67 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 How to cite this article: Alfi, C. F. (2023). A meta-analysis of the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour. International Journal of Banking and Finance, 18(1), 67-94. https://doi. org/10.32890/ ijbf2023.18.1.4 A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELIGIOSITY AND SAVING BEHAVIOUR Coky Fauzi Alfi State Polytechnic of Sriwijaya, Indonesia coky.fauzi.alfi@polsri.ac.id Received: 2/2/2022 Revised: 2/3/2022 Accepted: 30/3/2022 Published: 5/1/2023 ABSTRACT The purpose of this study was to synthesize the findings of previous studies on the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour by using a meta-analysis approach. It also sought to determine the strength of the relationship, besides its direction. Eleven studies which met the five criteria and four techniques used in the study were used as samples for the meta-analytic analysis. The size of the effect in each study was then determined by Pearson’s product-moment correlations (r). To estimate the average distribution of relationship true effects, the Fisher r-to-z transformation and random-effects methods were used. The empirical evidence showed that there was a positive correlation between religiosity and saving behaviour. However, according to Guilford’s convention, the true effect size (r = 0.303) would mean that religiosity had a weak correlation with saving behaviour. It is recommended that authorities and financial institutions use the findings of this study to develop plans focused on advocating and facilitating saving behaviour among religious people. https://e-journal.uum.edu.my/index.php/ijbf INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BANKING AND FINANCE 68 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Keywords: Meta-analysis, religiosity, saving behaviour, Fisher r-to-z transformation, random-effects method. JEL Classification: Z120, G410. INTRODUCTION It appears that the global community is becoming more religious. Based on a Pew Research Center (2015) survey, all major religions are estimated to show a rise in the number of followers by 2050. The survey found that 84 percent of the world population was religiously affiliated in 2010, with projections predicting that this share would rise to 87 percent by 2050. These findings and projections appear to contradict the views of several influential scholars, such as Karl Marx, Emile Durkheim, and Max Weber, who predicted that religion would be less important in various socioeconomic activities as industrialization progressed, economic markets expanded, and science, technology, and education advanced rapidly (Basedau et al., 2018). Furthermore, the findings of various studies in economics (e.g. Azzi & Ehrenberg, 1975; Iannaccone, 1998; Iyer, 2016), sociology (e.g. Geertz, 1973; Inglehart, 2018; Lenski, 1961), and psychology (e.g. Allport & Ross, 1967; Berry et al., 2002; Pargament, 1999) have acknowledged the importance of religion in human society. For example, it plays an important role in, energising people to work for social change, promoting mental well-being, or acting as a social control agent. The investigation into the relationship between religion and economic growth has received considerable attention, ever since Max Weber (1905) recognized the significance of religious affiliations in economic performance. He argued that the values in Protestant teachings would shape their adherents’ work ethic, resulting in professionalism and efficiency in economic activities. More than a century after Weber’s thesis, a large body of literature has noticed a link between religion and macroeconomic prosperity. For instance, it has been discovered that religious beliefs, particularly beliefs in hell and heaven, have a positive effect on economic attitudes, leading to higher incomes and Gross Domestic Product (GDP), and Christianity is the religion with 69 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 the greatest impact on economic growth, with Protestants being more capitalists than other Christians (Barro & McCleary, 2003; Filipova, 2012; Guiso et al., 2003; Hayward & Kemmelmeier, 2011). Moreover, religion has long been associated with the teachings of thriftiness and customary living. The research findings however, show that there are differences in which religions adhere to the most frugal and conventional ways of life. According to Keister (2003), Guiso et al. (2003), and Renneboog and Spaenjers (2012), Catholics appeared to value frugality and convenient living more than Protestants, whereas Arruñada (2010) and Filipova (2012) discovered the opposite. Although many studies found a correlation between religious belief and thriftiness, their findings are less convincing when used to explain a link between religious belief and saving decisions. This is due to the distinction between thriftiness and saving decisions. Thriftiness is the trait to try and reduce spending, whereas saving decisions are initiated by residual income. Therefore, research into how religion influences economic behaviour and financial decisions at the microeconomic level seems to remain limited (see Klaubert, 2010; Yayeh, 2014). In terms of empirical assessments that link individual saving attitudes to religious preferences or practices, it needs to be explored further. This investigation should be beneficial because it could help us to solve pressing issues in the national economy, for instance, pressing concerns such as wealth inequality (Bilen, 2016; Keister, 2003) and consumerism (Tjahjono, 2014), or even the issues of conserving energy and natural resources (Singh et al., 2021). Since the investigation of religiosity on saving behaviour is an emerging research area, the present study is interested in knowing the ‘true’ effect size of the relationship between these variables. Therefore, this study has performed a meta-analysis to gain a more objective, robust, and less biased understanding of the relationship between the variables by investigating the distribution of effect sizes. A meta-analysis is an approach for aggregating effect-size indices from multiple studies (Borenstein et al., 2011). It contributes to answering the question of whether the observed variations in effect sizes across studies are due to a single population effect size (Law, 1995). To date, there has been no other study examining religiosity and saving behaviour across samples, methodologies, and time. This study has utilized meta-analysis as a quantitative tool to synthesize the findings 70 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 of previous studies, and to determine the strength of the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour, as well as the direction of that relationship. As a result, the strength of the correlation between previous findings and their direction, whether positive or negative, has been held to the same standards, as long as they meet the inclusion criteria set for the study sample. One of the inclusion criteria, for example, was that the study sample would include religiosity, religious belief, or religious faith as an independent variable. In addition, the other objective is to contribute to the growth of the literature in this area of study. This study can provide a retrospective summary of the existing literature and provide further empirical evidence of the true effect of religiosity on saving behaviour. It could help shape new research by describing what was already known and synthesizing the new body of evidence. After reviewing previous studies and establishing the inclusion criteria, eleven journal articles were identified as the study sample; all together these sources provided a total of 1,063 participants coming from various locations. More specifically, Yayeh (2014) collected samples in Ethiopia, while Ababio and Mawutor (2015) did so in Ghana. Satsios and Hadjidakis (2017) gathered data in Greece. In Indonesia, questionnaires were administered by Murdayanti et al. (2020); Prastiwi (2021); Priyo Nugroho et al. (2017); Wijaya et al. (2019). Meanwhile, data was collected in Malaysia by Abdullah and Abd. Majid (2001); Ismail et al. (2018); Kassim et al. (2019); Mei Teh et al. (2019). As a result, this meta-analytic study was able to generate numerous plot functions, such as the forest plot, standardized residual plot, and Cook’s Distance plot, as well as measurements, such as the random-effect model, heterogeneity statistics, outliers, and influential case diagnostics. LITERATURE REVIEW The relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour is typically measured using one of two methods: methods which are either economically or psychologically oriented. In the economic approach, the goal is to create forecasts about behaviour as accurately as possible. However, this approach often neglects to explore the true underlying causes of why individuals behave the way they do (Nyhus, 71 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 2017). Researchers who employ this approach rely on secondary data surveys, such as the World Values Survey (WVS), the International Social Survey Program (ISSP), the Gallup Millennium Survey, the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID), or the Konda Araştrma ve Danşmanlk, to determine an individual’s religious behaviour. From these data sources, researchers discovered that the ‘average’ person’s religiosity could be measured in the following five ways, namely participation in religious services, belief in heaven and hell, belief in the afterlife, faith in God, and self-identification as a religious person (Barro & McCleary, 2003). These religious aspects are then examined in relation to the adherents’ amount of income or consumption using various econometric methodologies so as to understand the significance of religiosity in saving behaviour. Klaubert (2010), for example, used the PSID to investigate the link between individual saving decisions and religiosity, as measured by church attendance, in the United States. Using the Konda data survey, Davutyan and Öztürkkal (2016) investigated the effect of religious affiliation on financial behaviour in Turkey. They discovered however, weak evidence that religious people have distinct preferences for saving decisions. This was due to there being no difference between religious and non-religious people when it comes to saving decisions. Meanwhile, Guiso et al. (2003) discovered a link between religious intensity and thriftiness in a cross- national study using the WVS sample statistics. On the other hand, the psychological viewpoint begins from a different place. This approach frequently concentrates on psychological factors, and examines individual differences rather than average human behaviour. Therefore, various explanatory variables and methods have been employed in the analysis of the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour, which makes it different from the economic approach. The psychological viewpoint often employs primary data sources and applies behavioural theories, for example the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991) or social learning theory (Bandura, 1977). The theory of planned behavior identifies specific factors, namely intentions and perceived behavioral control, that can be utilised to estimate and describe human behavior in various contexts. Intentions are motivational variables that demonstrate how far individuals are willing to go and how much effort they intend to put in, whereas perceived behavioral control refers to the perception of how easy or difficult it is to control an interest (Ajzen, 1991). Meanwhile, 72 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 attitudes toward behavior, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control can all have an impact on intentions. Furthermore, social learning theory is often used as a base theory to describe the role of financial literacy in saving behavior. The theory hypothesizes that the cognitive abilities of individuals, i.e., knowledge and skills, impact on changing their behaviors. This cognitive ability can be learned by seeing, imitating, practicing, and processing information from the behaviour of others and its environments, including families, friends, neighbours, the workplace, or the media. The psychological viewpoint also employs religiosity measurement scales, such as the orthodoxy measurement (Glock, 1962) or the religious orientations (Allport & Ross, 1967). The orthodoxy measurement uses the following five scales: belief, practice, knowledge, experience, and consequences, and these would inform the preferred faith. Belief is an ideological dimension that a religious person will adhere to. Prayer, fasting, involvement in special sacraments, worship, and other ritualistic activities are included in the practice. Knowledge refers to the understanding of the fundamental tenets of a religious person’s faith and its sacred scriptures. Experience gives a religious emotional experience, and consequences are all of the religious prescriptions for what a religious person should do. Meanwhile, the measurement of religious orientation uses the following two dimensions: intrinsic and extrinsic, and they would inform us of the primary motive for life in religion. Those who are intrinsically motivated find that their primary motive in religion is to live according to their religious convictions and prescriptions. However, extrinsically oriented people may find religion useful in a variety of ways, including stability and reassurance, social connection and diversionary tactics, status, and self-justification. Few researchers have adopted this approach in their studies. For example, Priyo Nugroho et al. (2017) expanded the theory of planned behaviour by including two new variables: religiosity and self-efficacy. They then employed Allport and Ross’s (1967) scale for measuring religiosity to investigate the saving behaviour of Islamic bank customers. In the meantime, Kassim et al. (2019) who used the social learning theoretical framework and the religiosity measurement scale which had its root in Glock’s (1962) work, discovered that whereas religiosity had no effect on saving behaviour, financial literacy did. 73 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 METHODOLOGY Criteria and Search Procedure The samples for the present meta-analytic study were selected because they had discussed the influence of religiosity on saving behaviour directly. To be included in the meta-analytic sample, the studies must fulfil five criteria. They are as follows: • The studies used religiosity, religious belief, or religious faith as an independent variable. • The studies used saving behaviour or saving habits, saving money, or saving decisions as a dependent variable. • The studies used a quantitative research approach. • The studies used primary data at a micro analytical level. • The studies presented the Pearson’s r effect size clearly or could be processed using another statistical method. Studies would be excluded if they had found a relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour indirectly. Finding studies from various journals, such as journals on economics, business, management, finance, marketing, religion, culture, and social science, that fit the inclusion and exclusion criteria for a meta- analysis study was challenging. For example, to avoid the possibility that this might turn out to be a time-consuming process, an effective search strategy was used from start to finish. The present study has implemented four techniques to conduct a wide-ranging literature search. They were as follows: (1) deciding search terms and keywords, (2) searching for specific phrases, (3) using truncated and wildcard searches as well as Boolean logic, and (4) using citation searching. Firstly, these terms and keywords were applied in the search process: religiosity, religious belief, religious faith, saving behaviour, saving habits, saving money, and saving decisions. Secondly, quotation marks were used for words which appear next to each other, e.g., “religious belief,” “religious faith,” “saving behaviour,” “saving habits,” “saving money,” “saving decisions.” Thirdly, the search used combined truncation and wildcard searches with Boolean logic, e.g., “religio*” AND “saving behavio?r”. Fourthly, articles that were cited in other publications were also included in the search. These techniques were then employed to search for studies in the various research search engines and databases, such as Semantic Scholar, 74 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Google Scholar, Research Gate, EBSCOhost, ProQuest, JSTOR, and Wiley Online Library. All potentially relevant titles and abstracts were then saved and managed systematically for the next stage, which was screening. The first stage of the screening was to import all the references into a reference management software package and de-duplicate them. In this case, a software called EndNote was used. The next stage was to read and identify all the saved articles of study, and filter them out if they were irrelevant. The stages of screening resulted in 11 journal articles identified as the relevant data selected for the meta-analysis. Data Extraction Once screening has been done and all relevant articles selected for the study have been identified, the next step is the data extraction (see Teshome et al., 2018; Zuckerman et al., 2013). In this process, the key aspects that will be used for the statistical meta-analysis will have to be extracted from the articles. Some key aspects of the articles are set, namely the authors’ name and year of publication, sampling methods, measurement techniques, variables identification (independent and dependent), methods of statistical analysis, and a summary of the results (see Table 1). The characteristics of each article that met the criteria for inclusion were also highlighted. For example, the eleven studies used various themes related to religiosity and saving behaviour as independent and dependent variables, respectively. These are described in the column on variables. In another column, such as the measurement technique, it is stated that all studies applied a questionnaire survey to ensure that primary data was used. The most useful information, however, is in the results column. It discusses the significance of the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour, as well as various goodness of fit tests, e.g., chi-square, odds ratio, or t-statistic, that can be used to compute the effect size r. Effect Size Computation Following data extraction, the next task was to determine the size of the effect in each study and ensure that this effect size was expressed in the same way. The effect sizes are used to describe the strength of the relationship between the variables. There are two common types of effect size: the r type and the d type. The two most commonly 75 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 used of the r type are Pearson’s product-moment correlations (r) and Fisher’s r-to-z transformation (Zr), whereas the three most commonly used d type are Cohen’s d, Hedges’s g, and Glass’s D (Rosenthal, 1995). In this study, Pearson’s r was the preferred effect size. In this regard, it involved calculating the r value for each study carrying out the meta-analysis. There was no need to do anything if a study had used the r value. However, because some studies had no effect size value and only provided various fit test indicators (e.g., chi-square, odds ratio, t-test statistic), a conversion to Pearson’s r was performed via an online calculator at www.psychometrica.de/effect_size.html (Lenhard & Lenhard, 2016). Meanwhile, if the authors did not provide the indicators and could not compute a conversion to the r value, they were contacted via email to gain the relevant information. A reminder was sent if they did not reply. Method of Analysis The analysis is carried out using the Fisher r-to-z transformed correlation coefficient as the outcome measure. The Fisher’s r-to-z transformation is commonly used because samples from a meta- analysis contain a variety of effect sizes. It is also to achieve normality in the effect sizes (Cheung et al., 2012). There are three steps in implementing this method (Borenstein et al., 2011; Field & Gillett, 2010). To begin, use Fisher’s r-to-z transformation to convert the effect size in each study into a standard normal metric. The Fisher’s r-to-z transformation formula is given as where is the effect size in each study. After that, for each study, a weighted average of scores are computed by the formula where is the number of studies and is the sample size. Finally, it should be converted back to using the formula In addition, the random-effects statistical model was applied to estimate the average distribution of true effects. The random-effects method was chosen because the effect size was extracted from a series of studies conducted by various authors in various populations at various times. The present analysis also reported on the estimate of the index, the H2 index, the I2 index, and the Q-test (Cochran, 1954) with a p-value as the heterogeneity statistics outcome. The Q-test was used to assess the null hypothesis that all effect sizes A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELIGIOSITY AND SAVING BEHAVIOUR Method of Analysis 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒 � 1+𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 �, 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�̅�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 , 𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖+1 . τ2 A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELIGIOSITY AND SAVING BEHAVIOUR Method of Analysis 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒 � 1+𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 �, 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�̅�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 , 𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖+1 . τ2 A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELIGIOSITY AND SAVING BEHAVIOUR Method of Analysis 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒 � 1+𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 �, 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�̅�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 , 𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖+1 . τ2 A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELIGIOSITY AND SAVING BEHAVIOUR Method of Analysis 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒 � 1+𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 �, 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�̅�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 , 𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖+1 . τ2 A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELIGIOSITY AND SAVING BEHAVIOUR Method of Analysis 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒 � 1+𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 �, 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�̅�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 , 𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖+1 . τ2 A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELIGIOSITY AND SAVING BEHAVIOUR Method of Analysis 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒 � 1+𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 �, 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�̅�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 , 𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖+1 . τ2 three steps to implementing this method (Borenstein et al., 2011; Field & Gillett, 2010). To begin, use Fisher's r-to-z transformation to convert the effect size in each study into a standard normal metric. The Fisher's r-to-z transformation formula is given by 𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒 ( 1+𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 ), where 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 is the effect size in each study. After that, for each study, a weighted average of 𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟 scores are computed by �̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑘𝑘𝑖𝑖=1 , where 𝑘𝑘 is the number of studies and 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖 is the sample size. Finally, it should be converted back to 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 using the formula 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒2�̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒2�̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖+1 . In addition, the random-effects statistical model is applied to estimate the average distribution of true effects. The random-effects method was chosen because the effect size was extracted from a series of studies conducted by various authors in various populations at various times. The analysis also reports the estimate of the 2 index, the H2 index, the I2 index, and the Q-test (Cochran, 1954) with a p-value as the heterogeneity statistics outcome. The Q-test is used to assess the null hypothesis that all effect sizes from all studies are homogenous (Chen & Peace, 2021). If the p-value is less than  (the typical significance level is 0.05), the null hypothesis should be rejected, indicating that the effect sizes from all studies are not homogenous. Meanwhile, 2, H2, and I2 are used to determine the strength of the distribution of true effect sizes. The 2 index is estimated using the Hedges’ estimator (Hedges & Olkin, 1985) to measure the variance of the true effect sizes, and the index should be greater than zero. The H2 index is quantified using Higgins and Thompson’s (2002) formula to inform the relative extent of heterogeneity in comparison to all studies, and the index should be greater than 1. The I2 index is also calculated using Higgins and Thompson's (2002) formula to determine the percentage of observed heterogeneity versus real heterogeneity. As a rule of thumb, the I2 index could be considered as having low heterogeneity (I2 = 25%), moderate heterogeneity (I2 = 50%), and high heterogeneity (I2 = 75%). To display the conclusions of meta-analyses, forest plots are generated. Forest plots provide information about each study’s effect size and confidence interval, as well as the average distribution of true effects. The analysis also examines whether studies may be outliers and/or influential in the random-effect model. They can have a significant impact on the value of the estimated random-effect model coefficients, i.e., the intercept. If they had remained in the analysis, they could have changed the entire outcome. The standardized residuals are used to detect outliers, while the Cook's distances (Cook, 1977) and DFFITS (Difference in Fits) are applied to diagnose the influential studies. Studies are considered as potential outliers if they have a standardized residual larger than 3 or smaller than -3 (rstudent > ± 3), while they are considered to be influential if the Cook's distance value is more than 1 (cook.D > 1) and DFFITS is larger than 2 (dffits > 2) (Gerbing, 2014). The meta-analysis was carried out with the help of open-source statistical software Jamovi version 1.6.23 (The Jamovi Project, 2021). The MAJOR meta-analysis module library was used to compute r-to-z transformations, as well as to generate a random-effect model, heterogeneity statistics, a forest plot, and outlier and influential case diagnostics. RESULTS Table 1 shows the results of data extraction processing. The eleven studies were published between 2001 and 2021, with Abdullah and Abd. Majid (2001) is the longest and Prastiwi (2021) is the most recent. The studies used two types of sampling methods: probability sampling and non-probability sampling. Ababio and Mawutor (2015); Kassim et al. (2019); Murdayanti et al. (2020); and Yayeh (2014) applied the probability sampling method, whereas Ismail et al. (2018); Mei Teh et al. (2019); Priyo Nugroho et al. three steps to implementing this method (Borenstein et al., 2011; Field & Gillett, 2010). To begin, use Fisher's r-to-z transformation to convert the effect size in each study into a standard normal metric. The Fisher's r-to-z transformation formula is given by 𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒 ( 1+𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 ), where 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 is the effect size in each study. After that, for each study, a weighted average of 𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟 scores are computed by �̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑘𝑘𝑖𝑖=1 , where 𝑘𝑘 is the number of studies and 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖 is the sample size. Finally, it should be converted back to 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 using the formula 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒2�̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒2�̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖+1 . In addition, the random-effects statistical model is applied to estimate the average distribution of true effects. The random-effects method was chosen because the effect size was extracted from a series of studies conducted by various authors in various populations at various times. The analysis also reports the estimate of the 2 index, the H2 index, the I2 index, and the Q-test (Cochran, 1954) with a p-value as the heterogeneity statistics outcome. The Q-test is used to assess the null hypothesis that all effect sizes from all studies are homogenous (Chen & Peace, 2021). If the p-value is less than  (the typical significance level is 0.05), the null hypothesis should be rejected, indicating that the effect sizes from all studies are not homogenous. Meanwhile, 2, H2, and I2 are used to determine the strength of the distribution of true effect sizes. The 2 index is estimated using the Hedges’ estimator (Hedges & Olkin, 1985) to measure the variance of the true effect sizes, and the index should be greater than zero. The H2 index is quantified using Higgins and Thompson’s (2002) formula to inform the relative extent of heterogeneity in comparison to all studies, and the index should be greater than 1. The I2 index is also calculated using Higgins and Thompson's (2002) formula to determine the percentage of observed heterogeneity versus real heterogeneity. As a rule of thumb, the I2 index could be considered as having low heterogeneity (I2 = 25%), moderate heterogeneity (I2 = 50%), and high heterogeneity (I2 = 75%). To display the conclusions of meta-analyses, forest plots are generated. Forest plots provide information about each study’s effect size and confidence interval, as well as the average distribution of true effects. The analysis also examines whether studies may be outliers and/or influential in the random-effect model. They can have a significant impact on the value of the estimated random-effect model coefficients, i.e., the intercept. If they had remained in the analysis, they could have changed the entire outcome. The standardized residuals are used to detect outliers, while the Cook's distances (Cook, 1977) and DFFITS (Difference in Fits) are applied to diagnose the influential studies. Studies are considered as potential outliers if they have a standardized residual larger than 3 or smaller than -3 (rstudent > ± 3), while they are considered to be influential if the Cook's distance value is more than 1 (cook.D > 1) and DFFITS is larger than 2 (dffits > 2) (Gerbing, 2014). The meta-analysis was carried out with the help of open-source statistical software Jamovi version 1.6.23 (The Jamovi Project, 2021). The MAJOR meta-analysis module library was used to compute r-to-z transformations, as well as to generate a random-effect model, heterogeneity statistics, a forest plot, and outlier and influential case diagnostics. RESULTS Table 1 shows the results of data extraction processing. The eleven studies were published between 2001 and 2021, with Abdullah and Abd. Majid (2001) is the longest and Prastiwi (2021) is the most recent. The studies used two types of sampling methods: probability sampling and non-probability sampling. Ababio and Mawutor (2015); Kassim et al. (2019); Murdayanti et al. (2020); and Yayeh (2014) applied the probability sampling method, whereas Ismail et al. (2018); Mei Teh et al. (2019); Priyo Nugroho et al. three steps to implementing this method (Borenstein et al., 2011; Field & Gillett, 2010). To begin, use Fisher's r-to-z transformation to convert the effect size in each study into a standard normal metric. The Fisher's r-to-z transformation formula is given by 𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒 ( 1+𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 ), where 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 is the effect size in each study. After that, for each study, a weighted average of 𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟 scores are computed by �̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑘𝑘𝑖𝑖=1 , where 𝑘𝑘 is the number of studies and 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖 is the sample size. Finally, it should be converted back to 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 using the formula 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒2�̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒2�̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖+1 . In addition, the random-effects statistical model is applied to estimate the average distribution of true effects. The random-effects method was chosen because the effect size was extracted from a series of studies conducted by various authors in various populations at various times. The analysis also reports the estimate of the 2 index, the H2 index, the I2 index, and the Q-test (Cochran, 1954) with a p-value as the heterogeneity statistics outcome. The Q-test is used to assess the null hypothesis that all effect sizes from all studies are homogenous (Chen & Peace, 2021). If the p-value is less than  (the typical significance level is 0.05), the null hypothesis should be rejected, indicating that the effect sizes from all studies are not homogenous. Meanwhile, 2, H2, and I2 are used to determine the strength of the distribution of true effect sizes. The 2 index is estimated using the Hedges’ estimator (Hedges & Olkin, 1985) to measure the variance of the true effect sizes, and the index should be greater than zero. The H2 index is quantified using Higgins and Thompson’s (2002) formula to inform the relative extent of heterogeneity in comparison to all studies, and the index should be greater than 1. The I2 index is also calculated using Higgins and Thompson's (2002) formula to determine the percentage of observed heterogeneity versus real heterogeneity. As a rule of thumb, the I2 index could be considered as having low heterogeneity (I2 = 25%), moderate heterogeneity (I2 = 50%), and high heterogeneity (I2 = 75%). To display the conclusions of meta-analyses, forest plots are generated. Forest plots provide information about each study’s effect size and confidence interval, as well as the average distribution of true effects. The analysis also examines whether studies may be outliers and/or influential in the random-effect model. They can have a significant impact on the value of the estimated random-effect model coefficients, i.e., the intercept. If they had remained in the analysis, they could have changed the entire outcome. The standardized residuals are used to detect outliers, while the Cook's distances (Cook, 1977) and DFFITS (Difference in Fits) are applied to diagnose the influential studies. Studies are considered as potential outliers if they have a standardized residual larger than 3 or smaller than -3 (rstudent > ± 3), while they are considered to be influential if the Cook's distance value is more than 1 (cook.D > 1) and DFFITS is larger than 2 (dffits > 2) (Gerbing, 2014). The meta-analysis was carried out with the help of open-source statistical software Jamovi version 1.6.23 (The Jamovi Project, 2021). The MAJOR meta-analysis module library was used to compute r-to-z transformations, as well as to generate a random-effect model, heterogeneity statistics, a forest plot, and outlier and influential case diagnostics. RESULTS Table 1 shows the results of data extraction processing. The eleven studies were published between 2001 and 2021, with Abdullah and Abd. Majid (2001) is the longest and Prastiwi (2021) is the most recent. The studies used two types of sampling methods: probability sampling and non-probability sampling. Ababio and Mawutor (2015); Kassim et al. (2019); Murdayanti et al. (2020); and Yayeh (2014) applied the probability sampling method, whereas Ismail et al. (2018); Mei Teh et al. (2019); Priyo Nugroho et al. 76 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 from all studies were homogenous (Chen & Peace, 2021). If the p-value was less than (the typical significance level is 0.05), the null hypothesis should be rejected, indicating that the effect sizes from all studies were not homogenous. Meanwhile, , H2, and I2 were used to determine the strength of the distribution of true effect sizes. The index was estimated using the Hedges’ estimator (Hedges & Olkin, 1985) to measure the variance of the true effect sizes, and the index should be greater than zero. The H2 index was quantified using the Higgins and Thompson’s (2002) formula to inform the relative extent of heterogeneity in comparison to all studies, and the index should be greater than 1. The I2 index was also calculated using the Higgins and Thompson’s (2002) formula to determine the percentage of observed heterogeneity versus real heterogeneity. As a rule of thumb, the I2 index could be considered as having low heterogeneity (I2 = 25%), moderate heterogeneity (I2 = 50%), and high heterogeneity (I2 = 75%). To display the conclusions of the meta-analyses, forest plots were generated. Forest plots provided information about each study’s effect size and confidence interval, as well as the average distribution of true effects. The analysis also examined whether studies may be outliers and/or influential in the random-effect model. They can have a significant impact on the value of the estimated random-effect model coefficients, i.e., the intercept. If they had remained in the analysis, they could have changed the entire outcome. The standardized residuals are used to detect outliers, while the Cook’s distances (Cook, 1977) and DFFITS (Difference in Fits) are applied to diagnose the influential studies. Studies are considered as potential outliers if they have a standardized residual larger than 3 or smaller than -3 (rstudent > ± 3), while they are considered to be influential if the Cook’s distance value is more than 1 (cook.D > 1) and DFFITS is larger than 2 (dffits > 2) (Gerbing, 2014). The meta-analysis was carried out with the help of an open-source statistical software the Jamovi version 1.6.23 (The Jamovi Project, 2021). The MAJOR meta-analysis module library was used to compute r-to-z transformations, as well as to generate a random-effect model, heterogeneity statistics, a forest plot, and outlier and influential case diagnostics. three steps to implementing this method (Borenstein et al., 2011; Field & Gillett, 2010). To begin, use Fisher's r-to-z transformation to convert the effect size in each study into a standard normal metric. The Fisher's r-to-z transformation formula is given by 𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒 ( 1+𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 ), where 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 is the effect size in each study. After that, for each study, a weighted average of 𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟 scores are computed by �̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑘𝑘𝑖𝑖=1 , where 𝑘𝑘 is the number of studies and 𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖 is the sample size. Finally, it should be converted back to 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 using the formula 𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒2�̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒2�̅�𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖+1 . In addition, the random-effects statistical model is applied to estimate the average distribution of true effects. The random-effects method was chosen because the effect size was extracted from a series of studies conducted by various authors in various populations at various times. The analysis also reports the estimate of the 2 index, the H2 index, the I2 index, and the Q-test (Cochran, 1954) with a p-value as the heterogeneity statistics outcome. The Q-test is used to assess the null hypothesis that all effect sizes from all studies are homogenous (Chen & Peace, 2021). If the p-value is less than  (the typical significance level is 0.05), the null hypothesis should be rejected, indicating that the effect sizes from all studies are not homogenous. Meanwhile, 2, H2, and I2 are used to determine the strength of the distribution of true effect sizes. The 2 index is estimated using the Hedges’ estimator (Hedges & Olkin, 1985) to measure the variance of the true effect sizes, and the index should be greater than zero. The H2 index is quantified using Higgins and Thompson’s (2002) formula to inform the relative extent of heterogeneity in comparison to all studies, and the index should be greater than 1. The I2 index is also calculated using Higgins and Thompson's (2002) formula to determine the percentage of observed heterogeneity versus real heterogeneity. As a rule of thumb, the I2 index could be considered as having low heterogeneity (I2 = 25%), moderate heterogeneity (I2 = 50%), and high heterogeneity (I2 = 75%). To display the conclusions of meta-analyses, forest plots are generated. Forest plots provide information about each study’s effect size and confidence interval, as well as the average distribution of true effects. The analysis also examines whether studies may be outliers and/or influential in the random-effect model. They can have a significant impact on the value of the estimated random-effect model coefficients, i.e., the intercept. If they had remained in the analysis, they could have changed the entire outcome. The standardized residuals are used to detect outliers, while the Cook's distances (Cook, 1977) and DFFITS (Difference in Fits) are applied to diagnose the influential studies. Studies are considered as potential outliers if they have a standardized residual larger than 3 or smaller than -3 (rstudent > ± 3), while they are considered to be influential if the Cook's distance value is more than 1 (cook.D > 1) and DFFITS is larger than 2 (dffits > 2) (Gerbing, 2014). The meta-analysis was carried out with the help of open-source statistical software Jamovi version 1.6.23 (The Jamovi Project, 2021). The MAJOR meta-analysis module library was used to compute r-to-z transformations, as well as to generate a random-effect model, heterogeneity statistics, a forest plot, and outlier and influential case diagnostics. RESULTS Table 1 shows the results of data extraction processing. The eleven studies were published between 2001 and 2021, with Abdullah and Abd. Majid (2001) is the longest and Prastiwi (2021) is the most recent. The studies used two types of sampling methods: probability sampling and non-probability sampling. Ababio and Mawutor (2015); Kassim et al. (2019); Murdayanti et al. (2020); and Yayeh (2014) applied the probability sampling method, whereas Ismail et al. (2018); Mei Teh et al. (2019); Priyo Nugroho et al. A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELIGIOSITY AND SAVING BEHAVIOUR Method of Analysis 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒 � 1+𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 �, 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�̅�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 , 𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖+1 . τ2 A META-ANALYSIS OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELIGIOSITY AND SAVING BEHAVIOUR Method of Analysis 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 0.5𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑙𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒 � 1+𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 1−𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 �, 𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�̅�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 ∑ 𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑛𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 𝑘𝑘𝑘𝑘 𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖=1 , 𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖 = 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖−1 𝑒𝑒𝑒𝑒2𝑧𝑧𝑧𝑧�𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑟𝑖𝑖𝑖𝑖+1 . τ2 77 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Ta bl e 1 O ve rv ie w o f S tu di es In cl ud ed in M et a- A na ly si s A ut ho rs (y ea r) Sa m pl in g M et ho d M ea su re m en t Te ch ni qu e V ar ia bl es St at is tic al A na ly si s R es ul t I nd ep en de nt D ep en de nt A bd ul la h an d A bd . M aj id (2 00 1) N ot m en tio ne d Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey R el ig io si ty in de x an d in co m e Sa vi ng M ul tip le li ne ar re gr es si on “… th er e ex is t a c on cl us iv e re la tio ns hi p be tw ee n sa vi ng an d R el ig io si ty In de x … ” (t -s ta tis tic s = 1. 99 3, p < 0 .0 5) (p . 7 5) . Y ay eh (2 01 4) M ul tis ta ge cl us te r s am pl in g an d pr ob ab ili ty pr op or tio na l t o si ze (P PS ) s am pl in g Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey re lig io n af fil ia tio n, re lig io us at te nd an ce , r el ig io n id en tit y, ho us eh ol d ne t i nc om e pe r m on th , g en de r, ho us eh ol d ac ce pt in g in te re st p ay m en t, le ve l o f e du ca tio n, fa m ily si ze , a ge , m ar ita l s ta tu s, w ea lth , a nd k no w le dg e ab ou t sa vi ng in te re st p ay m en t. sa vi ng L in ea r a nd p ro bi t re gr es si on “… th e m or e of te n pe op le go in g to c hu rc h/ m os qu e, i. e. th e m or e re lig io us th ey a re , th e lo w er is th ei r pr op en si ty to s av e m on ey .” (W al d ch i- sq ua re o f 6 2. 58 w ith p -v al ue of 0 .0 00 ) A ba bi o an d M aw ut or (2 01 5) Si m pl e ra nd om sa m pl in g an d co nv en ie nc e sa m pl in g Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey re lig io si ty , u nc er ta in ty , liq ui di ty c on st ra in t, st ag e in lif e, a nd in te rg en er at io na l ef fe ct in co m e sa vi ng L og it m od el C hu rc h at te nd an ce v er y si gn ifi ca nt ly e xp la in s th at re lig io si ty e ff ec ts s av in g be ha vi or (o dd s ra tio = 0 .8 22 , p < 0. 05 ) ( p. 5 5) . (c on tin ue d) 78 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 A ut ho rs (y ea r) Sa m pl in g M et ho d M ea su re m en t Te ch ni qu e V ar ia bl es St at is tic al A na ly si s R es ul t I nd ep en de nt D ep en de nt Pr iy o N ug ro ho et a l. (2 01 7) Pu rp os iv e sa m pl in g Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey Se lf -e ffi ca cy , R el ig io si ty , A tti tu de , a nd S ub je ct iv e no rm In te nt io n an d B eh av io r Si m ul ta ne ou s E qu at io n M od el in g “… r el ig io si ty h as a p os iti ve an d si gn ifi ca nt in flu en ce o n cu st om er b eh av io r us in g pr od uc ts a nd s er vi ce s of Is la m ic b an ks ” (i .e . b an k sa vi ng s or d ep os its ) ( p. 4 4) . Sa ts io s an d H ad jid ak is (2 01 7) Sn ow ba ll sa m pl in g Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey re lig io si ty a nd s el f- m as te ry fiv e in te nt io ns to sa vi ng s ub sc al es : th ri ft , s av in g in vo lv em en t, sa vi ng ha bi ts , s ha m e of de bt a nd n o ne ed to s av e Pe ar so n co rr el at io n “… , r el ig io si ty is s ig ni fic an tly po si tiv el y co rr el at ed w ith al l 5 in te nt io n su bs ca le s, ... ” (p . 2 0) . T he s ub sc al e of sa vi ng h ab its is s ig ni fic an tly po si tiv el y co rr el at ed w ith re lig io si ty (r (1 00 ) = 0 .3 32 , p < 0. 01 ). Is m ai l e t a l. (2 01 8) Pu rp os iv e sa m pl in g Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey Se rv ic e qu al ity , r el ig io us be lie f, an d kn ow le dg e. sa vi ng b eh av io r M ul tip le li ne ar re gr es si on “… r el ig io us b el ie f i s si gn ifi ca nt ly r el at ed to s av in g be ha vi ou r (t = 4 .6 0, p = 0. 00 ).” (p . 1 07 6) K as si m e t a l. (2 01 9) D is pr op or tio na te st ra tifi ed s am pl in g Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey Fa m ily b ac kg ro un d, R el ig io si ty , A tti tu de , L ite ra cy , H ou se ho ld In co m e, A ge , L ev el o f e du ca tio n, a nd L oc al ity . Sa vi ng b eh av io r M ul tip le li ne ar re gr es si on “… th e re su lts d em on st ra te th at r el ig io si ty , … a re n ot si gn ifi ca nt ly r el at ed to s av in g be ha vi or .” (p . 2 48 ) ( t- st at is tic s = 1. 41 8) (c on tin ue d) 79 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 A ut ho rs (y ea r) Sa m pl in g M et ho d M ea su re m en t Te ch ni qu e V ar ia bl es St at is tic al A na ly si s R es ul t I nd ep en de nt D ep en de nt M ei T eh e t a l. (2 01 9) C on ve ni en ce sa m pl in g Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey In di vi du al c ha ra ct er is tic , So ci al is at io n, C og ni tiv e ab ili ty , R el ig io n fa ith , a nd Se lf -e ffi ca cy . Pr iv at e sa vi ng L og is tic re gr es si on “A s fo r re lig io us fa ith , d iv in e gu id an ce (o dd s ra tio = 6 .5 1) si gn ifi ca nt ly p re di ct ed a n in di vi du al ’s lik el ih oo d to s av e m on ey .” (p . 1 0) W ija ya e t a l. (2 01 9) C on ve ni en ce sa m pl in g Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey R el ig io si ty le ve l Sa vi ng d ec is io ns C hi -s qu ar e te st “… a c hi -s qu ar e te st b et w ee n re lig io si ty le ve l a nd s av in g de ci si on s cr ite ri a, w hi ch sh ow ed th er e is a s ig ni fic an t di ffe re nc e (p < 0 .0 1) . M or e th an 6 0 pe r ce nt o f t he re sp on de nt s de ci de d to s av e m on ey in B M Ts b ec au se of th ei r pr od uc ts b ei ng in ac co rd an ce w ith S ha ri a. ” (p . 14 75 ) ( ch i- sq ua re = 6 .4 63 67 ) M ur da ya nt i e t al . ( 20 20 ) Pr op or tio na te st ra tifi ed ra nd om sa m pl in g Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey Fi na nc ia l k no w le dg e, s el f- co nt ro l, an d re lig io us b el ie fs . Sa vi ng b eh av io r Pa rt ia l L ea st Sq ua re “… r el ig io us b el ie fs h av e a si gn ifi ca nt p os iti ve e ffe ct o n sa vi ng s be ha vi or , … ” (p . 8 ) (t -s ta tis tic s = 6. 77 , p < 0 .0 01 ) Pr as tiw i ( 20 21 ) N ot m en tio ne d Q ue st io nn ai re su rv ey R el ig io si ty , E nv ir on m en t, an d R ep ut at io n. Sa vi ng d ec is io n M ul tip le li ne ar re gr es si on “… R el ig io si ty , . .. ha ve a si gn ifi ca nt p os iti ve e ffe ct o n sa vi ng d ec is io ns .” (p . 2 22 ) (t -s ta tis tic s = 2. 16 1, p < 0 .0 5) (c on tin ue d) 80 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Ta bl e 2 O ve rv ie w o f D es cr ip tiv e St at is tic s an d E ffe ct S iz e A ut ho rs (y ea r) N A ge (y /o ) G en de r M ar ita l s ta tu s R el ig io n L oc at io n (C ou nt ry ) Pe ar so n r A bd ul la h an d A bd . M aj id (2 00 1) 16 0 78 .1 3% 1 8 to 2 3 19 .3 8% 2 4 to 2 9 2. 50 % 3 0 to 3 5 34 .3 8% M al e 65 .6 3% F em al e 95 .6 3% S in gl e 4. 38 % M ar ri ed M us lim In te rn at io na l I sl am ic U ni ve rs ity M al ay si a (I IU M )- Se la ng or (M al ay si a) 0. 15 76 Y ay eh (2 01 4) 38 4 42 (M ea n) N ot m en tio ne d 74 % M ar ri ed 16 % W id ow ed 67 .5 % O rt ho do x C hr is tia n 30 .8 % M us lim 1. 7% P ro te st an t W es t A m ha ra n at io na l r eg io na l st at e (E th io pi a) 0. 40 37 A ba bi o an d M aw ut or (2 01 5) 20 0 N ot m en tio ne d N ot m en tio ne d N ot m en tio ne d C hr is tia n A cc ra M et ro po lit an (G ha na ) 0. 05 4 Pr iy o N ug ro ho et a l. (2 01 7) 22 0 42 % le ss 3 1 45 % 3 1 to 4 0 13 % a bo ve 4 0 N ot m en tio ne d N ot m en tio ne d M us lim Y og ya ka rt a (I nd on es ia ) 0. 56 6 Sa ts io s an d H ad jid ak is (2 01 7) 10 0 N ot m en tio ne d N ot m en tio ne d N ot m en tio ne d M us lim X an th i, R od op i a nd E vr os - T hr ac e (G re ec e) 0. 33 2 Is m ai l e t a l. (2 01 8) 15 0 2. 7% le ss 2 0 54 % 2 0 to 3 0 29 .3 % 3 1 to 4 0 14 % a bo ve 4 0 42 .7 % M al e 57 .3 % F em al e N ot m en tio ne d 61 .3 % M us lim 15 .3 % B ud dh a 12 .7 % H in du 7. 3% C hr is tia n 3. 3% o th er s (M al ay si a) 0. 37 56 (c on tin ue d) 81 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 A ut ho rs (y ea r) N A ge (y /o ) G en de r M ar ita l s ta tu s R el ig io n L oc at io n (C ou nt ry ) Pe ar so n r K as si m e t a l. (2 01 9) 53 1 N ot m en tio ne d N ot m en tio ne d 51 .2 % M ar ri ed 48 .8 % S in gl e M us lim Se la ng or (M al ay si a) 0. 06 15 M ei T eh e t a l. (2 01 9) 22 4 16 to 6 0 N ot m en tio ne d N ot m en tio ne d M us lim (M al ay si a) 0. 45 88 W ija ya e t a l. (2 01 9) 52 3 2. 68 % 1 0 to 2 0 13 % 2 1 to 2 5 18 .1 6% 2 6 to 3 0 20 .6 5% 3 1 to 3 5 21 .0 3% 3 6 to 4 0 24 .4 7% a bo ve 40 50 .8 6% M al e 49 .1 4% F em al e N ot m en tio ne d M us lim Su ra ka rt a an d Su ko ha rj o (I nd on es ia ) 0. 11 12 M ur da ya nt i e t al . ( 20 20 ) 26 8 13 to 2 0 N ot m en tio ne d N ot m en tio ne d M us lim D ar un na ja h Is la m ic B oa rd in g Sc ho ol - Ja ka rt a (I nd on es ia ) 0. 41 35 Pr as tiw i ( 20 21 ) 10 0 12 % le ss 1 8 27 % 1 8 to 2 5 36 % 2 5 to 3 0 25 % 3 0 to 4 0 N ot m en tio ne d 45 % M al e 55 % F em al e M us lim K SP PS B M T A m an ah U m m ah - S ur ab ay a (I nd on es ia ) 0. 21 61 82 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 RESULTS Table 1 shows the results of data extraction processing. The 11 studies were published between 2001 and 2021, with Abdullah and Abd. Majid (2001) being the oldest and Prastiwi (2021) is the most recent. The studies used two types of sampling methods: probability sampling and non-probability sampling. Ababio and Mawutor (2015); Kassim et al. (2019); Murdayanti et al. (2020); and Yayeh (2014) applied the probability sampling method, whereas Ismail et al. (2018); Mei Teh et al. (2019); Priyo Nugroho et al. (2017); Satsios and Hadjidakis (2017); and Wijaya et al. (2019) employed the non-probability sampling method. Meanwhile, Abdullah and Abd. Majid (2001); and Prastiwi (2021) there was no mention of the method used in their studies. To collect primary data, all studies developed a self-administered questionnaire. Furthermore, various themes of religiosity and saving behaviour, such as religious attendance (Yayeh, 2014), religious belief (Ismail et al., 2018; Murdayanti et al., 2020), religion faith (Mei Teh et al., 2019), saving habits (Satsios & Hadjidakis, 2017), and saving decisions (Prastiwi, 2021; Wijaya et al., 2019), were used to as the independent and dependent variables. Various statistical analyses were also applied, namely Pearson correlation (Satsios & Hadjidakis, 2017), chi-square test (Wijaya et al., 2019), multiple linear regression (Abdullah & Abd. Majid, 2001; Ismail et al., 2018; Kassim et al., 2019; Prastiwi, 2021), logit regression (Ababio & Mawutor, 2015; Mei Teh et al., 2019), probit regression (Yayeh, 2014), partial least square (Murdayanti et al., 2020), and simultaneous equation modelling (Priyo Nugroho et al., 2017). On the other hand, various fit test indicators, such as chi-square (Wijaya et al., 2019; Yayeh, 2014), odds ratio (Ababio & Mawutor, 2015; Mei Teh et al., 2019), and t-statistic (Abdullah & Abd. Majid, 2001; Ismail et al., 2018; Kassim et al., 2019; Prastiwi, 2021), have been used to assess the significance of the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour. These indicators had to be converted into Pearson’s r before they are used in a meta-analysis. Meanwhile, Priyo Nugroho et al. (2017) have provided another fit test indicator, namely the critical ratio (8.395) (personal communication). Table 2 summarizes the descriptive statistics from each study, including the number of observations, age, gender, marital status, religion, and location, as well as the estimated effect sizes. As previously stated, 11 articles were used as samples for the meta- 83 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 analytic study, and these gave a total of 1,063 observations. Kassim et al. (2019) observed 531 people, making it the largest population sample, while Prastiwi (2021); as well as Satsios and Hadjidakis (2017) had 100 observations, making it the smallest. Meanwhile, the age range varied from 13 to more than 40 years. Furthermore, only four studies provided gender information, i.e., Abdullah and Abd. Majid (2001); Ismail et al. (2018); Prastiwi (2021); and Wijaya et al. (2019), in which most of their respondents were women. Similarly, marital status was only provided by three studies, i.e., Abdullah and Abd. Majid (2001); Kassim et al. (2019); and Yayeh (2014), with the majority of their respondents being married. On the other hand, all studies provided religious information, with Muslims representing the majority of their respondents. Furthermore, the study locations reveal that respondents were from a variety of countries, including Ethiopia, Ghana, Greece, Malaysia, and Indonesia. Meanwhile, all studies were ready to use the same method to express effect sizes in Pearson’s r. The study conducted by Priyo Nugroho et al. (2017) had the largest effect size (0.566), while the study performed by Ababio and Mawutor (2015) had the smallest one (0.054). Table 3 Random-effect Model Estimate se z P CI Lower Bound CI Upper Bound Intercept 0.303 0.0587 5.17 < .001 0.188 0.418 Table 4 Heterogeneity Statistics I² H² Df Q P 0.181 0.0328 (SE= 0.0171) 89.08% 9.155 10 108.574 < .001 Table 3 provides the random-effects model, and Table 4 presents the heterogeneity statistics. Based on the random-effects model, the estimated average Fisher r-to-z transformed correlation coefficient was 0.303. (95% CI: 0.188 to 0.418) and was statistically significant (z = 5.17, p < 0.001). It showed that statistically there was a positive correlation between religiosity and saving behaviour, with a true effect Table 3 Random-effect model Estimate se z P CI Lower Bound CI Upper Bound Intercept 0.303. 0.0587 5.17 < .001 0.188 0.418 Table 4 Heterogeneity statistics  ² I² H² Df Q P 0.181 0.0328 (SE= 0.0171) 89.08% 9.155 10 108.574 < .001 Note: Random-effects model (k = 11); ² estimator: Hedges Table 5 The source of heterogeneity analysis Heterogeneity source Coefficients T P 95% Confidence Interval Lower Upper Publication year -0.131 -0.395 0.703 -0.895 0.634 Sample size -0.334 -1.007 0.343 -1.098 0.431 Muslim religion -0.535 -1.79 0.111 -1.22 0.153 Figure 1 Forest plot Table 3 Random-effect model Estimate se z P CI Lower Bound CI Upper Bound Intercept 0.303. 0.0587 5.17 < .001 0.188 0.418 Table 4 Heterogeneity statistics  ² I² H² Df Q P 0.181 0.0328 (SE= 0.0171) 89.08% 9.155 10 108.574 < .001 Note: Random-effects model (k = 11); ² estimator: Hedges Table 5 The source of heterogeneity analysis Heterogeneity source Coefficients T P 95% Confidence Interval Lower Upper Publication year -0.131 -0.395 0.703 -0.895 0.634 Sample size -0.334 -1.007 0.343 -1.098 0.431 Muslim religion -0.535 -1.79 0.111 -1.22 0.153 Figure 1 Forest plot 84 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 size of 0.303 and a significance value less than 0.001. Meanwhile, according to heterogeneity statistics, the true effects appear to be non- homogenous (Q(10) = 108.574, p < 0.001). By using the Hedges’ estimator, the index (0.0328) agreed with the Q-test result, indicating that there was some between-study heterogeneity in the data, whereas = 0.181 indicated that the true effect sizes had an estimated standard deviation of SD = 0.181. In addition, the H2 (9.155) and I2 (89.08%) indices confirmed that true effect size differences account for more than half of the variation in the studies, which meant that the level of heterogeneity was high. Table 5 The Source of Heterogeneity Analysis Heterogeneity source Coefficients T P 95% Confidence Interval Lower Upper Publication year -0.131 -0.395 0.703 -0.895 0.634 Sample size -0.334 -1.007 0.343 -1.098 0.431 Muslim religion -0.535 -1.79 0.111 -1.22 0.153 Based on a random-effects model, Figure 1 depicts correlation coefficients with corresponding 95 percent confidence intervals for each study graphically. The estimated r from each study ranged from 0.0541 (Ababio & Mawutor, 2015) to 0.6416 (Priyo Nugroho et al., 2017). Meanwhile, the weights ranged from 7.99 percent to 9.92 percent, in Prastiwi (2021), with Satsios and Hadjidakis (2017) having the lowest and Kassim et al. (2019) having the highest. On the other hand, with Kassim et al. (2019) having the shortest interval and Prastiwi (2021) having the longest interval, the 95 percent confidence intervals ranged from (-0.02, 0.15) to (0.02, 0.42). Furthermore, the majority of confidence intervals were completely positive of zero, indicating that the majority of studies had a statistically significant positive effect. However, in some studies, i.e., Ababio and Mawutor (2015) and Kassim et al. (2019), the confidence intervals were not entirely positive of zero, indicating that these studies had a statistically insignificant positive effect (0.05 and 0.06). Thus, all observed dispersions reflected genuine differences in effect size and p-value between studies. However, a meta-analysis method only uses the effect size from each study rather than the p-value (Borenstein et al., 2011). 85 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Figures 2, 3, and 4 provide the outcomes of outlier and influential case diagnostics. Figure 2 shows the standardized residual values, which are used to identify outliers, while Figures 3 and 4 show the DFFITS values and Cook’s distances, which are used to detect influential cases. A look at the studentized residuals revealed that none of the studies had a value greater than ±3, indicating that there were no outliers in the random-effect model. Meanwhile, based on the DFFITS values of no more than 2 and Cook’s distances of no more than 1, none of the studies could be considered overly influential. Figure 1 Forest Plot Figure 2 Standardized Residual Table 3 Random-effect model Estimate se z P CI Lower Bound CI Upper Bound Intercept 0.303. 0.0587 5.17 < .001 0.188 0.418 Table 4 Heterogeneity statistics  ² I² H² Df Q P 0.181 0.0328 (SE= 0.0171) 89.08% 9.155 10 108.574 < .001 Note: Random-effects model (k = 11); ² estimator: Hedges Table 5 The source of heterogeneity analysis Heterogeneity source Coefficients T P 95% Confidence Interval Lower Upper Publication year -0.131 -0.395 0.703 -0.895 0.634 Sample size -0.334 -1.007 0.343 -1.098 0.431 Muslim religion -0.535 -1.79 0.111 -1.22 0.153 Figure 1 Forest plot 14 Figure 2 Standardized Residual Figure 3 DFFITS Values 86 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Figure 3 DFFITS Values Figure 4 Cook’s Distances The current study’s goal was to examine the existing empirical evidence on the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour through a meta-analysis approach. The findings show that religiosity has a positive, but weak correlation with saving behavior with r = 0.303 (p < 0.001; 95% CI = (0.188; 0.418)). As a rule of thumb, the correlation strength is described as negligible (r < 0.2), low (0.2 ≤ r < 0.4), moderate (0.4 ≤ r < 0.7), high (0.7 ≤ r < 0.9), or very high (r ≥ 0.9) (Guilford & Fruchter, 1973). According to this rule of thumb, the correlation will be ignored when its strength is less than 0.2. This finding implies that changes in religiosity have little impact on changes in saving behaviour. Moreover, the low r value was caused by the fact that the r values of the observed studies were mostly low (see Figure 1), and even two of them were negligible, i.e., Ababio and 14 Figure 2 Standardized Residual Figure 3 DFFITS Values 15 Figure 4 Cook's Distances The current study's goal was to examine the existing empirical evidence on the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour through a meta-analysis approach. The findings show that religiosity has a positive, but weak correlation with saving behavior with r = 0.303 (p < 0.001; 95% CI = (0.188; 0.418)). As a rule of thumb, the correlation strength is described as negligible (r < 0.2), low (0.2 ≤ r < 0.4), moderate (0.4 ≤ r < 0.7), high (0.7 ≤ r < 0.9), or very high (r ≥ 0.9) (Guilford & Fruchter, 1973). According to this rule of thumb, the correlation will be ignored when its strength is less than 0.2. This finding implies that changes in religiosity have little impact on changes in saving behaviour. Moreover, the low r value was caused by the fact that the r values of the observed studies were mostly low (see Figure 1), and even two of them were negligible, i.e., Ababio and Mawutor (2015), and Kassim et al. (2019). Meanwhile, there were only four studies that provided empirical evidence that religiosity had a moderate impact on saving behaviour, namely Mei Teh et al. (2019), Murdayanti et al. (2020), Priyo Nugroho et al. (2017), and Yayeh (2014). On the other hand, Ababio and Mawutor (2015) obtained the lowest r value (0.05), showing that religious belief had no effect on household savings. They assumed that religious beliefs that promoted values like frugality, hard work, and honesty did not increase the savings habits of households in Ghana. Likewise, Kassim et al. (2019) found that religiosity had no effect on saving behaviour (r = 0.06). They assumed that an individual's level of religiosity would lead to the preference to spend money in God's name rather than saving it. Unfortunately, neither of them tried to dig a bit deeper into this finding, such as linking it to the lowest frequency value of the saving questions in their questionnaire. In fact, it could be a clue to crucial evidence showing the low impact of the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour. The result obtained also supports the notion that religious people have varying understandings of saving behaviour. These understandings stem from two opposing perspectives on saving money taught by various religions, including Christianity and Islam. These religions consider saving to be either a positive or negative practice (Yayeh, 2014). In terms of saving as a positive practice, Christianity and Islam, for example, teach saving when there is enough to go around in times of scarcity (Genesis 37-50; Sahih al- Bukhari 5357). Religious people who put into practice this belief will also use their saving behaviour to support their frugal lifestyle (Agarwala et al., 2019). Meanwhile, in terms of saving as a negative practice, religions such as Christianity and Islam, for example, warn against the dangers of hoarding wealth (Luke 12:16-21; Quran 9:34). Religious people who engage in this practice are motivated by fears that if saving money becomes a passion, they will become trapped in a cycle of amassing wealth and being stingy one 87 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Mawutor (2015), and Kassim et al. (2019). Meanwhile, there were only four studies that provided empirical evidence that religiosity had a moderate impact on saving behaviour, namely Mei Teh et al. (2019), Murdayanti et al. (2020), Priyo Nugroho et al. (2017), and Yayeh (2014). On the other hand, Ababio and Mawutor (2015) obtained the lowest r value (0.05), showing that religious belief had no effect on household savings. They assumed that religious beliefs that promoted values like frugality, hard work, and honesty did not increase the savings habits of households in Ghana. Likewise, Kassim et al. (2019) found that religiosity had no effect on saving behaviour (r = 0.06). They assumed that an individual’s level of religiosity would lead to the preference to spend money in God’s name rather than saving it. Unfortunately, neither of them tried to dig a bit deeper into this finding, such as linking it to the lowest frequency value of the saving questions in their questionnaire. In fact, it could be a clue to crucial evidence showing the low impact of the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour. The result obtained also supports the notion that religious people have varying understandings of saving behaviour. These understandings stem from two opposing perspectives on saving money taught by various religions, including Christianity and Islam. These religions consider saving to be either a positive or negative practice (Yayeh, 2014). In terms of saving as a positive practice, Christianity and Islam, for example, teach saving when there is enough to go around in times of scarcity (Genesis 37-50; Sahih al-Bukhari 5357). Religious people who put into practice this belief will also use their saving behaviour to support their frugal lifestyle (Agarwala et al., 2019). Meanwhile, in terms of saving as a negative practice, religions such as Christianity and Islam, for example, warn against the dangers of hoarding wealth (Luke 12:16-21; Quran 9:34). Religious people who engage in this practice are motivated by fears that if saving money becomes a passion, they will become trapped in a cycle of amassing wealth and being stingy one day (Sawyer, 1954). As a result, in studies where there is empirical evidence that religiosity has a moderate impact on saving behaviour, their respondents are likely to view saving as a positive practice. In contrast, respondents in studies with empirical evidence that religiosity has a low impact on saving behaviour may be less open to the idea of saving as a positive practice. In addition, the current study discovered heterogeneity at a high level (Q(10) = 108.574, p < 0.001; I2 = 89.08%). Heterogeneity may exist, 88 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 because of differences in study quality (e.g., values for effect size and significance), methodology, sample size, demographic factors, and respondent characteristics. From a statistical perspective, quantifying heterogeneity can be used to determine whether or not population effect sizes are likely to be consistent or varying (Borenstein et al., 2011; Hedges & Olkin, 1985). Meanwhile, the summary information shown in Tables 1 and 2 demonstrates that the characteristics of studies were not all the same. There were differences even within the studies themselves, like variation in religious factors (see Table 2). For instance, Ismail et al. (2018); and Yayeh (2014) collected data from a variety of religious adherents, whereas other studies only collected data from a single religious adherent. As a result, the present study was motivated to investigate the cause of heterogeneity using a technique known as meta-regression analysis. In meta-regression, the effect size of each study as the independent variable is regressed on the study characteristics as the dependent variable (Chen & Peace, 2021). Furthermore, the year of publication, sample size, and Muslim religion were investigated to determine the source of heterogeneity. They acted as independent variables in the meta-regression analysis because they were relatively complete data. The results of meta- regression analysis then revealed that publication year, sample size, and Muslim religion were not statistically significant for the presence of heterogeneity (see Table 5). CONCLUSION AND FUTURE STUDIES The current study used a meta-analysis to synthesize the findings of previous studies to determine the effect of religiosity on saving behaviour. The meta-analysis of 11 journal articles and 1,063 respondents revealed that religiosity has a low impact on saving behaviour. The current study also confirmed the notion that religious people have two different perspectives on saving behaviour, holding the divergent view that saving can be either a negative or positive practice. Because the study’s findings indicated that religiosity has little influence on saving behaviour, it is possible that people with a high level of religiosity have a less rigid perspective on saving as a positive behaviour. The findings have important implications for the development of theories in the field of saving behaviour. In researching religiosity and 89 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 saving behaviour, this study has pioneered a new analytic approach known as meta-analysis. It has provided a retrospective summary of the existing literature on the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour. It could help researchers make a more informed decision about which variables to use. Furthermore, this study has added to the body of knowledge about the true effect of the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour. It could be used as a model for future research as well as a tool for additional analysis. The findings also have practical implications for increasing public awareness. This study has provided insights for authorities and financial institutions interested in encouraging religious people to save. More understanding of the findings can aid them in the improvement of plans focused on savings advocacy and savings facilitation. Savings advocacy would help religious households and individuals understand the importance of saving and resource management. It would be a critical step in developing their saving habits. Meanwhile, financial institutions can create a type of savings account that corresponds to the religious predisposition. It would promote the notion that saving money is a good deed. The plans are beneficial in encouraging religious people to engage in responsible financial behaviour. Collective savings will have a significant impact on the economy if these individuals have a strong savings predisposition. However, a number of important limitations of the study have to be considered. First, the current meta-analytic method has synthesized only a few studies. Although the limited sample used in the present study was acceptable, the findings would be more reliable if a larger number of studies could be synthesized. Second, of the 11 previous studies that were synthesized, Islam was the religion which had the highest proportion of adherents, with only a small proportion of adherents from other religions. Although that was the nature of the meta-analytic samples, the current pattern of results could be more generalizable if the proportion of adherents was more balanced. Third, the current study synthesized 11 studies had used primary data and measured variables with a questionnaire. This technique opens the possibility of a person’s proclivity to respond to a questionnaire with a positive self-image, also known as a socially desirable response (Van de Mortel, 2008). Although bias has been reduced by selecting studies from reputable journals, it is still possible to be biased when answering sensitive questions, such as religious ones. 90 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Furthermore, more research is needed to examine the relationship between religiosity and saving behaviour, more specifically, the importance of using longitudinal studies at multiple points in time. This type of research could shed light on the minor impact of the relationship and contribute to a critical explanation for this phenomenon. ACKNOWLEDGMENT This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. REFERENCES Ababio, A. G., & Mawutor, G. (2015). Does religion matter for savings habit of households in Ghana? Singaporean Journal of Business Economics and Management Studies, 4(8). https:// doi.org/10.12816/0019680 Abdullah, N., & Abd. Majid, M. S. (2001). Saving behaviour in Islamic framework: The case of international Islamic University Malaysia. Jurnal Syariah, 9(2), 61-84. https://mjs.um.edu.my/ index.php/JS/article/view/22905. Agarwala, R., Mishra, P., & Singh, R. (2019). Religiosity and consumer behavior: A summarizing review. Journal of Management, Spirituality & Religion, 16(1), 32-54. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 14766086.2018.1495098 Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 179-211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-t Allport, G. W., & Ross, J. M. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5(4), 432-443. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0021212 Arruñada, B. (2010). Protestants and Catholics: Similar work ethic, different social ethic. The Economic Journal, 120(547), 890- 918. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0297.2009.02325.x Azzi, C., & Ehrenberg, R. (1975). Household allocation of time and church attendance. Journal of Political Economy, 83(1), 27-56. https://doi.org/10.1086/260305 Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall. 91 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Barro, R. J., & McCleary, R. M. (2003). Religion and economic growth across countries. American Sociological Review, 68(5), 760. https://doi.org/10.2307/1519761 Basedau, M., Gobien, S., & Prediger, S. (2018). The multidimensional effects of religion on socioeconomic development: A review of the empirical literature. Journal of Economic Surveys, 32(4), 1106-1133. https://doi.org/10.1111/joes.12250 Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., & Dasen, P. R. (2002). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications: Cambridge University Press. Bilen, M. (2016). Global wealth inequality: Islamic economic perspective for market or state oriented solution. Turkish Journal of Islamic Economics, 3(1), 17-35. https://doi. org/10.15238/TUJISE.2014.3.1.17-35 Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., Rothstein, H. R., & Higgins, D. J. P. T. (2011). Introduction to meta-analysis. John Wiley & Sons. Chen, D.-G. D., & Peace, K. E. (2021). Applied meta-analysis with R and Stata. CRC Press. Cheung, M. W.-L., Ho, R. C. M., Lim, Y., & Mak, A. (2012). Conducting a meta-analysis: Basics and good practices. International Journal of Rheumatic Diseases, 15(2), 129-135. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-185X.2012.01712.x Cochran, W. G. (1954). Some methods for strengthening the common χ2 tests. Biometrics, 10(4), 417-451. https://doi. org/10.2307/3001616 Cook, R. D. (1977). Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics, 19(1), 15-18. https://doi. org/10.2307/1268249 Davutyan, N., & Öztürkkal, B. (2016). Determinants of saving- borrowing decisions and financial inclusion in a high middle income country: The Turkish case. Emerging Markets Finance and Trade, 52(11), 2512-2529. https://doi.org/10.1080/154049 6X.2016.1187596 Field, A. P., & Gillett, R. (2010). How to do a meta-analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 63(3), 665-694. https://doi.org/10.1348/000711010X502733 Filipova, L. (2012). Are attitudes conducive to economic growth stronger in protestants than in others? https://nupi.brage. unit.no/nupi-xmlui/bitstream/handle/11250/277240/WP--804- Lenka-R.pdf?sequence=3 92 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays (Vol. 5019): Basic books. Gerbing, D. (2014). Influence of data points on the estimated model. School of Business Administration Portland State University. http://web.pdx.edu/~gerbing/515/Resources/Influence.pdf Glock, C. Y. (1962). On the study of religious commitment. Religious Education, 57(sup4), 98-110. https://doi. org/10.1080/003440862057S407 Guilford, J. P., & Fruchter, B. (1973). Fundamental statistics in psychology and education (5th ed.). McGraw-Hill. Guiso, L., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2003). People’s opium? Religion and economic attitudes. Journal of Monetary Economics, 50(1), 225-282. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304- 3932(02)00202-7 Hayward, R. D., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2011). Weber revisited: A cross-national analysis of religiosity, religious culture, and economic attitudes. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(8), 1406-1420. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111412527 Hedges, L., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press. Higgins, J. P., & Thompson, S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21(11), 1539-1558. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.1186 Iannaccone, L. R. (1998). Introduction to the economics of religion. Journal of Economic Literature, 36(3), 1465-1495. Inglehart, R. (2018). Culture shift in advanced industrial society. Princeton University Press. Ismail, S., Khairuddin, N. S., Alias, N. E., Loon-Koe, W., & Othman, R. (2018). An empirical analysis of saving behavior among Malaysian employees. International Journal of Academic Research in Business and Social Sciences, 8(10), 1070-1080. https://doi.org/10.6007/ijarbss/v8-i10/4822 Iyer, S. (2016). The new economics of religion. Journal of Economic Literature, 54(2), 395-441. https://doi.org/10.1257/ JEL.54.2.395 Kassim, A. A. M., Mohamed, J., Jahari, N. A., Nasyikin, N., & Zain, M. (2019). Personal saving behavior among Muslim employees. International Journal of Academic Research in Business and Social Sciences, 9(11). https://doi.org/10.6007/ ijarbss/v9-i11/6545 Keister, L. A. (2003). Religion and wealth: The role of religious affiliation and participation in early adult asset accumulation. 93 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Social Forces, 82(1), 175-207. https://doi.org/10.1353/ sof.2003.0094 Klaubert, A. (2010). Striving for savings: Religion and individual economic behavior Working Paper Series in Economics. Lüneburg: Leuphana Universität Lüneburg, Institut für Volkswirtschaftslehre. Law, K. S. (1995). The use of Fisher’s Z in Schmidt-Hunter. Type meta-analyses. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 20(3), 287-306. https://doi.org/10.2307/1165404 Lenhard, W., & Lenhard, A. (2016). Calculation of effect sizes. https://www.psychometrica.de/effect_size.html Lenski, G. (1961). The religious factor: A sociological study of religion’s impact on politics, economics, and family life. Garden City. Mei Teh, G., Palil, M. R., Mohd Zain, N. H., & Mamat, N. H. (2019). Predicting private saving within Malay community. Asian Academy of Management Journal, 24(Supp.1), 1-17. https:// doi.org/10.21315/aamj2019.24.s1.1 Murdayanti, Y., Ulupui, I., Gurendrawati, E., & Purwohedi, U. (2020). An assessment of saving behavior in the religious education institution. Journal of Southwest Jiaotong University, 55(4). https://doi.org/10.35741/ISSN.0258-2724.55.4.27 Nyhus, E. K. (2017). Saving behaviour: Economic and psychological approaches. In R. Ranyard (Ed.), Economic Psychology (pp. 206). John Wiley & Sons. Pargament, K. I. (1999). The psychology of religion and spirituality? Yes and no. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 9(1), 3-16. https://doi.org/10.1207/ S15327582IJPR0901_2 Pew Research Center. (2015). The future of world religions: Population growth projections, 2010-2050. https://assets.pewresearch. org/wpcontent/uploads/sites/11/2015/03/PF_15.04.02_ ProjectionsFullReport.pdf Prastiwi, I. E. (2021). Analysis of the influence of religiosity, environment, and reputation factors on customer decisions on saving at KSPPS BMT amanah ummah. International Journal of Economics, Business and Accounting Research (IJEBAR), 5(1). https://doi.org/10.29040/IJEBAR.V5I1.2237 Priyo Nugroho, A., Hidayat, A., & Kusuma, H. (2017). The influence of religiosity and self-efficacy on the saving behavior of the Іslamic banks. Banks and Bank Systems, 12(3), 35-47. https:// doi.org/10.21511/bbs.12(3).2017.03 94 The International Journal of Banking and Finance, Vol. 18, Number 1 (January) 2023, pp: 67–94 Renneboog, L., & Spaenjers, C. (2012). Religion, economic attitudes, and household finance. Oxford Economic Papers, 64(1), 103– 127. https://doi.org/10.1093/oep/gpr025 Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological B u l l e t i n , 11 8 ( 2 ) , 1 8 3 . h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 0 3 7 / 0 0 3 3 - 2909.118.2.183 Satsios, N., & Hadjidakis, S. (2017). The effect of religiosity and self mastery on saving behaviour of Pomak households in thrace. Journal Sustaibable Development, Culture, Traditions, 17-27. https://doi.org/10.26341/issn.2241-4002-2017-1b-2 Sawyer, W. (1954). Christ’s teachings on the use of money. https:// scholarworks.harding.edu/wyatt-sawyersermons/281 Singh, G., Sharma, S., Sharma, R., & Dwivedi, Y. K. (2021). Investigating environmental sustainability in small family- owned businesses: Integration of religiosity, ethical judgment, and theory of planned behavior. Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 173, 121094. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. techfore.2021.121094 The Jamovi Project. (2021). Jamovi (Version 1.6) [Computer Software]. https://www.jamovi.org Teshome, H. M., Ayalew, G. D., Shiferaw, F. W., Leshargie, C. T., & Boneya, D. J. (2018). The prevalence of depression among diabetic patients in Ethiopia: A systematic review and meta- analysis. Depression Research and Treatment, 6135460. https:// doi.org/10.1155/2018/6135460 Tjahjono, G. (2014). Religiosity and the intention to buy luxury goods among young Indonesian Muslims in Jakarta (Unpublished master’s thesis). University of Western Sydney. Van de Mortel, T. F. (2008). Faking it: Social desirability response bias in self-report research. Australian Journal of Advanced Nursing, 25(4), 40-48. Weber, M. (1905). The Protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism. Charles Scribner’s Sons. Wijaya, I. F., Hakim, A. R., Saputro, N., & Mulyadi, M. (2019). Religiosity level and saving decisions in Baitul Maal wat Tamwil: The case of Indonesia. Journal of Islamic Marketing, 11(6), 1465-1483. https://doi.org/10.1108/JIMA-09-2018-0160 Yayeh, F. A. (2014). The impact of religion on household saving behavior. Microeconomics: Intertemporal Consumer Choice & Savings eJournal. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2518129 Zuckerman, M., Silberman, J., & Hall, J. A. (2013). The relation between intelligence and religiosity: A meta-analysis and some proposed explanations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17(4), 325-354. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868313497266