = 0.02, p = .984. Because of this general influence of
      gender, we included gender as a control variable in all following
      analyses. 

     In addition, achievement is associated both with trait anxiety
      (Ma, 1999) and with physiological reaction (Mattarella-Micke et
      al., 2011). In our data, we similarly found a significant relation
      between the test score and reports of MAtrait, r(86) = -.28, p =
      .008, self-reports of MAstate, r(86) = -.31, p = .004, and
      control, r(86) = .51, p = .000, respectively, but no significant
      relation between the test score and EDA, r(86) = .15, p = .177,
      and perceived value, r(86) = .07, p = .518, respectively. Since
      our analyses focused on the interplay of MAtrait and MAstate,
      irrespective of achievement, we also controlled for the test score
      in the following analyses. For both measures of MAstate
      (self-reports and EDA), we used the data from the relaxation
      exercise as respective baseline measures. 

     3.2 Main analyses 

      3.2.1 Descriptive results  

     Table 1 provides the means and standard deviations for MAtrait
      and appraisals of control and perceived value. Additionally, mean
      scores and standard deviations for both measures of MAstate during
      the relaxation exercise and the test are included. For both
      measures, MAstate was significantly higher during the test
      compared to the relaxation exercise, confirming hypothesis 1.
      While physiological MAstate increased from 15.43 events per minute
      to 20.04 events per minute (F(85) = 10.53, p = .002, 2 = .10),
      self-reported anxiety increased from 1.37 to 1.62 (F(85) = 17.23,
      p < .001, 2 = .17). 

     Table 1 

      Descriptive statistics and differences between MAstate in
        relaxation exercise and tests  

      

      Note. The unit for physiological state Mathematics Anxiety
      is SCR.freq [1/min]. 

      **p < .01 ***p < .001. 

     3.2.2 Correlations  

     Table 2 provides correlations between all measures. All
      correlations were controlled for gender and test score and for the
      respective MAstate baseline during the relaxation exercise.
      Contrary to hypothesis 2, no significant correlation was observed
      between the two measures of MAstate (r = .06, p = .63). MAtrait
      showed a moderate and significant correlation with self-reported
      MAstate (r = .34, p = .002), but not with physiological MAstate (r
      = .08, p = .48), which supports hypothesis 3a, but not 3b. 

     Including appraisals of control and perceived values, MAtrait
      correlated moderately and significantly with control and value (r
      = -.38, p < .001; r = .24, p = .029). A significant, moderate
      correlation emerged between appraisals of control and
      self-reported MAstate (r = -.29, p = .008), but not physiological
      MAstate (r = .05, p = .65). In contrast, appraisals of the
      perceived value were significantly related to physiological
      MAstate (r = .29, p = .007), but not to self-reported MAstate (r =
      .16, p = .15). Appraisals of control and perceived value showed no
      significant relation (r = -.01, p = .95). 

     Table 2 

      Correlations between measures of anxiety and appraisals of
        control and perceived value  

      

       Note.  Correlations of the two measures of state
      Mathematics Anxiety are controlled for their respective baseline.
      All correlations are controlled for gender and test score. n = 86.
      

      *p < .05 **p < .01 ***p < .001. 

      3.2.3 Hierarchical multiple regression  

     Results of the hierarchical multiple regressions are reported in
      Table 3. It displays only the predictors added in each step. For
      the full hierarchical models, see Appendix A.1. For the two
      regressions, we used the two measures of MAstate as outcome
      measures respectively. Inclusion of the control variables
      explained 58% of the variance in physiological MAstate during the
      test (p < .001), and 24% of the variance in self-reported
      MAstate (p < .001). 

     For self-reported MAstate, step 2 revealed a significant
      relation between MAtrait and self-reported MAstate (  β 
      = 0.32, p = .002) that explained additional 9% of the variance in
      self-reported MAstate (p = .002). Step 3 did not confirm a
      relation between appraisals of control or perceived value and
      self-reported MAstate ( β  = -0.20, p = .090; 
        β  = 0.09, p = .37). Step 4 did not reveal an
      interaction effect of control x value ( β  = -0.00,
      p = .98), and step 5 revealed no three-way interaction effect of
      MAtrait x control x value ( β  = -0.15, p = .20).
      Similarly, the interaction effects MAtrait x control and MA x
      value were not significant ( β = -0.06, p = .59; 
        β  = -0.23, p = .053). These findings do not support
      hypothesis 4a or 5a. Overall, the predictors explained 39% of the
      variance in self-reported MAstate (p < .001). 

     We conducted the same hierarchical multiple regression for
      physiological MAstate. Contrary to self-reported MAstate, step 2
      did not reveal a significant relation with MAtrait ( β 
      = 0.06, p = .48). In step 3, adding appraisals of control and
      perceived value increased the R2 significantly by 4% (p = .033).
      In this step, perceived value had a significant positive relation
      with physiological MAstate ( β  = 0.19, p = .009),
      while no relation was found for control ( β  =
      -0.04, p = .65). Again, step 4 did not reveal an interaction of
      the control and value on MAstate ( β  = 0.04, p =
      .65). Contrary to self-reported MAstate, step 5 revealed a
      negative three-way interaction effect of MAtrait x control x value
      ( β  = -0.23, p = .008), while the interaction
      effects MAtrait x control and MAtrait x value were not significant
      ( β  = -0.06, p = .44;  β  = -0.01, p
      = .96). These effects explained an additional 4% of the variance
      in physiological MAstate (p = .041). The three-way interaction
      effect is displayed in Figure 1 (right). For comparison, Figure 1
      (left) displays the non-significant interaction for self-reported
      MAstate. Because there was no significant direct relation between
      MAtrait and physiological MAstate, the slopes are less steep in
      Figure 1 (right) than for self-reported MAstate. However, it
      illustrates that the slope of MAtrait on physiological MAstate
      increases for students appraising low control and high perceived
      value at the same time. These results support hypothesis 5b, but
      not hypothesis 4b. Overall, the predictors explained 66% of the
      variance in physiological MAstate (p < .001). 

     Table 3 

      Hierarchical multiple regression analyses for physiological
        MAstate and self-reported MAstate  

      

      

      Figure 1.  Relation between MAtrait and MAstate in
      dependence of control and perceived value. 

     4. Discussion 

     4.1 Measures of state anxiety in mathematics tests 

     In line with hypothesis 1, we found significant differences
      between the relaxation exercise and the test for both measures of
      MAstate. This implies that the mathematics test induced anxiety
      compared to the relaxation exercise. However, descriptive analyses
      showed that self-reports of MAstate were relatively low in our
      study. This might be due to the fact that the experiment was a
      low-stakes test for the participants. We would assume that our
      result might emerge even stronger in a high-stakes test situation.
    

     In contrast to our hypothesis, students’ self-reports about
      MAstate and their physiological MAstate were not significantly
      related. Our assumption had been that even though self-reports and
      physiological measures might differ to some extent, they should
      still refer to a similar MAstate and hence be related. Judging
      from our results, the two measures might refer to conceptually
      different aspects of MAstate. Some researchers suggest that
      MAtrait is a multidimensional construct, usually differentiating
      between a cognitive and an affective dimension (Lukowski et al.,
      2016; Wigfield & Meece, 1988). Similarly, physiological
      MAstate and self-reported MAstate as assessed in this study might
      refer to different facets of MAstate. Consequently, they might not
      necessarily be related. For example, EDA might be more associated
      with arousal and an affective, emotional dimension of MAstate. In
      contrast, self-reports might be more related to a cognitive
      dimension of MAstate that is associated with worries and cognitive
      resources (Ashcraft & Moore, 2009; Beilock, 2008; Liebert
      & Morris, 1967). Future research could include a
      multi-dimensional assessment of MA to address this possibility.
      Additionally, the measures might differ because of their differing
      mode of assessment (Pekrun & Bühner, 2014). Self-reports might
      not be able to paint an adequate picture of achievement emotions,
      especially for a highly physiological emotion like anxiety (Pekrun
      & Bühner, 2014). Furthermore, self-reports of MAstate might be
      subject to biases like social desirability (Pekrun & Bühner,
      2014) or stereotypes (Goetz et al., 2013). 

     4.2 The relation between MAstate and MAtrait 

     In line with Goetz et al. (2013), we found a significant
      relation between MAtrait and self-reported MAstate which was
      within the range of previous findings reported by Hembree (1990).
      Students with higher MAtrait also reported higher MAstate during a
      mathematical test. However, we did not find a relation between
      MAtrait and physiological MAstate. This finding is contrary to
      hypothesis 3b but is in line with previous findings by Dew et al.
      (1984). Dew et al. (1984) proposed two explanations. First, the
      results might be viewed as questioning the construct validity of
      MAtrait scales. Since these scales have been further validated
      since then and worked as expected with regard to self-reported
      MAstate, this explanation seems unlikely. Alternatively, since
      students reporting MAstate need to evaluate their perceived
      anxiety cognitively, it is assumed that they might in part refer
      to generalized beliefs about mathematics. This might include the
      same resources as their evaluation of MAtrait (Bieg, Goetz, &
      Lipnevich, 2014; Goetz et al., 2013), or students might even refer
      directly to their MAtrait when trying to evaluate MAstate. This
      would increase the relation between self-reported MAstate and
      MAtrait, but not between physiological MAstate and MAtrait. 

     4.3 The control-value theory 

     According to the control-value theory (Pekrun, 2006), MAstate
      should primarily be determined by appraisals of control and
      perceived value. These appraisals should also moderate the
      relation between MAtrait and MAstate. For the two measures of
      MAstate, the application of the control-value theory in the
      present study produced diverging results. 

     Both MAtrait and self-reported MAstate were related to
      appraisals of control. Nevertheless, the hierarchical multiple
      regression did not produce signs that appraisals of control or
      value play an important role for the relation between MAtrait and
      self-reported MAstate. Rather, this relation seemed to be direct.
      Hence, we did not find support for the control-value theory for
      self-reported MAstate. As was proposed above, the relation between
      MAtrait and self-reported MAstate might be increased by the
      similar mode of assessment. The resulting direct relation could
      overweight a possible indirect effect of appraisals of control and
      perceived value. 

     In contrast, physiological MAstate showed a different pattern.
      In opposition to self-reported MAstate, we did not find a direct
      correlation with MAtrait. However, we found strong support for the
      control-value theory in this second multiple regression analysis.
      First, perceived value was related to MAstate, independent of
      MAtrait. Second, including appraisals of control and perceived
      value explained additional 8% of variance of physiological
      MAstate, which indicates a substantial contribution to its
      emergence. Third, the interplay between MAtrait, control, and
      value also was observed as expected. As illustrated in Figure 1
      (right), high MAtrait was related to high MAstate, but only when
      students appraised their control low and their perceived value
      high. This effect is in line with the control-value theory, since
      MAtrait is considered a distal antecedent, whereas appraisals of
      control and perceived value are considered proximal causes of
      MAstate. These results further support the notion that the causal
      relation between MAtrait and the two measures of MAstate might be
      conceptually different. 

     4.4 Limitations 

     Using EDA comes with some immanent limitations, and only some of
      them can be overcome. For example, EDA is subject to physiological
      gender differences. This inhibits its practicality for inquiring
      the gender gap in MA. Even when controlling for a baseline value,
      differences in reactivity exist. In general, a large variance
      between students’ EDA makes comparisons more difficult. In our
      study, we assessed the baseline value during a relatively short
      period of time. A more reliable value could be obtained through
      several hours or days of baseline recordings (Boucsein, 2012). Of
      course, such a study requires much more time. Lastly, even though
      MA is common in students of all ages, our specific sample cannot
      be overgeneralized. It needs to be verified if EDA recording can
      be useful in schools and for specific groups of students, for
      example high-anxiety students or younger students. 

     More generally, our test did not seem to induce a very strong
      emotional reaction. In order to generalize our findings to
      high-stakes testing which might cause more MAstate, additional
      studies are needed. Moreover, we followed Goetz at al. (2013) in
      using a single-item scale to assess self-reported MAstate. This
      keeps the disruption of the participants at a minimum but might
      result in some inaccuracies. Our results indicate that the scale
      was working properly, but future studies might try to assess
      MAstate at more occasions or check if the one-item scale is
      appropriately precise. Similarly, a number of different
      questionnaires exist to assess MA and general test anxiety.
      Comparing these questionnaires regarding their relation to EDA,
      particularly regarding cognitive and affective dimensions of these
      scales, could help to explain the absent relation between
      self-reported and physiological MAstate. 

     The relation between EDA and physiological arousal has been well
      established by previous research (Boucsein, 2012). However, other
      factors than MAstate might additionally influence EDA during
      mathematics tests. Future research could incorporate additional
      state measures that assess cognitive load or situational
      motivation to further narrow down the processes associated with
      EDA reactivity, and might support these findings through
      qualitative data like interviews or think-aloud-protocols. Until
      the validity of EDA as a measure of physiological MAstate is fully
      understood, results will always require a cautious discussion of
      limitations and different explanations. 

     The control-value theory is generalizable to various achievement
      emotions, including both trait and state emotions (Pekrun, 2006).
      In the current cross-sectional study, we focused on MAstate as an
      outcome, and task-specific appraisals of control and perceived
      value as moderators. Consequently, we considered MAtrait as a
      distal predictor. However, future studies could also consider
      MAtrait as an outcome itself. For analyzing effects of general
      appraisals of control and perceived value towards mathematics as
      predictors of MAtrait, longitudinal designs would be more
      advantageous. 

     4.5 Conclusion 

     Our study combined several innovative approaches that have
      emerged in research on MA within the last years. With the
      distinction between MAtrait and MAstate, we differentiated between
      two different facets of MA. Further, through the adoption of the
      control-value theory, we compared EDA recordings and common
      self-reports as a tool for observing MAstate and investigated
      their unique relations to MAtrait. Overall, we found that EDA was
      related to MAtrait, but that this relation only got visible when
      taking appraisals of control and perceived value into account.
      Students reporting high MAtrait were not necessarily more
      physiologically anxious during mathematical activities. Rather, a
      pattern of appraisals of low control and high perceived value
      accompanied that relation. Hence, with regard to EDA, our results
      were in line with the control-value theory, which on the other
      hand was not supported by self-reported measures of MAstate. In
      sum, our findings match the plea by Goetz at al. (2013) to
      consequently distinguish between MAtrait and MAstate in research
      on MA, as well as to additionally include physiological data in
      assessing emotions in learning. 

     Furthermore, our results indicate that self-reports and
      physiological measures might refer to different aspects of
      MAstate. Thus, our results support theoretical considerations and
      empirical findings that self-reports of MAstate should be
      interpreted cautiously. Ultimately, we cannot decide if
      self-reports or EDA captured actual MAstate. Rather, the two
      measures both seem to be related to MAtrait, but in different
      ways. Therefore, we cannot conclude that EDA can make self-reports
      obsolete, but we propose that the assessment of EDA can provide
      additional information about underlying affective aspects of
      MAstate. Because of recent technical advances in recording and
      analyses of EDA, the method seems to offer a convenient addition
      to the common practice of self-reports. Furthermore, the advances
      in EDA-recording offer the possibility to conduct studies in the
      classroom during regular classes with hardly any disruption. We
      believe that our study can be a first step into this promising
      direction of in vivo research on MA. 

     As a next step, the relation to mathematical achievement should
      be investigated. In the recent study, we used a mathematics test,
      the goal of which was to trigger MA, but that was not designed to
      diagnose mathematical achievement in detail. Our preliminary
      results indicate that achievement might be differently associated
      with self-reported and physiological MAstate, but a study that
      assesses mathematical performance in more detail is needed to shed
      light on this question. Additionally, achievement under conditions
      of MAstate and no MAstate should be compared in a within-subject
      design, since EDA shows a notable variance between subjects.
      Similarly, using tests that are not mathematical could help to
      distinguish how specific MAstate is linked to mathematics. At the
      same time, using EDA for other domains or test anxiety in general,
      possibly using the control-value theory, might be an interesting
      and fruitful perspective for future research. Lastly, the relation
      to working memory capacity, which has proven to be a key factor in
      the effects of MA, should be taken into account. Ultimately, this
      knowledge could be used to design longitudinal and intervention
      studies that use EDA to observe the role of MAstate for learning
      processes or create ways to decrease MAstate in mathematics tests,
      possibly without necessarily tackling MAtrait. 

     With a number of questions remaining unanswered, our study is
      merely a first step in including EDA as an indicator for MAstate.
      Nevertheless, the results illustrate that self-reports only
      comprise one perspective on the multi-faceted phenomenon of
      Mathematics Anxiety, and that including EDA can be uniquely
      insightful. 

     Keypoints 

    	 We did not find a correlation between EDA and measures of
        state anxiety or trait Mathematics Anxiety, respectively. 


    	 Self-reported state anxiety correlated significantly with
        trait anxiety independent of appraisals of control and perceived
        value, which is in contrast to the control-value theory. 


    	 In line with the control-value theory, trait Mathematics
        Anxiety predicted physiological state anxiety when high
        perceived value and low control of the achievement situation
        were reported. 


     Acknowledgments 

     We would like to thank Ashley L. Johnson and Kathrin Ebenhöh for
      their contributions during data collection. This research was
      funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF)
      and the Standing Conference of the Ministers of Education and
      Cultural Affairs of the Länder in the Federal Republic of Germany
      (KMK) [Grant number ZIB2016]. 

     References 

     

      Artemenko, C., Daroczy, G., Nuerk, H.-C. (2015). Neural correlates
      of math anxiety - an overview and implications. Frontiers in
        Psychology 6, 1333. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01333 

      Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math Anxiety: Personal, Educational, and
      Cognitive Consequences. Current Directions in Psychological
        Science 11(5), 181-185. doi:10.111/1467-8721.00196 

      Ashcraft, M. H. (2007). Is math anxiety a mathematical learning
      disability? In D. B. Berch & M. M. M. Mazzocco (Eds.), 
        Why Is Math So Hard for Some Children? The Nature and Origins of
        Mathematical Learning Difficulties and Disabilities  (pp.
      329-348). Baltimore: Brookes. 

      Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among
      working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of
        Experimental Psychology: General, 130(2), 224–237.
      doi:10.1037//0096-3445.130.2.224 

      Ashcraft, M. H., & Moore, A. M. (2009). Mathematics anxiety
      and the affective drop in performance. Journal of
        Psychoeducational Assessment, 27(3), 197-205.
      doi:10.1177/0734282908330580 

      Ashcraft, M. H., & Ridley, K. S. (2005). Math anxiety and its
      cognitive consequences: A tutorial review In J. I. D. Campbell
      (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 315-327).
      New York, NY: Psychology Press. 

      Beilock, S. L. (2008). Math performance in stressfull situations.
      Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(5),
      339–343. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00602.x 

      Benedek, M., & Kaernbach, C. (2010). A continuous measure of
      phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of Neuroscience
        Methods, 190(1), 80-91. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.028
      

      Bieg, M., Goetz, T., & Lipnevich, A. A. (2014). What students
      think they feel differs from what they really feel - academic
      self-concept moderates the discrepancy between students’ trait and
      state emotional self-reports. PLoS ONE, 9(3).
      doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0092563 

      Bieg, M., Goetz, T., Wolter, I., & Hall, N. C. (2015). Gender
      stereotype endorsment differentially predicts girls’ and boys’
      trait-state discrepancy in math anxiety. Froniers in
        Psychology, 6(1404). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01404 

      Boucsein, W. (2012). Electrodermal Activity (2 ed.). New
      York: Springer. 

      Ching, B. H.-H. (2017). Mathematics anxiety and working memory:
      Longitudinal associations with mathematical performance in Chinese
      children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51,
      99-113. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.06.006 

      Dew, K. M. H., Galassi, J. P., & Galassi, M. D. (1984). Math
      anxiety: relation with situational test anxiety, performance,
      physiological arousal, and math avoidance behaviour. Journal
        of Counseling Psychology, 31 (4), 580-583.
      doi:10.1037/0022-0167.31.4.580 

      Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., & Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics
      anxiety: What have we learned in 60 years? Frontiers in
        Psychology, 7, 508. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508 

      Dindar, M., Malmberg, J., Järvelä, S., Haataja, E., &
      Kirschner, P. A. (2019). Matching self-reports with electrodermal
      activity data: Investigating temporal changes in self-regulated
      learning. Education and Information Technologies.
      doi:10.1007/s10639-019-10059-5 

      Engeser, S., & Rheinberg, F. (2008). Flow, performance and
      moderators of challenge-skill balance. Motivation and
        Emotion, 32(3), 158-172. doi:10.1007/s11031-008-9102-4 

      Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and
      performance: the Processing Efficiency Theory. Cognition and
        Emotion, 6(6), 409. doi:10.1080/02699939208409696 

      Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G.
      (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional Control
      Theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336-353.
      doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336 

      Faust, M. W., Ashcraft, M. H., & Fleck, D. E. (1996).
      Mathematics anxiety effects in simple and complex addition. Mathematical






        Cognition, 2, 25-62. doi:10.1080/135467996387534 

      Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., & Goetz, T. (2007). Girls and
      mathematics - A „hopeless“ issue? A control-value approach to
      gender differences in emotions towards mathematics. European
        Journal of Psychology of Education, 22(4), 497-514.
      doi:10.1007/BF03173468 

      Goetz, T., Bieg, M., Lüdtke, O., Pekrun, R., & Hall, N. C.
      (2013). Do girls really experience more anxiety in mathematics? Psychological






        Science, 24(10), 2079–2087. doi:10.1177/0956797613486989 

      Goldin, G. A. (2014). Perspectives on emotion in mathematical
      engagement, learning, and problem solving. In R. Pekrun & L.
      Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International handbook of emotions
        in education (pp. 391-414). New York: Routledge. 

      Goldin, G. A., Hannula, M. S., Heyd-Metzuyanim, E., Jansen, A.,
      Kaasila, R., Lutovac, S., . . . Zhang, Q. (Eds.). (2016). 
        Attitudes, beliefs, motivation and identity in Mathematics
        Education. An overview of the field and future directions 
      . Hamburg: Springer Open. 

      Hannula, M. S. (2016). Introduction. In G. A. Goldin, M. S.
      Hannula, E. Heyd-Metzuyanim, A. Jansen, R. Kaasila, S. Lutovac, P.
      Di Martino, F. Morselli, J. A. Middleton, M. Pantziara, & Q.
      Zhang (Eds.),  Attitudes, beliefs, motivation and identity in
        Mathematics Education. An overview of the field and future
        directions  (pp. 1-2). Hamburg: Springer Open. 

      Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects and relief of mathematics
      anxiety. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21.
      doi:10.2307/749455 

      Ho, H.-Z., Senturk, D., Lam, A. G., Zimmer, J. M., Hong, S.,
      Okamoto, Y., . . . Wang, C.-P. (2000). The affective and cognitive
      dimensions of math anxiety: A cross-national study. Journal
        for Research in Mathematics Education, 31(3), 362-379.
      doi:10.2307/749811 

      International Association for the Evaluation of Educational
      Achievement [IEA] (2013). TIMSS 2011 assessment.
      Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center. 

      Kantor, L., Endler, N. S., Heslegrave, R. J., Kocovski, N. L.
      (2001). Validating self-report measures of state and trait anxiety
      against a physiological measure. Current Psychology 20(3),






      207-215. doi: 10.1007/s12144-001-1007-2 

      Kehr, H. M., Von Rosenstiel, L., & Bles, P. (1997). 
       Zielbindung, subjektive Fähigkeiten und intrinsische
        Motivation [Goal commitment, subjective abilities, and intrinsic
        motivation].  Paper presented at the 16th Colloquium of
      Motivational Psychology, Potsdam, Germany. 

      Lee, J. (2009). Universals and specifics of math self-concept,
      math self-efficacy, and math anxiety across 41 PISA 2003
      participating countries. Learning and Individual Differences,
        19(3), 355–365. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2008.10.009 

      Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and
      emotional components of test anxiety: a distinction and some
      initial data. Psychological Reports, 20(3), 975-978.
      doi:10.2466/pr0.1967.20.3.975 

      Lukowski, S. L., DiTrapani, J., Jeon, M., Wang, Z., Schenker, V.
      J., Doran, M. M., . . . Petrill, S. A. (2016). Multidimensionality
      in the measurement of math-specific anxiety and its relationship
      with mathematical performance. Learning and Individual
        Differences. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2016.07.007 

      Lyons, I. M., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Mathematics Anxiety:
      Separating the math from the anxiety. Cerebral Cortex, 22,
      2102-2110. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr289 

      Ma, X. (1999). A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety
      toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics. Journal
        for Research in Mathematics Education, 30(5), 520-540.
      doi:10.2307/749772 

      Maloney, E. A., Ansari, D., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2011). The
      effect of mathematics anxiety on the processing of numerical
      magnitude. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(1),





      10-16. doi:10.1080/17470218.2010.533278 

      Maloney, E. A., Schaeffer, M. W., & Beilock, S. L. (2013).
      Mathematics anxiety and stereotype threat: shared mechanisms,
      negative consequences and promising interventions. Research
        in Mathematics Education, 15(2), 115–128.
      doi:10.1080/14794802.2013.797744 

      Mattarella-Micke, A., Mateo, J., Kozak, M. N., Foster, K., &
      Beilock, S. L. (2011). Choke or thrive? The relation between
      salvary cortisol and math performance depends on individual
      differences in working memory and math-anxiety. Emotion, 11(4),






      1000-1005. doi:10.1037/a0023224 

      Meer, Y., Breznitz, Z., & Katzir, T. (2016). Calibration of
      self-reports of anxiety and physiological measures of anxiety
      while reading in adults with and without readig disability. Dyslexia,






        22, 267-284. doi:10.1002/dys.1532 

      Mudrick, N. V., Taub, M., Azevedo, R., Price, M. J., & Lester,
      J. (2017).  Can physiology indicate cognitive, affective,
        metacognitive, and motivational self-regulated learning
        processes during multimedia learning?  Paper presented at
      the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research
      Association (AERA), San Antonio, TX. 

      Namkung, J. M., Peng, P., & Lin, X. (2019). The relation
      between Mathematics Anxiety and mathematics performance among
      school-aged students: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational
        Research, 89(3), 459–496. doi:10.3102/0034654319843494 

      Naveteur, J., & Freixa i Baqué, E. (1987). Individual
      differences in Electrodermal Activity as a function of subjects’
      anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 8(5),
      615-626. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(87)90059-6 

      Niculescu, A. C., Tempelaar, D., Dailey-Hebert, A., Segers, M.,
      Gijselaers, W. (2015). Exploring the antecedents of
      learning-related emotions and their relations with achievement
      outcomes. Frontline Learning Research 3 (1), 1-17.
      doi:10.14786/flr.v3i1.136 

      Nikula, R. (1991). Psychological correlates of nonspecific skin
      conductance responses. Psychophysiology, 28(1), 86-90.
      doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb03392.x 

      Ng, E. L., & Lee, K. (2015). Effects of trait test anxiety and
      state anxiety on children's working memory task performance. Learning






        and Individual Differences, 40, 141-148.
      doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2015.04.007 

      OECD. (2005). PISA 2003 technical report. Retrieved from
      http://www.oecd.org/edu/school/programmeforinternationalstudentassessmentpisa/35188570.pdf






      

      OECD. (2013a). PISA 2012 released mathematics items. Retrieved







      from
https://www.oecd.org/pisa/pisaproducts/pisa2012-2006-rel-items-maths-ENG.pdf

      OECD. (2013b). PISA 2012 results: ready to learn: students
      ’engagement, drive and self-beliefs (Volume III):
      Pisa, OECD Publishing. 

      Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement
      emotions: assumptions, corollaries, and implications for
      educational research and practice. Educational Psychology
        Review, 18(4), 315–341. doi:10.1007/s10648-006-9029-9 

      Pekrun, R., & Bühner, M. (2014). Self-report masures of
      academic emotions. In R. Pekrun & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia
      (Eds.), International handbook of emotions in education
      (pp. 561-579). New York: Routledge. 

      Pekrun, R., Lichtenfeld, S., Marsh, H. W., Murayama, K., &
      Götz, T. (2017). Achievement emotions and academic performance:
      longitudinal models of reciprocal effects. Child Development,
        88(5), 1653-1670. doi:10.1111/cdev.12704 

      Pekrun, R., & Perry, R. P. (2014). Control-Value Theory of
      Achievement Emotions. In R. Pekrun & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia
      (Eds.), International handbook of emotions in education
      (pp. 120-141). New York: Routledge. 

      Plake, B. S., & Parker, C. S. (1982). The development and
      validation of a revised version of the Mathematics Anxiety Rating
      Scale. Educational and psychological measurement, 42(2),
      551-557. doi:10.1177/001316448204200218 

      Pletzer, B., Wood, G., Moeller, K., Nuerk, H. C., &
      Kerschbaum, H. H. (2010). Predictors of performance in a real-life
      statistics examination depend on the individual cortisol profile.
      Biological Psychology, 85, 410-416.
      doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.08.015 

      Pletzer, B., Kronbichler, M., Nuerk, H.-C., & Kerschbaum, H.
      H. (2015). Mathematics anxiety reduces default mode network
      deactivation in response to numerical tasks. Frontiers in
        Human Neuroscience, 9, 202. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00202 

      Richardson, F. C., & Suinn, R. M. (1972). The Mathematics
      Anxiety Rating Scale: psychometric data. Journal of
        Counseling Psychology, 19(6), 551–554. doi:10.1037/h0033456
      

      Sarkar, A., Dowker, A., & Cohen Kadosh, R. (2014). Cognitive
      enhancement or cognitive cost: trait-specific outcomes of brain
      stimulation in the case of mathematics anxiety. The Journal
        of Neuroscience, 34, 16605–16610.
      doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3129-14.2014 

      Steimer, T. (2002). The biology of fear- and anxiety-related
      behaviors. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 4(3),
      231-249. 

      Suárez-Pellicioni, M., Núñez-Peña, M. I., & Colomé, À. (2016).
      Math anxiety: a review of its cognitive consequences,
      psychophysiological correlates, and brain bases. Cognitive,
        Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(1), 3-22.
      doi:10.3758/s13415-015-0370-7 

      Wiedemann, K. (2015). Anxiety and anxiety disorders. In J. D.
      Wright (Ed.),  International Encyclopedia of the Social &
        Behavioral Sciences (Second Edition)  (pp. 804-810).
      Amsterdam: Elsevier. 

      Wigfield, A., & Meece, J. L. (1988). Math anxiety in
      elementary and secondary school students. Journal of
        Educational Psychology, 80 (2), 210-216.
      doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.2.210 

      Zeidner, M. (2014). Anxiety in education. In R. Pekrun & L.
      Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International handbook of emotions
        in education (pp. 120-141). New York: Routledge. 

       

    

     Table A.1 

      Full hierarchical multiple regression analyses for
        physiological MAstate and self-reported MAstate