© 2022 Published by KURA Education & Publish- ing. This is an open access article under the CC BY- NC- ND license. (https://creativecommons. org/licenses/by/4.0/) 199 Copyright © www.iejee.com ISSN: 1307-9298 International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education January 2023, Volume 15, Issue 3, 199-208 Autism and Behavior Analysis: From Dissonance to Dialogue Karola Dillenburgera,*, Mickey Keenanb Abstract Introduction Questions about human behavior and diversity have captured the attention of scientists from a wide range of philosophical stances for centuries. While behavior analysts would argue that natural science is an enlightening perspective when applied to the understanding of the human condition, at times arguments within certain campaigns would prefer Applied Behaviour Analysis (ABA) to be banned or abolished. In this paper we discuss some of the roots of this stance and suggest a way forward based on what are basically complementary views on the goals enshrined in rights-based practice. Exclusion in all its many guises is an ugly thing and, in this paper, we discuss consequences that arise for autistic individuals who have particularly high support needs (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Lord et al., 2022), if applied behavior analysis (ABA)-based services were abolished or excluded (Graber & Graber, 2023). We argue that countering exclusion and ensuring real inclusion requires dialogue and genuine empathy. Autistic self-advocates commonly report the consequences of discrimination, ableism, a normalizing agenda, being viewed from a medical rather than a social model, and misunderstandings concerning support needs (Tarvainen, 2019). Until the 1980s, autism was not officially recognised (Volkmar et al., 1988) and profoundly autistic people (Lord et al., 2022) were considered unreachable or unteachable and commonly confined to a lifetime in institutional care (Gómez-Durán et al., 2014). Those who presented with challenging behavior were exposed to electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) and/or physically and pharmacologically restraint (Nielson et al., 2021; Schnitzer et al., 2020). While this situation seems not to have changed in some countries (Buivydaite et al., 2017; Lipinski et al., 2022), in the 1960s, behavior analysts in the USA were the first professionals who were able to show that profoundly autistic individuals were not “unteachable,” but that they could be taught and Keywords: Natural Science, Behavior Analysis, Human Diversity, Autism Diversity, Discrimination Received : 19 November 2022 Revised : 20 January 2023 Accepted : 8 March 2023 DOI : 10.26822/iejee.2023.292 a,* Corresponding Author: Karola Dillenburger, Queen’s University Belfast, N. Ireland. E-mail: k.dillenburger@qub.ac.uk ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3410-5949 b Mickey Keenan, Ulster University, N. Ireland. E-mail: mickeykeenan@me.com ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5579-9169 200 January 2023, Volume 15, Issue 3, 199-208 that they could learn (Ferster & DeMyer, 1962). They applied the principles of behavior formulated by the science of behavior analysis to design environments that facilitated skills development for persons who had been exposed to long-term care in designated institutions. Since then, the field of applied behavior analysis (ABA; Baer et al., 1968, 1987) has grown, particularly with regards to supporting autistic individuals (Surgeon General, 1999). So much so, that today ABA is viewed as providing the basis for “gold standard” interventions used to support individuals on the autism spectrum (Koegel, 2011; Lovaas, 1987; Smith & Eikeseth, 2011). With such a high profile, it is not surprising that for some, the abbreviation ABA practically became a synonym for a method of autism intervention (Chiesa, 2006; Dillenburger & Keenan, 2009). In the meantime, disability advocates achieved some success in their fight against discrimination, health disparities, and ableism (Borowsky et al., 2021; Nario- Redmond et al., 2019; Tarvainen, 2019; Thorne & Barr, 2022), including extensive ratification of the United Nations Convention for the Rights for Persons with Disabilities (UNCRPD, 2006). As for autistic people, the fight for awareness, acceptance, and equal rights continues to be shaped by autistic self-advocates, mainly through the newly formed neurodiversity movement (Leadbitter et al., 2021a, 2021b; Silberman, 2015) as well as families of children with profound autism (Unumb, 2013). But somewhere along the way, something has gone wrong. The special and supportive relationship between behavior analysts and profoundly autistic individuals and their caregivers that was built on deeply rooted care and trust in science appears to have been highjacked by an anti-ABA movement that views the discipline of ABA as abusive, causing post-traumatic stress, being ableist, and promoting a normalizing agenda (Kupferstein, 2018; Milton, 2012; Sandoval-Norton & Shkedy, 2019; examples of shockingly bad practice by poorly trained professionals can be found on YouTube). The focus of this paper is to discuss how those who have dedicated their lives and their science to helping people (i.e., behaviour analysts; cf., APBA, 2017) are being singled out and accused as protagonists of all of society’s ills with regards to maltreatment of individuals with autism. While in his early writings Milton (2012) could be viewed as one of the leading denigrators of ABA, more recently he has sounded much more conciliatory and even seemed to hint at the solution when he discussed the double empathy problem (Milton, 2018): …when people with very different experiences of the world interact with one another, they will struggle to empathise with each other. This is likely to be exacerbated through differences in language use and comprehension. (Theory of double empathy section, para 1) In order to balance a debate that generally is dominated by the views of autistic self-advocates, we offer an insider view of the perspective of behavior analysts who aim to help profoundly autistic people. What follows is an overview of our personal story as behavior analysts and the struggles to address the imbalance in discussions that could prevent profoundly autistic individuals receiving the kinds of supports that our science can offer. Recently, we learned that the 3-year-old child we met in 1997, and whose parents we introduced to the discipline of ABA all those years ago, passed his driving test and he got a job in the open employment market. To be honest, that touched us deeply, more in fact than when he was awarded his Master’s degree in American History a few years earlier. Before we met him, he had been assessed by all the usual health and allied health professionals, none of whom (according to reports by his parents) were able to help him acquire even the most basic skills. According to their clinical assessment reports, he was never going to learn to speak in full sentences and was destined for an institution. Together with his mother and other parents, we established the charity “Parents Education as Autism Therapists” (PEAT; the title was chosen by parents). During the subsequent 20+ years, PEAT helped hundreds of families build important life skills for their children, we co-authored the first book about ABA and autism in Europe (Keenan, Kerr, & Dillenburger, 2000) that has been translated into German and Japanese (Keenan, Kerr, Dillenburger, et al., 2000), we co-produced a multilingual multimedia online ABA training platform that has been translated into ten languages (German, Spanish, Dutch, Italian, Swedish, Icelandic, Norwegian, Czech, Portuguese, Canadian French, with Greek and Japanese in progress; Simple Steps, 2013), and we collaborated in a range of large scale research projects (Dillenburger et al., 2010, 2015; Keenan et al., 2010; 2022) These are remarkable achievements inspired by a small parent-led charity that was established and provided services for over 20 years without any support from government. What is even more remarkable is that this charity was set up in Northern Ireland, in a context where their community has been torn apart by violent conflict over many years (Dillenburger, 1991, 2007; Fargas-Malet & Dillenburger, 2016), and their story continues to be one of struggle against prejudice, discrimination, and repression, as oftentimes the language used to caricature ABA (as ableist, abusive, prejudicial, discriminatory, and pushing a normalizing agenda), is itself ableist, abusive, prejudicial, and discriminatory (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 2014; Kupferstein, 2018; Sandoval-Norton & Shkedy, 2019). 201 Autism and Behavior Analysis: From Dissonance to Dialogue / Dillenburger & Keenan Here is the problem. As behavior analysts, we think differently from most other people. As scientists, we avoid mentalistic pseudo-explanations for behaviour (APBA, 2016; Chiesa, 1994; Green, 2016; Skinner, 1953b; Thyer, 2009; Thyer & Myers, 1999; Thyer & Pignotti, 2010). Instead, our raison d’etre is the discovery of fundamental principles of behavior. We investigate the intimate relationship between environmental contingencies and behavior. The language we use to describe these relationships is full of what sounds like “jargon” to those who are not familiar with the science. A point to make here is that in any other science the terms scientists use may appear strange, cold, alien, even overly technical (Keenan & Dillenburger, 2000). This is because the terms scientists use are short-hand for discussions with other scientists, and therefore they have to be defined very carefully. As Milton (2018) realised, there are “differences in language use and comprehension” (Theory of double empathy section, para 1) that can hinder communication between people with different experiences, as would be the case between scientists and non-scientists. In fact, while the everyday language we inherit from our parents has its role in everyday life, it interferes with the precision required by science (Skinner, 1953b) and thus does a poor job for scientists (Chiesa, 1994; Moore, 2013a). Take for example, the term “behavior.” In everyday use of the term, it refers to "the way in which one acts, especially towards others,” oftentimes meaning “bad behavior” (e.g., "Did the children behave?;" Cambridge University Press, 2020). In contrast, when behavior analysts talk about “behavior,” we define the term behavior as the “interaction of the organism with their environment,” both, historically as well as currently (Cooper et al., 2007; Phelps, 2007). We talk about lifelong “learning histories” as well as “cultural meta-contingencies” that impact on present behavior (Glenn, 1988, 2004). This holistic view of behavior includes private behavior (what happens inside the skin, e.g., thinking and feeling; Skinner, 1977; Keenan & Dillenburger, 2022 Mentalism Chapter) as well as public behavior (e.g., moving and speaking; Skinner, 1953a, 1957). The image of a stream or river is helpful here. We cannot talk about a river without referencing both the water and the banks through which it flows. Likewise, behavior analysts consider that the skin does not separate us from the world, but it connects us to it. This is a completely different way of viewing the world in which we live, with major implications for understanding the process of living where a typical lifespan can be over 2,208,988,800 s. Called the behavioral stream, the process of living is viewed as a stream of experiences accumulated throughout one’s lifetime (Keenan & Dillenburger, 2022, Behavioural Stream and Private Events chapter). There is no formal “diagnosis” for our way of being, nor should there be. As behaviour analysts we are just different, with diverse views within our community. The science we employ to study behavior is attracting more people from around the world (see ABAI, 2022; APBA, 2016). Yet, we are being told by some (e.g., the neurodiversity movement) that we are wrong in what we are doing, even alleging that we are causing harm and abusing vulnerable children on the autism spectrum (Sandoval-Norton & Shkedy, 2019). They demand that everyone (and that includes behavior analysts) conform to their normalizing agenda and their way of doing things (Milton, 2012). As mentioned previously, when behavior analysts study the behavioral stream, the goal is to increase awareness of how behavior is affected by environmental contingencies. This is a formidable task and the methods that have been developed have enriched lives in a wide range of areas (see Cambridge Centre for Behavior Studies, 2022; Heward et al., 2022). Sharing the findings from behavior analytic research is not without its problems. When autistic children experience severe challenges, commonly parents seek guidance on how to design experiences for their children in order to facilitate the emergence of confidence and independence. In other words, parents need to learn how to implement “interventions” with precision and fidelity. Behavior analysts have developed many strategies for designing bespoke experiences that straddle education and health issues. Denying either the existence of the principles of behavior or their relevance to education and health is simply not an honest way to proceed. In contrast to North America, where there are laws to ensure ABA-based interventions are available to those who need them (Unumb, 2013), in Europe the misinformation about ABA disseminated by people not trained in the science has resulted in ABA-based services generally not being endorsed by governments (Keenan et al., 2022). In fact, some parents report that they have been threatened with the removal of all autism services if they continued with their ABA-based home programs (Dillenburger et al., 2015). Intriguingly, these threats occur even though their children are making progress at home and are learning skills that they did not learn at school (Larsson, 2021). Keenan (2016) pointed out that this kind of anti-ABA perspective implies that parents who seek to improve their awareness of how best to educate their children should apologise to their children. Maybe they should say things like “We didn't know that by educating you, by preparing you for the future, we inadvertently did not ‘accept you as you are'?" (Keenan, 2016, p. 10). Or maybe they are supposed to say to their children: Please forgive us for designing experiences for you to have fun, for toilet-training you, for helping you make friends, for supporting you to manage your self-injury, for teaching you to communicate, for giving you the 202 January 2023, Volume 15, Issue 3, 199-208 skills to make choices and convey your individuality, and for expressing our love for you in numerous other practical ways. We did not know that this could be interpreted negatively, as trying to change who you are or as trying to make you ‘normal’ (Keenan, 2016, p .10). Those who perpetuate myths about behavior analysis are missing the point completely if they consider these parental apologies to be necessary. On one hand they dismiss the whole science because they object to the idea of “deliberately doing something to change a person” (i.e., education). Yet in the next breath, they propose to engage socially with a person in applied settings in the hope that the person be supported or empowered (i.e., changed). The additional layer of awareness provided by the science of behavior analysis in relation to the effects of environmental changes is considered an anathema. What is truly very sad about this situation is that it impacts directly on the benefits to be derived from awareness of how environmental contingencies contribute to the outcomes of education. The anti-ABA stance generally is at odds with the science of behaviour analysis for a number of other reasons. Behavior analysis embraces the full spectrum of neurodiversity and there is no better evidence than the way it addresses the differing educational needs of each person in applied settings (Kazdin, 2010). To ensure that the focus is on the individual, we use single-system research designs to monitor changes in the behavioral stream (Keenan & Dillenburger, 2011) instead of using comparative group designs which commonly leave one group without support that may be desirable (e.g., NICE, 2013). The result of the spread of misinformation is that a whole science has been branded as controversial (Parker, 2015) when the real controversy should revolve around the deliberate dissemination of misinformation. The misrepresentation is so pervasive in Europe that on one hand it is said that it would be wrong to invest in only one “thing” like ABA, while on the other hand it is argued that it is acceptable to invest in one “thing” called an “eclectic approach,” even though there is evidence that ABA-based interventions are more effective than eclectic interventions (Howard et al., 2005, 2014). Unfortunately, no-one addresses the ethical questions that arise from the role played by the Dunning-Kruger effect (Benzel, 2022; Hofer et al., 2022) when misinformation and the associated caricatures of ABA form the basis of government strategies and policies (Dillenburger et al., 2014). Vyse (2022) is right when he says: It is one thing to form a social movement in an effort to gain greater acceptance of and better supports for a group of people in need. It is quite another to do so at the expense of another group who is also in great need. Helping people on the autism spectrum should not be a zero-sum game with gains at one end of the spectrum requiring losses at another. Unfortunately, the autism self-advocacy movement’s attacks on ABA [applied behavior analysis] create just such a dilemma (The Death of Truth and Freedom section, para 3). To ward off the onslaught of the anti-ABA perspective (Note: this language is often used by parents on the receiving end of misinformation), some have suggested that it might be best to change the name of ABA. This suggestion is truly symptomatic of the stresses to which parents are exposed. What other science has to contend with a name change as a strategy for managing the aversive consequences produced by discrimination and prejudice. One of the authors (MK) explains: As a child who grew up in N. Ireland, I am familiar with many facets of prejudice. Raised by a Protestant mother and having a Catholic father, I experienced at first hand the damaging effects of misguided conclusions conceived in the shadow of myths. When visiting my grandparents, it was at times a surreal experience to cross from one perspective tainted by prejudice to the other perspective similarly tainted by a different kind of prejudice. I was exposed to exclusion and discrimination based on prejudice. In many walks of life during my childhood, Irish people were not welcome, as expressed in the sign in the window “No Irish”. Never did I imagine, though, that in my professional life those scenes from my childhood would be replayed. I should have known better. Behavior analysts are not welcome. The sign in the window should read “No ABA’ or “No Behavior Analysts.” In Shakespeare’s play Romeo and Juliet, Juliet described the conflict between the rivalling families with the poetic words: “A rose by any other name would smell as sweet.” The families of my own parents, and the families described by Shakespeare, were not able to break free from the experiences that shaped their beliefs in order to accommodate another with different experiences and consequently a different perspective. Yet, many parents of children with autism have been able to break free from the misinformation and myths about ABA and as a consequence their children have benefitted greatly (e.g., ABA4All, 2022). Unfortunately, we have seen parents pushed to the point of exhaustion in defence of ABA in courts and tribunals fighting for their children’s rights to have access to ABA-based interventions (Blakemore, 2021; Byrne & Byrne, 2000). Where else would this be allowed to happen? Where are the investigative journalists who want a human interest story, one about how health and education authorities peddle misinformation about a science in which they are not trained, while parents do what they can to defend a science which they evidently understand better than those professionals (Dillenburger et al., 2012)? The ethical drama that is played out in these situations simply is staggering and at the same time diabolical. 203 Autism and Behavior Analysis: From Dissonance to Dialogue / Dillenburger & Keenan It is almost as if the parents are put on trial for being witches, inhuman creatures who are hell bent on harming their children. The truth is that these parents are heroes, mustering all of their love to defend the opportunity for their children to acquire skills that others believed they were unable to acquire. So much for equality of opportunity and disability rights. So much for the rights of their children to be regarded as people deserving of the opportunity to be guided by a science dedicated to bringing out the best in people through its accumulated understanding of the principles of behavior change. Clearly, those who are against ABA are not the only bastions of love for children. Nevertheless, ABA professionals who guide parents when educating their children continue to be maligned for doing so. It is worth stating that again in other words. Parents who have used the discipline of ABA to successfully empower their children and improve the quality of their children’s lives have been criticised for doing so. They have been criticised because some people proposed that the evil world of ABA could never produce something positive. Indeed, they would consider these parents to have been duped into adopting a perspective that emphasises the importance of normalizing children, and not accepting their uniqueness. One of the authors (MK) elaborates: When I hear that kind of misguided anti-ABA rhetoric, I find myself straddling the mindsets of both sets of my grandparents (one set from a Protestant background and one from a Catholic background). It was never possible for them to accept that something positive could emanate from the other camp. Of course, this was not always the case for them, but when it came to important matters that divided the community, they each held firm to how they had been taught to view their nemesis. My time growing up in a divided community has brought many stresses to me personally. But it has also educated me in showing how twisted logic is used to defend either position when it is challenged. There was a time in my childhood, for example, when one could have been challenged in the streets along these lines: “Are you a Catholic or a Protestant?” “I am an atheist” “Are you a Catholic atheist or a Protestant atheist” There is no normalizing agenda at the heart of ABA. Apart from this correction, there is another misguided criticism that needs to be rebuffed. It is said that using ABA is tantamount to using a one-size-fits-all approach that ignores individual needs. This one-size-fits-all criticism is entirely misleading because there is indeed one thing that is used by the scientist/practitioner to guide the development of bespoke services, and it is called the scientific method. But this is entirely different from the kind of plug-and-play conceptualisation of one-size-fits-all conjured up by authorities who refuse to invest in ABA. Surely it cannot be wrong to employ professionals who use the scientific method in the 21st century for designing bespoke services. Nurturing an individual using the scientific method so they can feel empowered by the acquisition of skills is an enlightened act of human kindness, not the act of a despot intent on removing their uniqueness (Keenan & Dillenburger, 2020). To conclude, here is an example that illustrates what can be achieved with good quality ABA-based interventions. Following it is a contrived example of the sorts of criticism that have been levelled by others in the face of something that does not sit well with preconceived ideas about the nature of ABA. This case study was provided by Dr. Nichola Booth (personal communication): A young boy, 8 years old, had a confirmed diagnosis of autism and was non-verbal. It was reported that he had excessive amounts of saliva that was impacting on his day-to-day social and educational experiences. At school he was being isolated from his peer group as he was rubbing the saliva over other children, teachers, classroom assistants and equipment. The same behaviors were happening at home to the point where his parents would not have visitors because their furniture was covered in dried saliva. His clothing was destroyed from being caked in dried saliva. After medication failed, the parents went back to their GP. It was at this stage that they were told the next step would be a referral to a surgeon for an invasive operation where a piece of his mouth would be snipped to reduce or eliminate the extreme levels of saliva. Due to waiting times for this appointment, the parents accepted this and went home. While waiting for the hospital appointment the parents decided to access the services of a behavior analyst for support, but nothing related to the saliva issue. The ABA professional came to the family home and carried out parental interviews and behavioral observations to determine what supports would be required. While carrying out the observation he noticed the behavior associated with excessive saliva production, especially the wiping of the saliva on all items and people within the room. This was raised as a potential issue to be addressed and it was then that the parents explained what had happened to date. The behavior analyst suggested that they could perhaps put a programme in place to see if this behavior could be reduced without the need for surgery. The parents agreed. Following collection of data from additional observations, both at home and at school, a plan was developed. A sensory tray of all ‘goo-like’ items was made and placed in the main family room in the home. Alongside this, a visual sign for the tray was made available across the home environment for the child to access. The parents were educated in how to identify when the excessive saliva related behaviors were about to occur – these included heightened vocalisations, 204 January 2023, Volume 15, Issue 3, 199-208 hand-stimming, and facial movements. When the parents saw these behaviors, they were immediately to prompt the child to point to the visual for the sensory table visual and then immediately redirect him to the sensory tray. He was then requested to ‘play’ and the parents modelled some of the playing behaviors for him with the items in the tray. This strategy was implemented successfully in the home with the saliva related behavior reducing significantly before it was introduced in the school setting. The end results was that surgery was no longer required and the behavior had decreased with the child being fully reintegrated back into school and other social events. A Critique of this Case Study It is awful that the child was not accepted for who he was by both the medical professional and the ABA professional! Instead, they each attempted to normalize him, one using drugs and then proposing an invasive surgical procedure. The ABA professional was more deceptive and hood-winked the child into changing his behavior, thereby refusing to acknowledge the child’s need to engage in his preferred activity! Conclusion With so many parents of profoundly autistic children now championing ABA while others try to stop all ABA, it is clear that something is amiss. As can happen in any profession, some people have been on the receiving end of malpractice. In medical practice, some people have even been murdered by their doctors (Gunn, 2010) and of course, the actions of such people must be restricted, but not at the expense of the entire discipline of medicine. The same has not been the case with ABA. It seems that some malpractice has led to the condemnation of the whole science. In the midst of the struggle for equality, one very important point should not be overlooked. There is a strategy often fostered by an establishment in many walks of life called “divide et impera” [divide and conquer], an approach that Julius Cesar used successfully 22 centuries ago (Razzetti, 2018). While autistic self-advocates and behavior analysts are at logger heads, the establishment does not need to change and can continue to discriminate unhindered. This paper is a call from behavior analysts to autistic self-advocates to heed Milton’s (2018) call for the full appreciation the double empathy problem and join together against the real nemesis of autistic people, societal and political discrimination, inertia, and indolence. Competing Interests Authors are Irish doctoral-level behavior analysts and identify as somewhere on the spectrum of human diversity. They have no financial or non-financial interests that are directly or indirectly related to the work submitted for publication. No funding was received for research reported here. Author Contributions Authors contributed equally to the paper. References ABA4All. (2022). ABA parent support group. Applied Behavior Analysis Available for All. https://www. abaa4all.com/ ABAI. (2022). Association for Behavior Analysis- International. Website. https://www. abainternational.org/ American Psychiatric Association. (2013). DSM-5. diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. http://bit.ly/2BEXru3 APBA. (2016). Identifying applied behavior analysis interventions. White Paper. Association of Professional Behavior Analyts. https://www. bacb.com/wp-content/uploads/APBA-2017- White-Paper-Identifying-ABA-Interventions1. pdf Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some current dimensions of applied behavior analysis1. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1), 1310980. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1968.1-91 Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1987). Some still- current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20(4), 313– 327. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1987.20-313 Baron-Cohen, S. (2014). What scientific idea is ready for retirement? Radical behaviourism. Edge. https://www.edge.org/response-detail/25473 Benzel, E. (2022). Dunning-Kruger Effect. In World Neurosurgery (Vol. 160). https://doi.org/10.1016/j. wneu.2022.02.046 Blakemore, L. (2021). The long road: Defending the rights of children on the autism spectrum for access to the science of ABA in the UK: In the community, the curriculum and the courtroom. Centre for Behaviour Analysis, Queen’s University Belfast. https://www.youtube.com/ watch?v=BZN-ZJWDePs&t=1317s Borowsky, H., Morinis, L., & Garg, M. (2021). Disability and ableism in medicine: A curriculum for medical students. MedEdPORTAL : The Journal of Teaching and Learning Resources, 17. https:// doi.org/10.15766/mep_2374-8265.11073 205 Autism and Behavior Analysis: From Dissonance to Dialogue / Dillenburger & Keenan Buivydaite, R., Newton, C. R., & Prasauskiene, A. (2017). Scoping review: Autism research in Baltic States—What is known and what is still to be studied. In Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (Vol. 4, Issue 4). https:// doi.org/10.1007/s40489-017-0114-4 Byrne, H., & Byrne, T. (2000). Mikey – Dealing with courts, tribunals and politicians. In M. Keenan, M. Henderson, K. Dillenburger, & K. P. Kerr (Eds.), Applied behaviour analysis and autism: Building a future together (pp. 208–218). London and Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Cambridge Centre for Behavior Studies. (2022). Cambridge Centre for Behavior Studies. https:// behavior.org Cambridge University Press. (2020). Cambridge Dictionary: English Dictionary, Translations & Thesaurus. In Cambridge University Press. Chiesa, M. (1994). Radical behaviorism: The philosophy and the science. In Radical behaviorism: The philosophy and the science. Chiesa, M. (2006). ABA is not “a therapy for autism.” In Applied Behaviour Analysis and Autism: Building a Future Together. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied Behavior Analysis. Applied Behavior Analysis, 2(2), 2–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/13937- 004 Dillenburger, K. (1991). Violent Bereavement: Widows in Northern Ireland. Avebury. https://www. amazon.co.uk/Violent-Bereavement-Widows- Northern-Ireland/dp/1856283003/ref=sr_1_1?ie= UTF8&qid=1540580787&sr=8-1&keywords=dillenb urger+bereavement Dillenburger, K. (2007). A behavior analytic perspective on victimology. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 3(3), 433– 448. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100817 Dillenburger, K., Akhonzada, R., & Fargas, M. (2006). Post-trauma: Is evidence-based practice a fantasy? International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 2(1), 94–106. https:// doi.org/10.1037/h0100770 Dillenburger, K., Fargas, M., & Akhonzada, R. (2007a). Psychological impact of long-term political violence: An exploration of community service users. Traumatology, 13(2), 15–25. https://doi. org/10.1177/1534765607302279 Dillenburger, K., Fargas, M., & Akhonzada, R. (2007b). The PAVE Project report: Exploring the effectiveness of services for victims of the Troubles in Northern Ireland. OFMDFM/QUB. http://cain.ulst.ac.uk/ v i ct i m s /d o c s /g ro u p/p a ve/p a ve_ p ro j e ct_ report_07.pdf Dillenburger, K., & Keenan, M. (2009). None of the As in ABA stand for autism: Dispelling the myths. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 34(2), 193–195. https://doi. org/10.1080/13668250902845244 Dillenburger, K., Keenan, M., Doherty, A., Byrne, T., & Gallagher, S. (2010). Living with children diagnosed with autistic spectrum disorder: parental and professional views. British Journal of Special Education, 37(1), 1–25. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1467-8578.2010.00455.x Dillenburger, K., Keenan, M., Doherty, A., Byrne, T., & Gallagher, S. (2012). ABA-Based Programs for Children Diagnosed With Autism Spectrum Disorder: Parental and Professional Experiences at School and at Home. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 34(2), 111–129. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/07317107.2012.684645 Dillenburger, K., Keenan, M., & Gallagher, S. (2015). A seat at the table: Families affected by Autism. Amazon Kindle. https://www.amazon.co.uk/ seat-table-Families-affected-Autism-ebook/ dp/B00UOXF3F0/ref=sr_1_1?ie=UTF8&qid=154049 0808&sr=8-1&keywords=dillenburger Dillenburger, K., McKerr, L., & Jordan, J.-A. (2014). Lost in translation: Public policies, evidence-based practice, and Autism Spectrum Disorder. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 61(2), 134–151. https://doi.org/10.1 080/1034912X.2014.905059 Fargas-Malet, M., & Dillenburger, K. (2016). Intergenerational Transmission of Conflict- Related Trauma in Northern Ireland: A Behavior Analytic Approach. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma, 25(4), 436–454. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2015.1107172 Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1962). A method for the experimental analysis of the behavior of autistic children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 32(1). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1962. tb00267.x Glenn, S. S. (1988). Contingencies and Metacontingencies: Toward a Synthesis of Behavior Analysis and Cultural Materialism. The Behavior Analyst, 11(2). https://doi.org/10.1007/ bf03392470 206 January 2023, Volume 15, Issue 3, 199-208 Glenn, S. S. (2004). Individual behavior, culture, and social change. Behavior Analyst, 27(2). https:// doi.org/10.1007/BF03393175 Gómez-Durán, E. L., Guija, J. A., & Ortega-Monasterio, L. (2014). Medical-legal issues of physical and pharmacological restraint. Medicina Clinica, 142(SUPPL. 2). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0025- 7753(14)70068-5 Graber, A., & Graber, J. (2023). Applied Behavior Analysis and the abolitionist neurodiversity critique: An ethical analysis. Behavior Analysis in Practice. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-023-00780-6 Green, G. (2016). Science, pseudoscience and antiscience theories In autism. Different Roads to Learning. https://blog.difflearn. com/2016/07/14/pseudoscience-antiscience- theories-autism/ Gunn, J. (2010). Dr Harold Frederick Shipman: An enigma. In Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health (Vol. 20, Issue 3, pp. 190–198). https://doi. org/10.1002/cbm.768 Heward, W. L., Critchfield, T. S., Reed, D. D., Detrich, R., & Kimball, J. W. (2022). ABA from A to Z: Behavior Science Applied to 350 Domains of Socially Significant Behavior. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 45(2), 327–359. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s40614-022-00336-z Hofer, G., Mraulak, V., Grinschgl, S., & Neubauer, A. C. (2022). Less-intelligent and unaware? Accuracy and Dunning–Kruger Effects for self-estimates of different aspects of intelligence. Journal of Intelligence, 10(1). https://doi.org/10.3390/ jintelligence10010010 Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen, H. G., Green, G., & Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26(4), 359–383. Howard, J. S., Stanislaw, H., Green, G., Sparkman, C. R., & Cohen, H. G. (2014). Comparison of behavior analytic and eclectic early interventions for young children with autism after three years. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(12), 3326–3344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. ridd.2014.08.021 Irish Post. (2022). “No Irish, no blacks, no dogs”. No proof? How the infamous “No Irish, no blacks, no dogs” signs may never have existed. Irish Post. Kazdin, A. E. (2010). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. Keenan, M. (2016). ABA and the nail that sticks out … ABA – klinec, ktorý vytŕča….ŠPECIÁLNY PEDAGÓG, Časopis pre špeciálnopedagogickú teóriu a prax, 5. ročník, 2016, č. 1. p. 3-14 (Slovakian Journal of Special Education) Keenan, M., & Dillenburger, K. (2000). Images of behavior analysis: The shaping game and the behavioral stream. Behavior and Social Issues, 10, 19–38. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v10i0.132 Keenan, M., & Dillenburger, K. (2020). Behaviour analysis: A primer. Celtic Fringe Productions. http://www. behaviouranalysis.eu.com Keenan, M., & Dillenburger, K. (2011). When all you have is a hammer ...: RCTs and hegemony in science. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.02.003 Keenan, M., & Dillenburger, K. (2020). Nurturing. Celtic Fringe Productions. https://www.youtube.com/ watch?v=TKVkA5uk7rQ&t=8s Keenan, M., Dillenburger, K., Doherty, A., Byrne, T., & Gallagher, S. (2010). The experiences of parents during diagnosis and forward planning for children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 23(4), 390–397. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468- 3148.2010.00555.x Keenan, M., Dillenburger, K., Konrad, M.-H., Debetencourt, N., Vuksan, R., Kourea, L., Pancocha, K., Kingsdorf, S., Brandtberg, H. J., Ozkan, N., Abdelnour, H., da Costa-Meranda, M., Schuldt, S., Mellon, R., Herman, A., Tennyson, A., Ayvazo, S., Moderato, P., Attard, N., … Gallagher, S. (2022). Professional development of behavior analysts in Europe: A snapshot for 21 countries, Behavior Analysis in Practice. Behavior Analysis in Practice. Keenan, M., Kerr, K. P., & Dillenburger, K. (2000). Parents education as autism therapists. Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Keenan, M., Kerr, K. P., Dillenburger, K., & Röttgers (Translator), H. R. (2000). Eltern als Therapeuten von Kindern mit Autismus-Spektrum-Störungen: Selbständigkeit fördern mit Applied Behaviour Analysis. Kohlhammer Verlag. https://www. a m a zo n .d e/ E l te r n -T h e ra p e u te n - K i n d e r n - Autismus-Spektrum-Störungen-Selbständigkeit- ebook/dp/B00PWI24I6/ref=sr_1_2?ie=UTF8&qid= 1538923294&sr=8-2&keywords=dillenburger Koegel, R. (2011). O. Ivar Lovaas (1927-2010). American Psychologist, 66(3), 227–228. https://doi. org/10.1037/a0022693 207 Autism and Behavior Analysis: From Dissonance to Dialogue / Dillenburger & Keenan Kupferstein, H. (2018). Evidence of increased PTSD symptoms in autistics exposed to applied behavior analysis. Advances in Autism, 00–00. https://doi.org/10.1108/AIA-08-2017-0016 Larsson, E. v. (2021). Is Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) and Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention (EIBI) an effective treatment for Autism? A cumulative review of impartial reports. https:// doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-2362. Leadbitter, K., Buckle, K. L., Ellis, C., & Dekker, M. (2021a). Autistic Self-Advocacy and the Neurodiversity Movement: Implications for Autism Early Intervention Research and Practice. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fpsyg.2021.635690 Leadbitter, K., Buckle, K. L., Ellis, C., & Dekker, M. (2021b). Autistic Self-Advocacy and the Neurodiversity Movement: Implications for Autism Early Intervention Research and Practice. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fpsyg.2021.635690 Lipinski, S., Boegl, K., Blanke, E. S., Suenkel, U., & Dziobek, I. (2022). A blind spot in mental healthcare? Psychotherapists lack education and expertise for the support of adults on the autism spectrum. Autism, 26(6). https://doi. org/10.1177/13623613211057973 Lord, C., Charman, T., Havdahl, A., Carbone, P., Anagnostou, E., Boyd, B., Carr, T., de Vries, P. J., Dissanayake, C., Divan, G., Freitag, C. M., Gotelli, M. M., Kasari, C., Knapp, M., Mundy, P., Plank, A., Scahill, L., Servili, C., Shattuck, P., … McCauley, J. B. (2022). The Lancet Commission on the future of care and clinical research in autism. In The Lancet (Vol. 399, Issue 10321). https://doi. org/10.1016/S0140-6736(21)01541-5 Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. Milton, D. (2012). The normalisation agenda and the psycho-emotional disablement of autistic people. In Autonomy, the Critical Journal of Interdisciplinary Autism Studies (Vol. 1, Issue 1). Milton, D. (2018). The double empathy problem . National Autistic Society, 3–2. https://www. a u t i s m . o r g . u k / a d v i c e - a n d - g u i d a n c e / professional-practice/double-empathy Nario-Redmond, M. R., Kemerling, A. A., & Silverman, A. (2019). Hostile, Benevolent, and Ambivalent Ableism: Contemporary Manifestations. Journal of Social Issues, 75(3). https://doi.org/10.1111/ josi.12337 NICE. (2013). National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence Autism in Children and Young People Guideline Consultation Comments Table 29 March 2013-10 May 2013. https://www. n i c e.o rg .u k /g u i d a n c e/c g 1 70/d o c u m e nts / autism-management-of-autism-in-children- and-young-people-consultation-comments- table-and-responses2 Nielson, S., Bray, L., Carter, B., & Kiernan, J. (2021). Physical restraint of children and adolescents in mental health inpatient services: A systematic review and narrative synthesis. Journal of Child Health Care, 25(3). https://doi. org/10.1177/1367493520937152 Parker, S. (2015). Autism: does ABA therapy open society’s doors to children, or impose conformity? The Guardian, 03(20). https://www.theguardian. com/society/2015/mar/20/autism-does-aba- therapy-open-societys-doors-to-children-or- impose-conformity Phelps, B. J. (2007). Why we are still not cognitive psychologists: A review of Why I am not a cognitive psychologist: A tribute to B. F. Skinner. The Behavior Analyst, 30(2), 217–226. Razzetti, G. (2018). There are two ways to lead people – Divide and conquer Is one of them. Fearless Culture. https://www.fearlessculture.design/ blog-posts/dividing-people-is-the-best-way- to-lead Sandoval-Norton, A. H., & Shkedy, G. (2019). How much compliance is too much compliance: Is long- term ABA therapy abuse? In Cogent Psychology (Vol. 6, Issue 1). https://doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2 019.1641258 Schnitzer, K., Merideth, F., Macias-Konstantopoulos, W., Hayden, D., Shtasel, D., & Bird, S. (2020). Disparities in Care: The Role of Race on the Utilization of Physical Restraints in the Emergency Setting. Academic Emergency Medicine, 27(10). https:// doi.org/10.1111/acem.14092 Silberman, S. (2015). Neurotribes: The legacy of autism and how to think smarter about people who think differently. Allen & Unwin. 208 January 2023, Volume 15, Issue 3, 199-208 Simple Steps. (2013). Simple Steps Autism: The online teaching platform for the treatment of Autism. Parents Education as Autism Therapists. https:// simplestepsautism.com/ Skinner, B. F. (1953a). Operant behavior. Science and Human Behavior. https://doi.org/10.3390/ ijerph8093528 Skinner, B. F. (1953b). Science and human behaviour. In The Macmillan Company. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. Verbal Behavior. Skinner, B. F. (1977). Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism; Published by Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies (CCBS), 5(2), 1–10. Smith, T., & Eikeseth, S. (2011). O. Ivar Lovaas: Pioneer of applied behavior analysis and intervention for children with autism. In Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (Vol. 41, Issue 3, pp. 375–378). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010- 1162-0 Surgeon General. (1999). Mental health: A report of the Surgeon General. National Institute of Mental Health (U.S.). https://profiles.nlm.nih.gov/ps/ retrieve/ResourceMetadata/NNBBHS Tarvainen, M. (2019). Ableism and the life stories of people with disabilities. Scandinavian Journal of Disability Research, 21(1). https://doi. org/10.16993/sjdr.632 Thorne, J., & Barr, G. (2022). Then Barbara met Alan. BBC iPlayer . https://www.bbc.co.uk/iplayer/ episode/m0015p4q/then-barbara-met-alan Thyer, B. A. (2009). Epistemology: A behavior analytic perspective. . Humana.Mente. Https://Www. H u m a n a m e n te . E u / I n d ex . P h p/ H M / I s s u e/ View/11/Issue_11, 11(11), 45–64. Thyer, B. A., & Myers, L. L. (1999). On science, antiscience, and the client’s right to effective treatment. Social Work, 44(5). https://doi.org/10.1093/ sw/44.5.501 Thyer, B. A., & Pignotti, M. (2010). Science and pseudoscience in developmental disabilities: Guidelines for social workers. Journal of Social Work in Disability and Rehabilitation, 9(2). https://doi.org/10.1080/1536710X.2010.493480 UNCRPD. (2006). United Nations Convention for Persons with Disabilities. United Nations. https:// www.un.org/development/desa/disabilities/ convention-on-the-rights-of-persons-with- disabilities.html Unumb, L. (2013). Keynote address by Dr Lorri Unumb at Centre for Behaviour Analysis (QUB) conference. Queen’s University Belfast. h t t p s : // m e d i a s i te . q u b . a c . u k / M e d i a s i te / Play/564a563b432440b68375d102170c337a1d Volkmar, F. R., Bregman, J., Cohen, D. J., & Cicchetti, D. v. (1988). DSM-III and DSM-III-R diagnoses of autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145(11), 1404–1408. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.145.11.1404 Vyse, S. (2022). Autism politics and the death of truth and freedom. Skeptical Inquirer. The Magazine for Science and Reason. https:// skepticalinquirer.org/exclusive/autism-politics- and-the-death-of-truth-and-freedom/ .