IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

35 

(Im)mobilities and life satisfaction in times of 
COVID-19: The case of older persons in Switzerland 

 
 
Iuna Dones1,2,3, Ruxandra Oana Ciobanu1,3, Marie Baeriswyl 3,4 
 
1Faculty of Social Work, University of Applied Sciences and Arts Western Switzerland, (HETSL | HES-SO), Lausanne, 
Switzerland; 2Institute of Sociological Research (ISR), University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland; 3 Swiss Centre of 
Expertise in Life Course Research, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland;4 Centre for the Interdisciplinary 
Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland 
 
Corresponding author: I. Dones (iuna.dones@hetsl.ch) 
 
ABSTRACT  
 
The COVID-19 pandemic led to changes in mobilities worldwide. Physical movement and social contact are shown 
to be correlated to life satisfaction. In this paper we are interested in how during the pandemic other types of 
mobilities relate to life satisfaction. The paper draws on a survey (N=643) among persons aged 65+ in Switzerland. 
Results show that engagement in communicative mobilities are related to higher life satisfaction, imaginative 
mobilities only partially relate to life satisfaction, and virtual mobilities have no correlation. 
 
KEYWORDS 
COVID-19, communicative mobilities, imaginative mobilities, virtual mobilities 
 
FUNDING SOURCE 
This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation Professorship Grant ‘Transnational Ageing 
among Older Migrants and Natives: A Strategy to Overcome Vulnerability’, grant number PP00P1_179077/1. 
 
INTRODUCTION 
 
The End of Mobility as We Know It 
 
For long we have taken physical mobility for granted. 
Most of the research has been focusing on the hyper-
mobile society (Musselwhite et al., 2015; 
Musselwhite & Haddad, 2010). There is an abundance 
of scholarship looking at various types of physical 
mobilities and how these are associated to higher 
subjective well-being for younger and older persons 
(De Vos et al., 2013; Nordbakke & Schwanen, 2014; 
Schwanen & Ziegler, 2011; Ziegler & Schwanen, 
2011). In relation to older persons in particular, the 
independence and autonomy of exercising physical 
mobility contributes to this population’s subjective 
well-being (Schwanen & Ziegler, 2011). But what 
happens if these taken-for-granted mobilities, from 
the basic ones of going out for a walk or going grocery 
shopping, taking public transport to visit friends, all 

the way to taking a plane or a train for the holidays 
are disrupted by a global pandemic? 
 
     The COVID-19 pandemic has made the daily news 
and invaded our lives since early spring 2020. In 
Switzerland, the decision was to have a partial 
lockdown: shops and services that were not providing 
or responding to basic needs were closed, social 
gatherings were limited to a maximum of five 
persons, and people were advised to leave the house 
only if necessary. Persons aged 65 and over, and 
those with an underlying health condition were 
identified as especially vulnerable and were 
particularly recommended to remain secluded and 
avoid contact with others. Because of the absence of 
a complete lockdown like, for instance, the ones in 
Italy or France, we decided to use the terms partial 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

36 

confinement and semi-confinement throughout this 
paper. 
 
     The literature acknowledges that there are various 
forms of mobilities which are not limited to the 
physical ones. In this paper we draw on the definition 
of Ziegler and Schwanen (2011), who “conceptualize 
mobilities as the overcoming of any type of distance 
between a here and a there, which can be situated in 
physical, electronic, social, psychological or other 
kinds of space” (Ziegler & Schwanen, 2011, p. 758). In 
this paper we look at the alternative forms of 
mobilities in which older persons engaged during a 
period of reduced physical mobility or even 
immobility, and the extent to which these forms of 
mobility are related to life satisfaction. 
 
     We rooted the conceptualization of our survey in 
the new mobilities paradigm, which approaches 
mobilities in a broad sense. It considers mobilities as 
not being only limited to corporeal movement or 
travel, but also as encompassing other forms of 
mobility (Hannam et al., 2006; Sheller & Urry, 2006). 
According to Urry (2007), mobilities comprise 
imaginative travel through media, photos, books, and 
television; virtual travel via the Internet; 
communicative travel using various technologies, 
such as text messages, Skype, e-mails, and so on, the 
physical movement of objects, and corporeal travel. 
 
     Confronted with a unique situation, that of the 
COVID-19 pandemic, which has led to restrictive 
measures in most of the world – some marked by 
recommended confinement, and others by a total 
lockdown – people were compelled to adapt their 
behaviors, often reducing physical mobility, and 
engage in other forms of mobilities. Therefore, our 
research question is: which forms of alternative 
mobilities are associated with older persons’ life 
satisfaction? 
 
     The new mobilities paradigm has already been 
applied to older populations based on the assumption 
that once they experience a decrease in physical 
mobility due to old age, they can compensate by 
engaging in other forms of alternative mobilities 
(Ciobanu & Hunter, 2017; Ziegler & Schwanen, 2011). 
Therefore, given that COVID-19 semi-confinement 
was particularly focused on limiting older persons’ 
physical mobility to protect them from infection, 

studying other forms of mobilities in this context and 
in this population is particularly pertinent. 
 
     The paper is innovative in the following two ways: 
1) Most of the literature on the link between 
mobilities and different indicators of subjective well-
being has been focused on physical mobilities, and 
there is a paucity of research exploring other forms of 
mobilities – particularly virtual and imaginative – and 
their relation to life satisfaction, which we aim to 
contribute to; 2) We draw on an original survey 
conducted during this unusual health crisis. 
 
     The article is structured into five parts: we start 
with a theoretical anchoring of our paper in the 
literature on communicative, imaginative, and virtual 
mobilities and subjective well-being, then we present 
our data and methods, followed by our empirical 
findings, the discussion of our results, and finally the 
conclusions of the paper. 
 
CONCEPTUAL UNDERPINNING 
 
On Mobilities and Subjective Well-Being: A 
Theoretical Framework 
 
In the following section we discuss the concept of 
subjective well-being and how it has been linked to 
different forms of mobilities stemming from the new 
mobilities paradigm (Hannam et al., 2006; Sheller & 
Urry, 2006; Urry, 2007). 
 
     Subjective well-being is defined as “a person’s 
cognitive and affective evaluations of his or her life as 
a whole” (Oishi et al., 2018, p. 1). This includes the 
personal perceptions and experiences of positive and 
negative emotions, as well as global and specific 
cognitive evaluations of life satisfaction (Proctor, 
2014). Although the literature on subjective well-
being and alternative mobilities types is scarce, the 
research that does exist largely focuses on the 
cognitive dimension (Chai & Kalyal, 2019; Heo et al., 
2015). Therefore, we opted to use the concept of life 
satisfaction, a cognitive component of subjective 
well-being. Throughout the text, we use the terms life 
satisfaction and subjective well-being 
interchangeably. 
 
     There are two approaches to subjective well-being: 
the universalist and the contextualist. The former 
holds that subjective well-being is stable and 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

37 

independent of time and place. The latter holds that 
subjective well-being is rooted in people’s past 
experiences, aspirations, culture, and more generally 
context (Nordbakke & Schwanen, 2014). Given the 
existing research on the relationship between 
different factors – like health, social contact, physical 
mobility, etc. – and subjective well-being (De Vos et 
al., 2013; Deaton, 2008; Kööts-Ausmees & Realo, 
2015; Nordbakke & Schwanen, 2014; Pinquart & 
Sorensen, 2000; Revord et al., 2018; Schwanen & 
Ziegler, 2011; Ziegler & Schwanen, 2011), we adhere 
to the contextual approach. 
 
     Studies exploring particularly the impact of the 
COVID-19 pandemic on subjective well-being have 
also adopted this contextual approach. Pedraza and 
colleagues (2020) for instance, found that state-
mandated measures aimed at restricting physical 
mobilities were associated with increased life 
dissatisfaction and anxiety. These state-mandated 
measures were also perceived as a type of social 
isolation, which had important negative 
consequences for individuals’ subjective well-being 
(Anastasiou & Duquenne, 2021; Brooks et al., 2020; 
Clair et al., 2021). Some research has studied how, 
despite the negative correlation between reduced 
physical mobility and subjective well-being, 
individuals – and older adults in particular – were able 
to cope with this period. It was found that the most 
common coping strategies among older adults 
included outdoor activities, exercise, adhering to 
COVID-19 precautions, hobbies like reading, cooking, 
or listening to audiobooks, and social connection 
through various forms of communication (Finlay et 
al., 2021; Whitehead & Torossian, 2021). 
 
Communicative Mobility 
 
     If physical mobility can be used to reach out and 
meet others (Ziegler & Schwanen, 2011), similarly 
communicative mobility is a means to establish and 
maintain contact with friends and family. Both 
telephones and internet-based communication 
appear to be key in maintaining contact. Looking at 
Internet use, Heo and colleagues (2015) observe that 
this constitutes a tool for older persons to be in 
contact with others and exchange emotional support, 
which in its turn can lead to higher psychological well-
being and life satisfaction. It is particularly the social 
component of Internet-mediated communication 
that brings about well-being and happiness (Chai & 

Kalyal, 2019), and this even among the oldest-old 
(Sims et al., 2016). In this sense, the use of telephones 
also increases social connectedness and subjective 
well-being (Chai & Kalyal, 2019). The importance of 
communicative mobility has been studied also for 
older international migrants, for whom 
communication at a distance, mediated using 
technology, has been positively linked to quality of life 
(Zhang, 2016). 
 
     When comparing active to passive Internet use and 
their relation to life satisfaction, researchers found 
that active use for communication purposes is 
positively correlated to life satisfaction after the age 
of 63, while passive use for non-communicative 
activities like looking at content on a smartphone is 
negatively correlated to life satisfaction, and these 
relationships remain significant after controlling for 
social network size and interactions (Stevic et al., 
2019). Stevic and colleagues (2019) explained the 
positive effect of the use of smartphones for 
communicative purposes among retirement-age 
individuals through the social compensation 
hypothesis and the social convoy model. With 
retirement, individuals experience a reduction in their 
social convoy, that is the various social relations 
(partner and family relations, friends, and neighbors 
and work colleagues) that accompany a person 
throughout their life course (Antonucci et al., 2013). 
The social compensation hypothesis holds that 
“individuals compensate for the lack of face-to-face 
friendships by extending their online social sphere” 
(Stevic et al., 2019, p. 3), with positive implications for 
their life satisfaction. 
 
     Moreover, a study particularly relevant for our 
research shows that the use of information and 
communications technology (ICT) for older persons 
aged 75 and over can increase subjective well-being 
through its facilitation of contact with family, which is 
especially important for frail persons (Fang et al., 
2018). Specifically, during the first wave of the COVID-
19 pandemic, increased interaction with friends and 
families through various forms of communication like 
phone calls, text messages, and video calls, was 
shown to be an important coping strategy for older 
adults (Finlay et al., 2021; Whitehead & Torossian, 
2021). In fact, a   study on older persons in Germany 
revealed that, during the first period of the pandemic, 
individuals who used the internet less frequently to 
stay in contact with friends and family reported lower 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

38 

life satisfaction, increased loneliness, and more 
depressive symptoms than daily users (Hajek & König, 
2021). Nonetheless, the relationship between 
different forms of communication and life satisfaction 
during the COVID-19 pandemic is still not widely 
documented, and our research aims to fill this gap. 
 
     Following from this literature review, our first 
hypothesis is that frequent engagement in 
communicative mobility practices is associated with 
higher life satisfaction because it allows persons to 
stay in contact with others. 
 
Imaginative Mobility 
 
     In the category of imaginative mobility, one can 
include looking at photos, looking outside the 
window, media consumption, reading literature, and 
so on (Urry, 2007). Looking from the window appears 
to be a very important activity, particularly for those 
unable to leave their homes and for those whose 
physical mobilities are very limited (Musselwhite, 
2018; Rowles, 1981). Drawing on qualitative 
interviews, Musselwhite (2018) shows that older 
persons with physical mobility impairments engage 
and indirectly participate in the outside community 
through window watching. Musselwhite (2018) 
investigates how this view from the inside to the 
outside helps these individuals “stay somewhat 
connected to the outside space that they cannot 
physically inhabit” (p. 274). The view from the 
window – whether rural or urban, whether marked by 
the presence or absence of passers-by – “served as a 
way of creating representations of life and help 
[individuals] engage and reflect on changes in 
lifestyles, it kept them part of society” (Musselwhite, 
2018, p. 280). At the same time, looking from the 
window can also allow for indirect contact with 
nature, which enhances well-being (Kaplan Mintz et 
al., 2021; Kaplan, 2001). In fact, a quantitative study 
conducted during the first COVID-19 lockdown in 
Israel on adults of all ages demonstrated that 
individuals with high levels of nature views from 
windows reported higher subjective well-being than 
those with less nature views (Kaplan Mintz et al., 
2021). 
 
     In a similar way, as physical connections to the 
outside world become more difficult with old age – 
and were restricted during our study’s period of 
partial confinement – they can be replaced by 

memories and imaginative connections, which can 
also be formed through looking at photographs 
(Musselwhite, 2018). These feelings of inclusion in 
society enable people to experience a sense of 
belonging, even at a distance (Gehl, 2011), which in 
turn contributes to their life satisfaction (Massey et 
al., 2021). Despite this concept of belonging from a 
distance, and the importance of window views for 
individuals’ well-being (Kaplan Mintz et al., 2021; 
Peters & Halleran, 2021), not many studies have 
investigated the role of these imaginative mobilities 
in older adults’ life satisfaction during the COVID-19 
pandemic. 
 
     Our second hypothesis is therefore that frequent 
engagement in imaginative mobility practices is 
correlated to higher life satisfaction. 
 
Virtual Mobility 
 
     To the best of our knowledge, there is no research 
linking virtual mobility using virtual travel or virtual 
museum visits to subjective well-being. While not 
focused on measures of subjective well-being, 
Winstead and colleagues (2013) looked at how social 
and spatial barriers are affected by the use of ICTs in 
assisted and independent living communities. They 
observed that physically bound participants who 
enjoyed visiting museums earlier in their lives 
appreciated having the newly discovered ability to 
look at art on the Internet. Moreover, older 
individuals used Google Maps to travel back to their 
hometowns and visit places they were attached to 
and described these activities in a positive manner. 
Because of these positive experiences among 
research participants, we can infer that virtual 
mobility may have a positive impact on life 
satisfaction. This therefore leads us to our third 
hypothesis: frequent engagement in virtual mobility 
practices is associated with higher life satisfaction. 
 
METHODS 
 
The paper draws on an original survey on the impact 
of the COVID-19 pandemic on the situation of persons 
aged 65 and over in Switzerland. We developed a 
survey in French and put it online through the 
platform LimeSurvey. The questionnaire was 
submitted and accepted by the Ethics Committee of 
the Faculty of Social Sciences of the University of 
Geneva. To obtain informed consent, we inserted one 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

39 

question asking respondents if they accepted to have 
their answers used for scientific publications and 
presentations in conferences. 
 
     We applied a convenience sampling method and 
distributed the link to the questionnaire through the 
Platform of associations for older persons in Geneva, 
the University of Geneva website, one of the local 
newspapers in Geneva, and widely in our personal 
and institutional networks. The reason for the 
adoption of a convenience sampling method is the 
coronavirus health crisis, which made it difficult, if not 
impossible, to recruit and talk to participants in 
person, as well as the time constraints to gather 
information while the pandemic restrictions were still 
in place. 
 
     Data were collected from April 18 to May 19, 2020, 
which fell within the period of recommended limited 
mobility in Switzerland, during which a semi-
confinement was in place. The resulting data were 
cleaned and analyzed with the use of SPSS statistical 
software. 
 
     In total, 787 individuals 65 and older participated 
to the research, either by completing the 
questionnaire themselves (n= 748) or with the help of 
a friend or relative with internet access (n=39). 
Among these, we excluded from the working sample 
74 people who did not respond to all the Satisfaction 
with Life Scale items, and an additional 70 who did not 
respond to other interest variables included in our 
analysis, leaving us with 643 participants. Out of them 
67.3% are women and 32.7% men. The mean age in 
our working sample is 73.3 years old, with a standard 
deviation of 5.75. 
     Because of the convenience sampling method, our 
sample of individuals 65 and older is composed of an 
overrepresentation of individuals with a tertiary level 
of education (65.6% had a tertiary level of education 
and 29.9% a secondary level, in comparison to the 
Swiss population of 65 and over, with 20.2% at a 
tertiary education level and 49.1% at a secondary 
level) (FSO, 2018). See Table 1 for more descriptive 
statistics on the composition of our sample. 
 
Measures 
 
     Our dependent variable is life satisfaction, 
measured by the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener 
et al., 1985), a 5-item scale assessing the cognitive 

dimension of subjective well-being, and one of the 
most frequently used scales in subjective well-being 
research (Maddux, 2018). The scale is composed of 
the following items: 
 
1. In most ways my life is close to my ideal. 
2. The conditions of my life are excellent. 
3. I am satisfied with my life. 
4. So far, I have gotten the important things I 

want in life. 
5. If I could live my life over, I would change 

almost nothing. 
 
     Participants were instructed to indicate their level 
of agreement with each item on a 7-point scale, 
ranging from “1=strongly disagree” to “7=strongly 
agree”. The answers for each item were then added 
to create an individual score, ranging from 5 to 35. 
Cronbach’s alpha was 0.87. Our sample had a mean 
life satisfaction score of 28.8 and standard deviation 
of 5.0. These results were not very different from the 
Swiss population of 65 and older as another study on 
a representative sample of the Swiss population of 65 
and older showed a mean life satisfaction score of 
26.8 and standard deviation of 5.4 (Baeriswyl & Oris, 
2021). The slight difference between the mean life 
satisfaction of our sample and that of the Swiss 
population 65 and older can be attributed to the 
composition of our sample: we had an 
overrepresentation of healthy and highly educated 
individuals, and these aspects tend to positively 
influence life satisfaction (Diener et al., 1999). 
 
     Our key independent variables are indicators of 
communicative mobility, imaginative mobility, and 
virtual mobility during the partial confinement 
period. We used two separate questions for each type 
of mobility. 
 
     Because for the independent variables that we 
study – alternative forms of mobilities – there are no 
validated questions, to the best of our knowledge, we 
therefore elaborated ourselves the questions, taking 
inspiration in the existing literature (Ciobanu & 
Hunter, 2017; Urry, 2007). 
 
     For communicative mobility we asked, “In the past 
week, how often have you spoken on the phone?” as 
well as, “Last week, how often did you use digital 
means to communicate with friends or family?”. For 
imaginative mobility we asked, “People sometimes 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

40 

spend time looking out the window. This may be 
looking at passers-by, street life or nature. In the past 
week, how often have you done this?” and, “In the 
past week, how often have you looked at photos?”. 
Possible answer categories for these questions were 
“Never”, “Once during the week”, “Two to three 
times during the week”, “Four to six times during the 
week”, “Every day, less than three hours” and “Every 
day, three hours or more”. We then recoded the first 
two answer choices as “Once a week or less”, the next 
two as “Several times a week” and the last two as 
“Every day”. For virtual mobility we asked, “In the 
past week, have you visited any museums and/or 
exhibitions on the Internet?” as well as, “In the past 
week, have you researched possible travel 
destinations on the Internet?”. Answer choices were 
“Yes” or “No”. See Table 2 for descriptive statistics of 
these variables. 
 
     Basing ourselves on the life satisfaction and 
mobilities literature, we controlled our analyses of 
the links between life satisfaction and alternative 
mobilities for the following sociodemographic 
variables: sex, age, education level, relationship 
status, whether the participant has children, and self-
rated health. We coded education level into three 
categories: without post-compulsory training, 
secondary, and tertiary. We coded relationship status 
in two categories: single individuals, and individuals in 
a partnership (whether married or in another form of 
relationship). We measured self-rated health by 
asking participants, “How do you rate your health in 
general?”. Answer choices were “Very good”, 
“Good”, “Fairly good”, “Bad”, and “Very bad”. We 
then created a dichotomous variable with the first 
two categories signifying good health status, and the 
last three representing average/bad health status. 
 
     In addition, we controlled our analyses for the two 
following personal resources variables: praying and 
interaction with neighbors during the partial 
confinement period. We included the act of praying 
because religiosity has been found to positively 
impact life satisfaction (Amit, 2010), and we 
measured it as a dichotomous variable (has not 
prayed the previous week, has prayed the previous 
week). We also included interaction with neighbors as 
a form of social contact during a time when contact 
was limited due to partial confinement, as social 
contact has been found to be positively correlated 
with life satisfaction (Warr et al., 2004). We measured 

this by asking participants which of the following 
interactions they have had with their neighbors: 
“None”, “Said hello”, “Courtesy visits”, “Helped each 
other”. We then created a dichotomous variable by 
grouping the first two categories in “Minimal to no 
interaction” and the latter two in “Strong 
interaction”. Moreover, we controlled our analyses 
for self-related health and level of education. 
 
     We did not control for physical mobility because 
most of our participants (92.7%) went out of the 
house at least once during the semi-confinement, 
whether it was to walk the dog, go to the garden, or 
go grocery shopping. Despite this ability to go out, the 
restrictions on physical mobility were undeniable, as 
nonessential businesses were closed and face-to-face 
meetings were strongly discouraged. 
 
Data Analysis 
 
     We ran two linear regression models on the life 
satisfaction score. In the first model, we tested the 6 
mobility variables, and in the second we added the 
sociodemographic and personal resources variables 
as control variables. See Table 3 for the final models 
of the regression results. 
 
RESULTS 
 
Most of our working sample engaged in forms of 
communicative mobilities during partial confinement: 
53.8% reported talking on the phone every day, 42.6% 
did the activity several times a week, while only 3.6% 
did this once a week or less. Moreover, 68.6% of our 
sample used digital means to communicate every day, 
26.0% did so several times a week, and 5.4% did so 
once a week or less. 
 
     Engagement in imaginative mobilities during 
partial confinement varied: 36.4% reported looking 
out the window every day, 32.3% did this several 
times a week, and 31.3% did this once a week or less. 
The other measure of imaginative mobility – looking 
at photos – was less prominent among our 
participants: 8.1% did this every day, 28.9% did this 
several times a week, while 63.0% did this once a 
week or less. 
 
     Most of our working sample did not engage in 
imaginative mobilities during this period: only 18.8% 
reported having visited museums or expositions on 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

41 

the internet, and only 12.8% reported having 
searched travel destinations on the internet. 
 
     Table 3 shows that the importance of 
communicative mobility linked to life satisfaction is 
confirmed, supporting our first hypothesis. The 
analysis shows that individuals who talked on the 
phone once a week or less reported lower life 
satisfaction in comparison to those who talked on the 
phone every day. Moreover, those who use digital 
means to communicate with friends and family only 
once a week or less report lower life satisfaction than 
those who do so every day. These relationships 
remain significant even after controlling for 
sociodemographic and personal resources variables. 
 
     The results for the relationship between 
imaginative mobility and life satisfaction vary. 
Looking outside the window during this period is 
significantly associated to life satisfaction. Those who 
looked out the window every day reported higher life 
satisfaction than those who did it several times a 
week, but there is no significant difference between 
those who only did the activity once a week or less 
and those who did it every day. On the other hand, 
looking at photos during the partial confinement 
period is not significantly related to life satisfaction.  
Our second hypothesis is thus only partially 
confirmed. 
 
     In terms of virtual mobility, neither of the two 
virtual mobility variables are significantly correlated 
with life satisfaction. Our third hypothesis is therefore 
rejected. 
 
     Other variables that are significantly and positively 
linked to life satisfaction are being in a relationship 
and being in good health. Having had strong 
interactions with neighbors and having prayed during 
the partial confinement are also positively correlated 
with life satisfaction, but at a significance level 
between 0.05 and 0.10. 
 
DISCUSSION 
 
The Importance of Communicative Mobilities 
 
Whether directly or indirectly, all forms of mobilities 
are seen as means of connecting to others and 
connecting to our environment, but our study shows 
that among individuals 65 and older, it is primarily 

communicating with others that positively correlates 
to life satisfaction during the public health crisis. As 
Stevic and colleagues (2019) and Hajek and König 
(2021) found, smartphone and internet use for 
communicative purposes is positively associated with 
life satisfaction. Our study confirms this relationship 
between internet use and life satisfaction, if internet 
use implies communicative purposes, for we also 
found that internet use for visiting museums or 
researching travel destinations has no correlation to 
life satisfaction. 
 
     During a period in which physical socialization was 
severely restricted and in which older individuals 
were deemed a vulnerable population at risk of 
severe consequences due to COVID-19, this study 
highlights the importance of communicative 
mobilities that promote social support and social 
connectedness, whether through Internet-based 
means or more classic telephone communication. 
Social support, which can be conceptualized either as 
“the actual transfer of advice, aid, and affect through 
interpersonal networks during a specific period of 
time” (Liang et al., 2001, p. 512) or as “a perception 
of hypothetical resource availability” (Liang et al., 
2001, p. 512) has been shown to be a predictor of life 
satisfaction in older adulthood and in general across 
all ages (Aquino et al., 1996; Siedlecki et al., 2014). 
This paper underlines that, during a pandemic that 
physically drove individuals apart, social 
connectedness is still possible using communicative 
mobilities, and the more frequent use of these 
mobilities is correlated to higher life satisfaction. 
 
     The importance of social interaction for life 
satisfaction is further shown by two of our control 
variables: being in a relationship and, to a lesser 
extent, having had a strong interaction with 
neighbors during the partial confinement period. This 
confirms the existing literature stating that having 
meaningful relationships is important to life 
satisfaction (Baeriswyl & Oris, 2021; Revord et al., 
2018). 
 
     To the best of our knowledge, only one study 
(Kaplan Mintz et al., 2021) has investigated the 
relationship between window views and subjective 
well-being during COVID-19, but it focuses primarily 
on the presence of nature in the window views, rather 
than on the activity of looking outside the window 
itself. Moreover, the study does not specify the 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

42 

frequency of participants’ window watching, and it 
includes adults of all ages instead of focusing on older 
adults, for which the implications of window watching 
on their subjective well-being may be different. Our 
paper therefore adds an additional element to the 
literature on the subject. 
 
     We therefore base ourselves on Musselwhite’s 
(2018) study, which states that older persons 
indirectly participate in the outside community 
through window watching. Our results show that 
those who looked out the window every day reported 
a higher life satisfaction than those who did the 
activity several times a week. We can presume that 
looking out the window more often can be likened to 
greater social participation, albeit indirect, and since 
social participation is correlated with subjective well-
being measures (Baeriswyl & Oris, 2021), this can 
explain these results. However, if looking out the 
window is a proxy for indirect social participation, we 
would expect that those who looked out the window 
only once a week or less would also report lower life 
satisfaction in comparison to those who did it every 
day, but this was not the case. One way to explain this 
is to presume that those who rarely or never look out 
the window do not have a need for this indirect social 
participation, and this activity is therefore not related 
to their life satisfaction. 
 
     For older persons with reduced mobility, looking 
outside the window is an important pass time activity. 
In a period of reduced physical mobility, as was the 
semi-confinement, it became even more important. 
According to Musselwhite (2018), it is less important 
what somebody sees, rather it is important to 
broaden one’s horizon to the space and environment 
outside one’s home, which can be experienced as a 
limited space during this period. However, according 
to Kaplan Mintz and colleagues’ study (2021), what 
somebody sees is important indeed: windows with 
greater views of nature are related to higher levels of 
subjective well-being. Our study did not include 
variables on the type of scenery viewed from 
participants’ windows. Nonetheless, whether the 
views are of nature or of urban settings, by looking 
outside from the window, people can project 
themselves in mobility, and when seeing others 
outside, those who are immobile can experience a 
form of mobility by proxy. 
 

     Once again, we have not found any research on the 
relationship between looking at photos – our second 
imaginative mobility variable – and life satisfaction. It 
is therefore difficult to explain the absence of 
relationship between these two variables. We can 
however think that looking at photos can generate 
two different reactions: one the one hand it can 
trigger feelings of nostalgia, which have been found 
to be positively correlated with life satisfaction (Rao 
et al., 2018), as they “may render the present self 
more positive and promote a brighter outlook on the 
future” (Ye et al., 2018, p. 1749). On the other hand, 
it can act as a reminder of the physical divisiveness 
brought about by the pandemic. These opposing 
reactions may thus cancel each other out and explain 
the non-significance of this type of imaginative 
mobility in relation to life satisfaction. 
 
     The lack of correlation between either virtual 
mobility variable and life satisfaction can be explained 
by the fact that neither of these mobility variables 
concern any type of social participation or social 
connectedness. 
 
     Moreover, we find good self-reported health to be 
positively associated with life satisfaction, which 
replicates existing research findings (Deaton, 2008; 
Kööts-Ausmees & Realo, 2015; Pinquart & Sorensen, 
2000). And lastly, having prayed during partial 
confinement is positively associated with life 
satisfaction, which is congruent to the found 
relationship between religiosity and our dependent 
variable (Amit, 2010). 
 
     The general hypothesis that we started with, that 
mobilities as conceived by the New Mobilities 
Paradigm (Urry, 2007) can constitute important 
resources, notably in the context of reduced physical 
mobility imposed by the semi-confinement and that 
it would have an impact on well-being, is not globally 
confirmed. Yet, when looking at virtual, 
communicative, and imaginative mobilities, we 
conclude that what is of importance to life 
satisfaction in this pandemic context is social 
interaction, whether direct or indirect.   
 
LIMITATIONS AND CONCLUSION 
 
The COVID-19 pandemic and the partial confinement 
that was linked with it provided us with a unique 
situation. Researchers in social sciences quickly 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

43 

mobilized to study the impact of the public health 
crisis at the micro, meso, and macro levels (Gamba et 
al., 2020). The same variables that have shaped 
individuals’ lives have also had an impact on the 
conditions under which we conducted research. The 
main limitation of this study is the fact that the 
sample on which it is based is not representative, as 
we were not able to capture the entire diversity of 
Switzerland’s socio-economic levels. To accurately 
analyze both subjective evaluations and objective 
behaviors, it was essential to collect our data during 
the semi-confinement. Because of this, we had to rely 
on a convenience sampling method, which involved 
an internet-based survey. Consequently, our 
respondents were persons with internet connection, 
or who were interviewed by friends or family. Despite 
our awareness of this sample limitation, this was the 
only way we could reach individuals aged 65 and over 
given the time and contextual restraints. 
 
     Another limitation is that our study does not 
investigate whether communicative, imaginative, and 
virtual mobilities compensate for physical ones, as we 
do not analyze the change in mobilities before partial 
confinement and during it, but rather our research 
analyzed whether they constitute potential resources 
in a context that limits physical mobilities. 
 
     Given the importance of communicative mobilities 
for life satisfaction among older persons, it would be 
beneficial for future policies to invest in literacy and 
access to information and communication 
technologies. These would equip older individuals 
with the necessary reserves (Cullati et al., 2018) to 
activate communication channels when confronted 
with physical immobility brought about by old age. 
 
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT  
 
Authors would like to thank Cornelia Hummel, Michel 
Oris, Franz Schultheis and Philippe Wanner for their 
assistance in the elaboration of the present study and 
questionnaire. 
 
REFERENCES 
 
Amit, K. (2010). Determinants of life satisfaction 

among immigrants from Western countries and 
from the FSU in Israel. Social Indicators 
Research, 96(3), 515–534. 
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9490-1 

Anastasiou, E., & Duquenne, M. N. (2021). First-wave 
COVID-19 pandemic in Greece: the role of 
demographic, social, and geographical factors 
in life satisfaction during lockdown. Social 
Sciences, 10(6). 
https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci10060186 

Antonucci, T. C., Ajrouch, K. J., & Birditt, K. S. (2013). 
The convoy model: Explaining social relations 
from a multidisciplinary perspective. The 
Gerontologist, 54(1), 82–92. 
https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnt118 

Aquino, J. A., Russell, D. W., Cutrona, C. E., & Altmaier, 
E. M. (1996). Employment status, social 
support, and life satisfaction among the elderly. 
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 43(4), 480–
489. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
0167.43.4.480 

Baeriswyl, M., & Oris, M. (2021). Social participation 
and life satisfaction among older adults : 
diversity of practices and social inequality in 
Switzerland. Ageing & Society, 1–25. 
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X21001057 

Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, 
L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., & Rubin, G. J. 
(2020). The psychological impact of quarantine 
and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the 
evidence. The Lancet, 395(10227), 912–920. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-
6736(20)30460-8 

Chai, X., & Kalyal, H. (2019). Cell phone use and 
happiness among Chinese older adults: Does 
rural/urban residence status matter? Research 
on Aging, 41(1), 85–109. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/0164027518792662 

Ciobanu, R. O., & Hunter, A. (2017). Older migrants 
and (im)mobilities of Ageing: An introduction. 
Population, Space and Place, 23(5), e2075. 
https://doi.org/10.1002/psp.2075 

Clair, R., Gordon, M., Kroon, M., & Reilly, C. (2021). 
The effects of social isolation on well-being and 
life satisfaction during pandemic. Humanities 
and Social Sciences Communications, 8(1), 1–6. 
https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00710-3 

Cullati, S., Kliegel, M., & Widmer, E. (2018). 
Development of reserves over the life course 
and onset of vulnerability in later life. Nature 
Human Behaviour, 2(8), 551–558. 
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0395-3 

De Vos, J., Schwanen, T., Van Acker, V., & Witlox, F. 
(2013). Travel and subjective well-being: A 
focus on findings, methods and future research 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

44 

needs. Transport Reviews, 33(4), 421–442. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/01441647.2013.8156
65 

Deaton, A. (2008). Income, health, and well-being 
around the world: Evidence from the Gallup 
World Poll. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 
22(2), 53–72. 
https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.22.2.53 

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. 
(1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal 
of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75. 
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_1
3 

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. 
(1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of 
progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 276–
302. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
2909.125.2.276 

Fang, Y., Chau, A. K. C., Wong, A., Fung, H. H., & Woo, 
J. (2018). Information and communicative 
technology use enhances psychological well-
being of older adults: The roles of age, social 
connectedness, and frailty status. Aging & 
Mental Health, 22(11), 1516–1524. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2017.1358
354 

Finlay, J. M., Kler, J. S., O’Shea, B. Q., Eastman, M. R., 
Vinson, Y. R., & Kobayashi, L. C. (2021). Coping 
during the COVID-19 pandemic: A qualitative 
study of older adults across the United States. 
Frontiers in Public Health, 9(April), 1–12. 
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2021.643807 

FSO. (2018). Formation achevée la plus élevée en 
Suisse [Highest educational attainment in 
Switzerland]. Federal Statistical Office. 
https://www.bfs.admin.ch/bfs/fr/home/statist
iques/education-science/niveau-
formation/niveau-formation-
regional.assetdetail.11627134.html 

Gamba, F., Nardone, M., Ricciardi, T., & Cattacin, S. 
(2020). COVID-19: Le regard des sciences 
sociales. Seismo. 

Gehl, J. (2011). Life between buildings: Using public 
space. Island Press. 

Hajek, A., & König, H.-H. (2021). Frequency of contact 
with friends and relatives via internet and 
psychosocial factors in middle‐aged and older 
adults during the COVID‐19 pandemic. Findings 
from the German Ageing Survey. International 
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 37(1). 
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5623 

Hannam, K., Sheller, M., & Urry, J. (2006). Editorial: 
Mobilities, immobilities and moorings. 
Mobilities, 1(1), 1–22. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/17450100500489189 

Heo, J., Chun, S., Lee, S., Lee, K. H., & Kim, J. (2015). 
Internet use and well-being in older adults. 
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social 
Networking, 18(5), 268–272. 
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2014.0549 

Kaplan Mintz, K., Ayalon, O., Nathan, O., & Eshet, T. 
(2021). See or be? Contact with nature and 
well-being during COVID-19 lockdown. Journal 
of Environmental Psychology, 78(June), 101714. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101714 

Kaplan, R. (2001). The nature of the view from home. 
Environment and Behavior, 33(4), 507–542. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/00139160121973115 

Kööts-Ausmees, L., & Realo, A. (2015). The association 
between life satisfaction and self-reported 
Health Status in Europe. European Journal of 
Personality, 29(6), 647–657. 
https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2037 

Liang, J., Krause, N. M., & Bennett, J. M. (2001). Social 
exchange and well-being: Is giving better than 
receiving? Psychology and Aging, 16(3), 511–
523. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-
7974.16.3.511 

Maddux, J. E. (2018). Subjective well-being and life 
satisfaction: An introduction to conceptions, 
theories, and measures. In J. E. Maddux (Ed.), 
Subjective well-being and life satisfaction (pp. 
3–31). Routledge. 

Massey, B., Edwards, A. V., & Musikanski, L. (2021). 
Life satisfaction, affect, and belonging in older 
adults. Applied Research in Quality of Life, 16(3), 
1205–1219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-
019-09804-2 

Musselwhite, C. (2018). The importance of a room 
with a view for older people with limited 
mobility. Quality in Ageing and Older Adults, 
19(4), 273–285. 
https://doi.org/10.1108/QAOA-01-2018-0003 

Musselwhite, C., & Haddad, H. (2010). Mobility, 
accessibility and quality of later life. Mobility, 
Accessibility and Quality of Later Life, 11, 25–37. 

Musselwhite, C., Holland, C., & Walker, I. (2015). The 
role of transport and mobility in the health of 
older people. Journal of Transport & Health, 
2(1), 1–4. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JTH.2015.02.001 

Nordbakke, S., & Schwanen, T. (2014). Well-being and 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

45 

mobility: A theoretical framework and 
literature review focusing on older people. 
Mobilities, 9(1), 104–129. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/17450101.2013.7845
42 

Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (2018). Subjective 
well-being: The science of happiness and life 
satisfaction. In C. R. Snyder, S. J. Lopez, L. M. 
Edwards, & S. C. Marques (Eds.), The Oxford 
handbook of positive psychology, 3rd Edition 
(3rd ed.). Oxford University Press. 
https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/978019939
6511.013.14 

Pedraza, P. De, Guzi, M., & Tijdens, K. (2020). Life 
dissatisfaction and anxiety in COVID-19 
pandemic. In EUR 30243 EN. 
https://doi.org/10.2760/755327 

Peters, T., & Halleran, A. (2021). How our homes 
impact our health: using a COVID-19 informed 
approach to examine urban apartment housing. 
Archnet-IJAR: International Journal of 
Architectural Research, 15(1), 10–27. 
https://doi.org/10.1108/ARCH-08-2020-0159 

Pinquart, M., & Sorensen, S. (2000). Influences of 
socioeconomic status , social network , and 
competence on subjective well-being in later 
life : A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 
July. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-
7974.15.2.187 

Proctor, C. (2014). Subjective well-being. In 
Encyclopedia of quality of life and well-being 
research (pp. 6437–6441). Springer 
Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-
007-0753-5_2905 

Rao, M., Wang, X., Sun, H., & Gai, K. (2018). Subjective 
well-being in nostalgia : Effect and mechanism. 
Psychology, 1720–1730. 
https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2018.97102 

Revord, J., Walsh, L. C., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2018). 
Positive activity interventions to engage well-
being: looking through a social psychological 
lens. In J. E. Maddux (Ed.), Subjective well-being 
and life satisfaction (pp. 451–465). Routledge. 
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.4324/97813
51231879 

Rowles, G. D. (1981). The Surveillance zone as 
meaningful space for the aged. Gerontologist, 
21(3), 304–311. 
https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/21.3.304 

Schwanen, T., & Ziegler, F. (2011). Wellbeing, 
independence and mobility: An introduction. 

Ageing and Society, 31(5), 719–733. 
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X10001467 

Sheller, M., & Urry, J. (2006). The new mobilities 
paradigm. Environment and Planning A: 
Economy and Space, 38(2), 207–226. 
https://doi.org/10.1068/a37268 

Siedlecki, K. L., Salthouse, T. A., Oishi, S., & Jeswani, S. 
(2014). The relationship between social support 
and subjective well-being across age. Social 
Indicators Research, 117(2), 561–576. 
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-013-0361-4 

Sims, T., Reed, A. E., & Carr, D. C. (2016). Information 
and communication technology use is related to 
higher well-being among the oldest-old. The 
Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological 
Sciences and Social Sciences, 72(5), gbw130. 
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw130 

Stevic, A., Schmuck, D., Matthes, J., & Karsay, K. 
(2019). ‘Age matters’: A panel study 
investigating the influence of communicative 
and passive smartphone use on well-being. 
Behaviour & Information Technology, 1–15. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2019.1680
732 

Urry, J. (2007). Mobilities. Polity Press. 
Warr, P., Butcher, V., & Robertson, I. (2004). Activity 

and psychological well-being in older people. 
Aging & Mental Health, 8(2), 172–183. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/13607860410001649
662 

Whitehead, B. R., & Torossian, E. (2021). Older adults’ 
experience of the COVID-19 pandemic: A 
mixed-methods analysis of stresses and joys. 
The Gerontologist, 61(1), 36–47. 
https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnaa126 

Winstead, V., Anderson, W. A., Yost, E. A., Cotten, S. 
R., Warr, A., & Berkowsky, R. W. (2013). You can 
teach an old dog new tricks: A qualitative 
analysis of how residents of senior living 
communities may use the web to overcome 
spatial and social Barriers. Journal of Applied 
Gerontology, 32(5), 540–560. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/0733464811431824 

Ye, S., Ng, T. K., & Lam, C. L. (2018). Nostalgia and 
temporal life satisfaction. Journal of Happiness 
Studies, 19(6), 1749–1762. 
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-017-9884-8 

Zhang, J. (2016). Aging in cyberspace: Internet use 
and quality of life of older Chinese migrants. 
The Journal of Chinese Sociology, 3(1), 26. 
https://doi.org/10.1186/s40711-016-0045-y 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

46 

Ziegler, F., & Schwanen, T. (2011). ‘I like to go out to 
be energised by different people’: an 
exploratory analysis of mobility and wellbeing 
in later life. Ageing & Society, 31(5), 758–781. 
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X10000498 

 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

47 

 
Table 1. Sociodemographic Variables and Distribution Among Working Sample 
 
 

Sociodemographic variable n Distribution (%) 

Age  Min: 65 

Max: 99 

Mean: 73.3 

Std. Deviation: 5.75 

Gender   

Female 433 67.3 

Male 210 32.7 

Education   

Without post-compulsory training 29 4.5 

Secondary 192 29.9 

Tertiary 422 65.6 

Relationship status   

Single 230 35.8 

In a relationship 413 64.2 

Children   

No children 113 17.6 

Has at least 1 child 530 82.4 

Region / Canton of Residence*   

Geneva 296 46.0 

Vaud 165 25.7 

Other 182 28.3 

 

 
 
 
* In Switzerland, canton does not only have a geographical meaning, but also has a political significance. 
 
 
 
 
 
 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

48 

Table 2. Mobility Variables and Distribution Among Working Sample 
 

Mobility variable n Distribution (%) 

Frequency of talking on the phone during 
partial confinement 

  

Every day 346 53.8 

Several times a week 274 42.6 

Once a week or less 23 3.6 

Freq. of digital means use for communication 
during partial confinement 

  

Every day 441 68.6 

Several times a week 167 26.0 

Once a week or less 35 5.4 

Freq. of looking out the window during partial 
confinement 

  

Every day 234 36.4 

Several times a week 208 32.3 

Once a week or less 201 31.3 

Freq. of looking at photos 
during partial confinement 

  

Every day 52 8.1 

Several times a week 186 28.9 

Once a week or less 405 63.0 

Visited museums or expositions on the internet 
during partial confinement 

  

Yes 121 18.8 

No 522 81.2 

Searched travel destination on the internet 
during partial confinement 

  

Yes 82 12.8 

No 561 87.2 

NOTE: Looking at the variable distribution at first glance (Table 2), one may hypothesize that the non-significance 
of the relationship may be due to the small number of individuals who looked at photos every day (n=52) and the 
fact that “Every day” is the reference category in the regression. However, we also tried changing the reference 
category to “Once a week or less” but the relationship between frequency of looking at photos and life satisfaction 
did not change. We thus kept the reference category as “Every day” to be consistent with the other variables in the 
regression. 



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

49 

 
Table 3. Linear Regression Analyses: The Relationship Between Communicative, Virtual, and Imaginative 
Mobilities, and Life Satisfaction (n=643) 
 
 

 Dependent variable: Life Satisfaction Scale 

 Model 1: Mobility variables Model 2: + Sociodemographic 
and personal resources 

 Coef. Std.  
Error  

Sig. Coef. Std. 
Error 

Sig. 

Freq. of talking on phone during 
partial confinement 

      

Every day Ref.   Ref.   

Several times a week -0.416 0.414 0.315 -2.75 0.405 0.498 

Once a week or less -2.972** 1.081 0.006 -2.232* 1.051 0.034 

Freq. of digital means use for comm. 
During partial confinement 

      

Every day  Ref.   Ref.   

Several times a week -0.033 0.463 0.943 -0.163 0.446 0.715 

Once a week or less -2.407** 0.880 0.006 -2.069* 0.867 0.017 

Freq. of looking out the window 
during partial confinement 

      

Every day Ref.   Ref.   

Several times a week -0.942* 0.477 0.049 -0.974* 0.461 0.035 

Once a week or less -0.033 0.481 0.946 0.038 0.471 0.937 

Freq. of looking at photos during 
partial confinement 

      

Every day Ref.   Ref.   

Several times a week 0.341 0.780 0.662 0.374 0.748 0.617 

Once a week or less -0.026 0.738 0.972 0.134 0.712 0.851 

Visited museums or expositions on 
internet during partial confinement 
(Ref: Did not do so) 

-0.614 0.505 0.225 -0.588 0.490 0.231 

Searched travel destination on the 
internet partial confinement (Ref: 
Did not do so) 

0.279 0.589 0.635 0.350 0.572 0.541 

Male (Ref: Female)    -0.730 0.438 0.096 

Age    0.052 0.035 0.140 

Education level       

Tertiary    Ref.   



 

IHTP, 2(1), 35-50, 2022            CC BY-NC-ND 4.0                                ISSN 2563-9269 

 

50 

Secondary    -0.552 0.423 0.193 

Without post-compulsory training    1.271 0.955 0.184 

In a relationship (Ref: Not in a 
relationship) 

   1.423** 0.428 0.001 

No children (Ref: Has children)    0.638 0.511 0.213 

Strong interaction w/ neighbors (Ref: 
Minimal to no interaction) 

   0.739 0.392 0.060 

Health: Average to bad (Ref: Good 
health) 

   -2.869*** 0.449 <0.001 

Prayed during partial confinement       

No    Ref.   

Yes    0.789 0.418 0.059 

Missing    0.729 0.652 0.265