International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

DOI: 10.18357/ijcyfs111202019475 

YOUTH CONCEPTUALIZATION OF RESILIENCE STRATEGIES IN 

FOUR LOW- AND MIDDLE-INCOME COUNTRIES 

Panos Vostanis, Sadiyya Haffejee, Hikmet Yazici, Sajida Hussein, 

Munevver Ozdemir, Cansu Tosun, and John Maltby 

Abstract: The concept of resilience is increasingly influential in the development 

of interventions and services for young people, yet there is limited knowledge of 

how resilience-building strategies are conceptualized by young people across 

different cultures, particularly in low- and middle-income countries. The aim of this 

study was to capture 274 young people’s voices in disadvantaged communities in 

Kenya, Turkey, Pakistan, and Brazil through participatory research methods. 

Young people defined strategies in response to 4 adversity scenarios reflecting 

socioecological systems (young person, family, school, and community). Template 

analysis, underpinned by thematic design, was used to establish three broad themes 

of intrapersonal (self-management, cognitive re-appraisal, agency), interpersonal 

(social engagement, informal supports, formal supports), and religious resources. 

Proposed strategies were largely similar across the sites, with some contextual 

differences depending on the scenario (stressor) and cultural group. The findings 

support an ecological systems approach to resilience, which is consistent with the 

development of multimodal interventions for vulnerable youth and their families in 

disadvantaged communities in low- and middle-income countries. 

Keywords: young people, resilience, participatory research, psychosocial, cross-

cultural, low- and middle-income countries 

Panos Vostanis MD (corresponding author) is a Professor of Child Mental Health at the University 

of Leicester; George Davies Centre, University Road, Leicester LE1 7RH, UK. 

Email: pv11@le.ac.uk 

Sadiyya Haffejee PhD is a Research Fellow at the University of KwaZulu-Natal, 238 Mazisi 

Kunene Rd, Glenwood, Durban, 4041, South Africa. Email: sadiyya.haffejee@gmail.com 

Hikmet Yazici PhD is a Professor of Psychological Counselling and Guidance at Trabzon 

University, 61335, Trabzon, Turkey. Email: hyaziciktu@gmail.com 

Sajida Hussein PhD is a Director in the Child Development Programme, Hussaini Foundation, 

Karachi, Pakistan. Email: sajida79@googlemail.com 

Munevver Ozdemir MA is a Research Assistant at Trabzon University, 61335, Trabzon, Turkey. 

Email:  mnvr.ozdemir@outlook.com 

Cansu Tosun BA is a Research Assistant at Trabzon University, 61335, Trabzon, Turkey. 

Email:  cansu.tosun@ktu.edu.tr 

John Maltby PhD is a Professor of Differential Psychology at the University of Leicester, George 

Davies Centre, University Road, Leicester LE1 7RH, UK. Email:  jm148@le.ac.uk  

mailto:pv11@le.ac.uk
mailto:sadiyya.haffejee@gmail.com
mailto:hyaziciktu@gmail.com
mailto:sajida79@googlemail.com
mailto:mnvr.ozdemir@outlook.com
mailto:cansu.tosun@ktu.edu.tr
mailto:jm148@le.ac.uk


International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

93 

 

Globally, there is growing recognition of the need to increase the priority of addressing the 

mental health needs of young people (Patel & Rahman, 2015). Young people are potentially at risk 

for a range of adverse psychosocial outcomes resulting from exposure to complex vulnerability 

factors — such as adverse living conditions, poverty, violence, migration, and exposure to 

environmental hazards and communicable diseases — coupled with limited access to appropriate 

services and supports (Getanda et al., 2017; Kieling et al., 2011). Recent years have thus seen an 

expansion of research focused on factors that enable and support children’s resilience in the face 

of personal, familial, and environmental hazards (Ungar, 2004). 

In spite of considerable academic disparity in defining and explaining resilience processes, 

there is increasing consensus that resilience is not an inherent, static trait that resides within an 

individual (Garmezy, 1991; Masten, 2006; Rutter, 2006; Theron, 2018; Ungar, 2011; Werner, 

1989). Researchers in this field concur that resilience is a complex phenomenon that is sensitive 

to and dependent on sociocultural and contextual factors (Maltby et al., 2016; Panter-Brick, 2015; 

Theron, 2018; Ungar, 2011). In this way, resilience researchers acknowledge, to varying degrees, 

the role of the environment in nurturing resilience. Ungar (2011, 2012, 2015) urged a shift in our 

understanding of resilience by highlighting the seminal importance of both context and culture in 

his socioecological theory of resilience (SERT). Ungar (2011, 2012) thus held that resilience is a 

dynamic, culturally entrenched process shared between an individual and their context; in SERT, 

he introduced principles of decentrality and cultural relativity. Decentrality advocates for a shift 

in focus, away from the children themselves and towards what is happening around them that 

facilitates resilience, while cultural relativity suggests that adaptation is embedded in culture. 

Masten and Wright (2010) identify six universally occurring adaptive systems that inform 

resilience: the presence of supportive attachment relationships; the individual’s cognitive aptitude; 

self-regulation skills; the ability to make meaning; agency and mastery; and the influence of culture 

and traditions. The way these systems operate, however, varies across contexts and periods, and 

their use in analysis thus needs to be grounded in contextual awareness (Theron, 2018; Theron & 

Theron, 2013; Ungar, 2015). This focus on cross-cultural understanding of resilience is becoming 

increasingly popular, as it enables a more nuanced understanding of adaptive processes across 

cultures and nations (Sirikantraporn et al., 2018). 

The development of culturally sensitive and contextualized interventions requires a clearer 

understanding of how resilience and resilience-building are characterized by young people, 

families, and communities, especially in life circumstances of conflict and disadvantage 

(Tamburrino et al., 2018). Youth in these contexts experience different adversities and have access 

to different resources. Like adversity, resilience is thus conceptualized differently depending on 

context and culture (Huang, 2018). What may be seen as resilience in one context might not be 

seen that way in another. Context and culture determine the meaning apportioned to manifestations 

of resilience (Panter-Brick, 2015). For example, studies conducted with youth in sub-Saharan 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

94 

countries highlight the importance of social and kin relationships and of education in the resilience 

processes of youth (Theron & Phasha, 2015; Theron & van Rensburg, 2018; van Breda & Theron, 

2018). For young people in Pakistan, sharing problems with families was ranked highly, in contrast 

to the value placed upon independent decision-making in Western cultures (Sahar & Muzaffar, 

2017). For child labourers in Brazil, Libório and Ungar (2014) found that protective processes 

related to resilience were partly present as a result of the “pattern of protagonism among these 

youth” (p. 682). Four types of protective processes were identified: improving behaviour and view 

of oneself, increasing positive engagement with others, actively participating in the community, 

and engaging in cultural activities including the practice of artistic skills. Based on these findings, 

Libório and Ungar (2014) concluded that resilience processes for youth in Brazil are products of 

the individual’s subjectivity, their active engagement in interpersonal relationships, and their 

participation in valued cultural practices; hence resilience processes work in harmony with a 

facilitative social ecology. 

Despite the increasing interest in young people’s resilience across the world, and how this 

could be informed by the socioecological systems model, there is limited knowledge on how 

different resilience strategies could be devised by youth across cultural contexts and 

socioecological levels. There is also sparse evidence on how different types of resilience strategies 

may be linked to help-seeking patterns and to existing supports and services, particularly in low- 

and middle-income countries (LMIC) and areas of disadvantage. In recent years, we have 

developed a global program of mental health service transformation for vulnerable young people 

in LMIC, called World Awareness for Children in Trauma1 (WACIT; Vostanis, 2018, 2019). This 

program is based on an interdisciplinary scaled service model (United Nations High Commissioner 

for Refugees, 2013; World Health Organization, 2008) to address young people’s mental health 

needs at different socioecological levels by intervening through the young person and their family, 

school, and community. To date, we have captured the perspectives of adult stakeholders who have 

identified key barriers to access to mental health care. These included the stigma of mental illness, 

lack of awareness, lack of parental engagement, and absence of interdisciplinary cooperation 

(Vostanis, Maltby, et al., 2018). So far, young people’s voices have been absent in informing 

culturally appropriate and engaging interventions and services in LMIC. This study, therefore, 

attempts to fill this gap by exploring how young people across four culturally and contextually 

different communities in Kenya, Turkey, Brazil, and Pakistan conceptualized resilience strategies. 

  

 
1 www.wacit.org 

http://www.wacit.org/


International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

95 

Method 

The study aimed to address the following research questions: 

1. How do young people formulate resilience strategies when faced with different 

adversity scenarios reflecting the levels of the socioecological model? 

2. How do these strategies contrast across cultural groups in four LMIC? 

Consistent with these research questions, while placing the method in a person-centred and 

culturally appropriate context, a participatory research design was adopted. This research tool 

enables groups collectively affected by a problem to “participate” in the process of developing 

“actions” to address that problem. Consequently, this approach enables the active engagement of 

community members to raise awareness of the issues, and relies on them as a source of expertise 

in facilitating possible solutions (Reason, 1994). The process of engagement and co-production 

recognizes the relevance and importance of language and shared meaning, as well as emphasizing 

and valuing participants’ voices. Consistent with this, therefore, a social constructionist person-

centred theoretical framework underpinned our methods. This approach to data collection and the 

analytic process identifies and illuminates the constructed accounts of the young participants (Burr, 

2003). 

Participatory research is increasingly used to cooperatively address problems affecting 

individuals who are marginalized or excluded from service planning and implementation, such as 

youth from disadvantaged LMIC communities (Rhodes et al., 2015; Willow, 2013). Participants 

do not focus on merely understanding a given problem; rather, participants and researchers develop 

actions collectively (Kidd & Kral, 2005). 

Context and Participants 

Young people were selected from four LMIC across the socioeconomic spectrum 

(Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development, 2016). The sites were partners to the 

previously described WACIT program. Within each country, a host non-governmental 

organization (NGO) acted as facilitator and gatekeeper to the study. The target population included 

young people in disadvantaged urban communities in Nakuru, Kenya; Rio de Janeiro, Brazil; 

Trabzon, Turkey; and Karachi, Pakistan. In Pakistan, young people in care were also included as 

an additional representation of young people with complex psychosocial needs. This research 

focused on young people’s voices, as it is important to understand how they define resilience 

strategies and help-seeking, in order to develop engaging, youth-friendly interventions and 

services. Young people were recruited from schools in collaboration with a host NGO, which also 

acted as the partner of the wider WACIT program in each country (Vostanis, O’Reilly, et al., 

2018). Although we aimed to include age groups of mid-to-late adolescence in each country, this 

was not possible in Brazil, where the collaborating school was a primary education setting, so we 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

96 

included their oldest grade. In total, 274 young people between the ages of 10 and 17 years took 

part. Their characteristics are described in Table 1. 

Table 1. Characteristics of Participating Young People (N=274) 

Country Location n Groups Gender F Gender M Age 

Kenya School 1 19 3 9 10 14 

Kenya School 2 48 6 19 29 15–17 

Turkey School 1 32 5 16 16 16 

Turkey School 2 35 5 18 17 16 

Brazil School 1 17 4 8 9 10–11 

Pakistan School 1 23 6 18 5 13 

Pakistan School 2 34 4 19 15 13–14 

Pakistan Care Home 66 6 66 0 10–13 

 

Research Procedure 

Young people were asked to define resilience strategies in response to scenarios that were 

considered common and easily understood within their sociocultural context. The scenarios were 

underpinned by the ecological systems framework; that is, they were developed to reflect stressors 

at four levels representing distinct socioecological locations: within the young person, their family, 

their school, and their community (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). Scenarios were created based on the 

literature, then revised in consultation with the host facilitators. It was concluded that three 

scenarios relating resepctively to the young person, the family, and the school were universal to 

all countries, while the fourth, concerning the community, varied even within each country. For 

this reason, the community scenario was customized by each of the four host facilitators. Young 

people were asked how they could feel better, or advise another young person in the same situation 

to feel better, if faced with the following scenarios, each at a different socioecological level: 

• Young person: Feeling sad or unhappy, without being aware of a specific trigger. 

• Family: Parents, caregivers, or other adults arguing verbally or physically at home. 

• School: Another young person calling them names or hurting them at school (all groups 

understood the word “bullied”). 

• Community: Scenarios were codeveloped, depending on the context and common events 

in each country and community: 

o Kenya: No water in the neighbourhood or community. 

o Turkey: Fire in the neighbourhood or community. 

o Brazil: Violence in the neighbourhood or community. 

o Pakistan schools: Criminality in the neighbourhood or community. 

o Pakistan care home: Frequent power breakdowns (lasting more than 8 hours). 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

97 

Ethics approval was granted by the University of Leicester Psychology Research Ethics 

Committee following the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki, with the host agencies 

acting as gatekeepers. Parents (or legal guardians for young people in care) and young people 

provided consent to take part. Within each workshop, young people were asked to break into small 

mixed-gender groups of 6–8 participants each. They were given pens and a large piece of paper 

for each exercise. Young people were free to interact and discuss their responses, but were 

encouraged to write them down (“there is no right or wrong”). They were encouraged to support 

their written statements with drawings if they found it useful to do so. This option was mainly used 

by the youngest group, in Brazil. 

Each small group had a young facilitator in the discussion, who was also responsible for 

collating co-produced responses in writing. In Kenya, the discussions were largely in English, and 

partly in Swahili; all collected material was in English. In Turkey and Brazil respectively, 

discussions and collected material were in Turkish and Portuguese; these were subsequently 

translated into English. In Pakistan, the school participants carried out their discussions in English 

and Urdu, and wrote in English; while in the care home, the young people spoke and wrote in Balti 

(Figure 1), and their materials were translated into English. 

Figure 1. Example of Materials Created in Response to Scenario 1 (Feeling Sad) in Pakistan 

 
 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

98 

Analytic Approach 

Because of the required participatory methods, the age and cultural diversity of the 

participants, and the complexity of the evidence surrounding resilience in LMIC, it was deemed 

appropriate to utilize template analysis to address the research questions. Template analysis is a 

form of thematic analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006) that identifies salient issues in the data and the 

literature to represent the key topics at stake (King, 2004). Its emphasis is on participant meaning 

creation; to this end, it provides a structural framework to create a template of categories. The 

initial template was shaped and informed by the adopted conceptual framework (ecological 

systems and the four related scenarios). However, the template was bidirectional, in the sense that 

it was further influenced by the social-constructionist theoretical framework and by the youth 

perspectives that emerged through the participatory techniques. The template analysis thus allowed 

the research agenda and evidence base to influence the initial template design. 

This further encouraged the refinement and redevelopment of the template through the 

iterative inclusion of participant views to map and distinguish between relevant concepts (Johnson 

et al., 2013). The template was codeveloped within the team, as first-order categories were 

identified by authors Tosun and Vostanis. These were then subjected to second-order analysis by 

authors Haffejee and Vostanis to establish the final themes and subthemes across domains and 

countries, thus arriving at the final template. The analysis was data-driven: the emerging themes 

and subthemes were not mapped onto existing resilience theories during the analytic stage, 

although the findings are critically considered in the context of key theories and classifications in 

the Discussion section of this article. 

Results 

Template analysis was thus utilized to identify common resilience-building strategies 

(categories) across the four scenarios, as put forward by the 274 young people from the four LMIC. 

Emerging categories at the first analytic stage were combined into three themes and six subthemes 

in the second-order analysis, as presented in Table 2. Themes had different numbers of subthemes 

and categories. For example, religion was considered a conceptually independent theme, despite 

the smaller amount of data and lack of subthemes or categories. There is potential overlap between 

some categories, either because of conceptual similarities or because of lack of supporting context 

or reported examples regarding the defined strategies. These are discussed below, as well as the 

rationale for choosing these categories, subthemes, and themes. Key terms used and characteristic 

responses are also presented in Table 2. 

  



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

99 

Table 2. Resilience-Building Strategies Proposed by Young People in Four LMIC (N=274) 

Subtheme Category Key terms Characteristic responses 

Theme 1: Intrapersonal resources 

Self-

management 

Individual 

free-time 

activities 

Music, eat, food, read, 

film, mobile, TV, study, 

keep busy 

Listen to cool music 

Read some comic books 

Eat something sweet 

 Emotional 

regulation 

Cry, yell, calm down, cool 

down 

Cry to release the pain 

Scream out loud 

Calm down, so I do nothing wrong 

Unburden what you feel 

 Humour Funny, joking, going out Cheer the person up 

Tell him funny jokes so that he becomes happy 

Read funny jokes 

Make him laugh, smile, and when he is comfortable then 

we can ask the problem 

Cognitive 

reappraisal 

 Good moments, happy 

memories, assurance, self-

esteem 

Try to see the good side of life, even in your darkest 

hours 

Talk about good things 

See the good side of life 

Thinking of a good time 

Remember happy moments to feel better 

Agency Active 

adaptive 

Solve problem, understand 

reason, negotiate, patient, 

tolerate, control, educate, 

explain, face 

Educate them on importance of a peaceful family 

Try to reconcile them 

Tell him that he can change 

Behave oppositely 

Discuss and give a solution 

Stay safe 

 Non-active Stay away, sleep, ignore, 

avoid, don’t care, 

apologise 

Walk away and avoid them 

Do nothing 

Leave home 

Change schools 

Run away 

 Active 

maladaptive 

Fight, beat, harm, self-

harm, swear, humiliate 

Fight him back 

Beat people who do this 

Turn it on him/herself 

Hurt him back 

Laugh at his stupid face 

Theme 2: Interpersonal resources 

Social 

engagement 

 Fun, enjoy, party, park, 

game, go out, friends 

To go and enjoy, so that it [their distress] can come out 

Hanging out with friends 

Play in a friend’s house 

Have a party 

Informal 

supports 

 Talk, advice, ask, share, 

help, comfort, hug, friends, 

parents, grandparents, 

elders 

Talk to someone who understands, especially elderly 

people 

Go to the neighbour’s house 

Stay with someone I care for 

Formal 

supports 

 Tell, report, complain, 

authority, counselling, 

community elders 

Report to teacher 

Talk to peer counsellor 

Report to chief and village leaders 

Engage services of a pastor 

Complain to nearest authority such as police 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

100 

Subtheme Category Key terms Characteristic responses 

Theme 3: Religion 

  Praying, God, recite Pray and forget about it 

Encourage him to pray 

What goes round comes round 

Allah see you and Allah punish you 

By the leave of God 

 

Theme One: Intrapersonal Resources 

Young people drew on a range of their own resources across sites and scenarios. These 

included the subthemes of self-management, cognitive reappraisal, and agency, with the young 

person placing themselves as central to the solution, whether through introspection or through 

interaction with their environment. 

The first subtheme of self-management included the use of individual free-time activities, 

emotional regulation, and humour. Individual free-time activities appeared to act as generic 

strategies for space or distraction. These largely consisted of reading, listening to music, and 

eating. A developmental (teenage) connotation in these strategies appeared to transcend culture 

(listen to “music”, “good music”, “cool music”, “funk”). Some strategies were framed in the third 

person, as if recommended to another young person facing the stressor (“focus on his own 

activity”). Maybe because of their younger age, this category was mostly put forward by 

participants in Brazil. Interestingly, it was not mentioned at all by young people in care, perhaps 

because of the care home structure and lack of opportunities to function individually. 

Emotional regulation or containment mainly included crying across all groups, followed 

by screaming (“yell”, “scream out loud”, “shout”). These seemed to have a cathartic connotation 

rather than being focused on emotional reprocessing. In contrast, some proposed strategies 

included a regulatory function (“crying to release the pain”, “unburden what you feel”). Several 

statements indicated relaxation techniques such as “calm down”, “take a deep breath”, or “count 

to 10”. No differences were detected in the use of emotions in dealing with stressors or in distinct 

emotion-focused strategies at any of the four sites. Children in the care home group did not report 

any emotion-focused strategies in response to the scenarios. Emotional strategies were evenly 

reported across the four scenarios. 

Humour was the least reported category, but we considered the content of the statements 

to underpin a distinct concept. Strategies were usually placed in a social context, by “telling jokes” 

or “making and listening to jokes”, or by referring to another young person (“cheer the person up”, 

“we should tell him/her funny jokes, so he/she becomes happy”). A more complex strategy led to 

tackling the problem: “We will make him laugh, smile, and when he is comfortable, then we can 

ask the problem.” Reframing bullying with humour included “say thank you for giving beautiful 

name to me” and “I love the name you give me”. 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

101 

The second subtheme of cognitive reappraisal differed from the first in that there were 

contextual differences among the first three scenarios. In response to inner feelings of sadness, 

cognitive strategies included positive appraisal (“see the good side of life”, “thinking of a good 

time”), or forgetting “bad things”. An interesting application of cognitive reappraisal of feeling 

sad was the “happy game” quoted by a Brazilian young person, according to which “in your worst 

times you try to see the good side of life, even in your darkest hours”. When faced with adversity 

at the level of the home or family, responses included some of the previous strategies of positive 

appraisal and forgetting, as well as context-related “assurance”, potential self-blame (“think 

whether it is your fault”), distraction (“not concentrate on their arguments”), and recollection of 

positive family memories (“we can see good memories in the album”). In relation to being bullied 

at school, the majority of statements reflected internal coping strategies such as minimizing the 

impact (“we don’t mind what he is saying”, “will not take seriously”), feeling stronger (“having 

high self-esteem”), and using moral arguments (“keep the respect of humanity”). Some cognitive 

responses were directed at the aggressor in “forgiving”, “think that person is mad”, or “not mind 

what he is saying”. 

The third subtheme of agency largely included independent and active responses to the 

given stressor. Young people thus considered a range of active strategies. These were the most 

frequently reported in response to the family, school, and community scenarios. Categories were 

similar across countries, although their content (application) sometimes varied according to the 

context of the scenario. Strategies common to the scenarios were understanding (“ask reasons”) 

and solving the problem, or a combination of the two (some of which overlapped with interpersonal 

strategies): “… and when he has more fun, and when we had a lot of fun together, then he can 

easily share, and will try to solve his problem”. 

Faced with adversity in the home (parental arguments or fighting), the first steps were 

neutral listening (“to both sides”), talking (“I kuongekiesha wazazi” [“talking to parents” in 

Swahili]), and showing love (“hug them”; “Mum, I’m sorry, I love you”; “give a kiss”) or offering 

comfort (snack, ice cream, watch TV). These were followed by active attempts to stop the 

argument or fight by “reconciling”, “educating on importance of peaceful family”, or “calming 

them down”. The third step consisted of pointing out the impact of the family conflict on the young 

person: “it is [having an] effect on my study and behaviours”, “try to divert grown-ups attention 

towards him/her”, “I’m gonna run away”, and “live on the streets”. 

The above three processes also emerged in relation to facing adversity at school. 

Understanding, talking, tolerance (“behaving oppositely”), patience, and attempts at reconciliation 

(“do an agreement with him to stop bullying”) were commonly reported. Education and advice 

were often interlinked (“… that he can change”, “the effects of bullying”, “importance of keeping 

peace and respect”, “explaining that I also hurt”). The third and more active — albeit not 

confrontational, in contrast with a later subtheme — approach emphasized “facing the bully with 

bravery and intelligence” and being willing to “stand up for your rights”: insist on respect, issue a 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

102 

warning, but also “smile in front of him”. An interesting wider stance was “raising awareness about 

the effects of bullying”. 

Strategies responding to the community and neighbourhood scenario were responsive to 

the local context, but shared conceptual similarities. Solutions to lack of water and electricity 

showed willingness to actively participate rather than first seeking adult involvement: for water, 

fetch more from boreholes or rivers, or buy it; for electricity, light candles/lanterns, switch on a 

generator, or use an emergency light. Community and proactive actions were also suggested such 

as digging boreholes, planting more trees to attract rain, installing solar energy, and arranging 

electricity beforehand. Facing crime or a natural disaster predominantly involved safety-first 

reactions: for crime, staying in the house, locking up doors and windows, and not letting strangers 

in; for fire, raising the alarm and escaping from the fire. Nevertheless, young people also offered 

altruistic solutions such as keeping their younger siblings in a room and “acting like a security 

guard” in the face of violence, raising community awareness against violence (e.g, with a poster), 

or helping to put a fire out and save other people. 

Whether the strategies included in the next subtheme of non-active responses had an 

adaptive or maladaptive function was often not clear in the absence of elaboration from the young 

people regarding behaviours such as running away from an incident or leaving a room where 

parents are arguing. The most common responses denoted distraction and hope for comfort, largely 

by “doing nothing”, sleeping (“hope to see nice and sweet dream”, “so cannot be scared”), or 

keeping busy. 

Active but maladaptive strategies came up in every group in response to conflict and 

aggression. These were verbal, physical, or internalized. When faced with parental conflict, young 

people considered shouting, involving themselves in the fight, warning parents that they would 

also fight, being “mischievous”, or “slapping both of them”. A number of young people from 

Turkey considered self-harming as a way of acting out their distress. The majority of responses in 

this category were directed at bullies: arguing, teasing, shouting, swearing, hitting back, punching, 

hurting, or humiliating the bully (“tell him he is ugly”, “laugh at his stupid face”). Certain 

responses to community violence were particularly aggressive in Brazil (“kill bad guys”, “kill 

[name of local drug lord]”), and Pakistan (by keeping weapons, although one young person 

stressed that they should be legal). 

Theme Two: Interpersonal Resources 

Three subthemes are reported here: social engagement, informal supports, and formal 

supports. 

Social engagement was similar to individual free-time activities in not targeting the 

stressor, whilst providing the young person with relief and comfort in the presence of others, 

usually peers. This strategy was predominantly used in response to feeling sad, and occasionally 

when faced with environmental stressors. Strategies included having “fun”; going on outings 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

103 

(friend’s house, “hanging out with friends”, park, playground); and participating in play or games, 

creative activities (drawing, dancing), or sports. Some participants defined strategies in relation to 

helping another young person (usually one who was feeling sad) by giving a gift, telling a story, 

or organizing a party. 

In contrast, seeking informal supports was directly related to the stressor. When feeling 

sad, friends were almost always the first contact point (“unburden”) to talk, share, or “hug”; and 

occasionally parents also filled this role. When parents argued with each other, grandparents were 

instead most commonly mentioned, usually for help and advice, although some young people used 

the words “complain” or “report”. The bully’s parents were commonly stated as the equivalent 

contact in the third scenario. In cases of community adversity, parents, community elders, 

neighbours, friends, and community experts (“rainmakers” in Kenya) would be equally likely to 

be consulted. Interestingly, young people in care did not specifically mention residential staff as 

sources of informal support. They mainly connected with their peers (“consider her problem 

ours”), occasionally their parents, and often non-specific others (“someone” or “adults”), but did 

not name them as caregivers. 

No external formal supports were suggested for the first scenario of feeling sad: these were 

proposed only in response to external stressors that could not be dealt with through other 

mechanisms. The boundaries between informal and formal supports were not clear for some 

sources, namely teachers, elders, and community or spiritual leaders. However, the context of the 

quotes largely indicated the next step up the help-seeking pathway by using words like “seek”, 

“report”, and “complain”. A range of school officials and professionals would be sought to address 

bullying, including a student prefect (a more formal role than peer), class teacher, duty teacher (if 

urgency were needed), deputy or head teacher, peer counsellor, and teacher with responsibility for 

guidance and counselling. Teachers were also viewed as a source of help for family conflict, and 

even as mediators to call the police (which was likely to indicate concerns for domestic violence). 

Community and religious leaders, and “the nearest authority” — usually the police — were 

commonly considered as available recourses in response to family conflict. Counsellors, 

psychologists, and couples therapists were also reported. Responses to community adversity 

involved the young people themselves seeking help from relevant agencies (fire service, 

ambulance, police, water or electricity company), or community elders to address the problem 

(“put the criminals away for the safety of our society”). Young people would directly approach or 

“complain to” the government, involve the media, and appeal to political leaders (this response 

came predominantly from the oldest age group in Kenya). Interestingly, young people in care 

nominated the electricity company as an external source of support, but no other agency. 

Theme Three: Religion 

Despite some overlap with cognitive reappraisal, religion was conceptualized as a distinct 

theme, with similarities across the two major religions of the participating countries (Islam and 

Christianity in two countries each). Religious strategies were evenly mentioned in response to the 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

104 

four scenarios. Praying was the most commonly used. Specific readings (Quran, Bible) or Islamic 

prayers like Naat, Namaz, and Dua, were considered. Some young people elaborated on the 

function of praying in relieving fear (“so the fear goes”, “to be away from fear”), as a way to “feel 

better” and help “forget about it”, and as a reminder of “stopping you from sins” (in response to 

parents arguing). In the case of external stressors, religion-related cognitions helped young 

participants to feel protected (bullying : “Allah see you and Allah punish you”), or to endure the 

adversity (family conflict: “we think and make ourselves feel good that fights are the deeds of 

devil, and God is not happy with it”; community violence: “if you’re gonna die, you’re gonna 

die”). Young people in Brazil used concepts from a different religion to their own to help them 

cope with bullying (“wait for Karma”, “everything that goes round, comes round”). Young people 

in Kenya prayed “that God bring rain” and “to God for water”. 

Overall, no gender differences were detected in the use of resilience-building strategies in 

response to the four scenarios. However. possible methodological explanations and constraints are 

considered below. 

Discussion 

In this study, we captured the voices of young people from four LMIC on how they defined 

resilience-building strategies in the face of different scenarios of adversity. The participants gave 

a range of responses and supporting examples that were themed under intrapersonal, interpersonal, 

and religious resources. The themes, subthemes, and categories that emerged were broadly 

consistent across the four cultural groups. Young people sought solutions from different ecological 

systems, although this also depended on the identified stressor. For example, they would 

predominantly rely on self-management such as cognitive reappraisal or emotional regulation in 

response to internalizing problems, but also seek support from family, peers, or elders. Conversely, 

when faced by external threats and aggression, they would largely access interpersonal resources 

in addition to seeking external supports, but were less likely to use intrapersonal resources. These 

findings thus add support to the dynamic linkage between resilience strategies across the ecological 

systems (Folke, 2016). This theory is not mutually exclusive with viewing resilience strategies as 

contextualized; that is, as taking place in a particular sociocultural context. Instead, these findings 

may indicate that broad resilience-building strategies are similar across cultural groups, but are 

specifically applied depending on the young person’s life circumstances. Theron (2018) thus 

proposed that an ecological systems approach to resilience that is also sensitive to contextual 

determinants would be a useful model to help young people adapt to apparently intractable risks. 

Several concepts could be used in conjunction. For example, young people often linked 

religious and cognitive responses. It is well established that religion is posited as a specific coping 

mechanism, with both positive and negative elements, but there are also conceptual differences 

between religion and cognitive reappraisal; hence we maintained religion as a distinct theme in the 

analysis (Pargament, 1997; Pargament et al., 2005). The function of certain strategies could be 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

105 

viewed according to the underpinning theoretical framework. For example, the type of response 

that we defined as “maladaptive” (mainly responding with verbal or physical aggression) could be 

classified under Ungar’s (2015) concept of “atypical resilience”. Atypical resilience refers to 

alternative forms of adaptation. While this may appear to be counter-indicative to resilience in 

certain contexts, it can also be functional in particular contexts of adversity (Ungar, 2011). 

Responses across all four sites suggest that, in addition to identifying external resources 

that they would draw on when faced with adversity, youth did not see themselves as passive 

recipients of help. Their agency was apparent in the active manner in which they considered each 

given scenario. For example, young people articulated solutions that involved them as actors 

instead of merely referring the problem to adults. This was evident across stressors within the 

family, the school, and the community. They similarly put forward agentic responses to helping 

their peers and other people when faced with natural or human-induced threats, as well as in regard 

to enhancing safety and natural resources in their communities. 

Emerging differences between contexts need to be acknowledged too. For example, young 

people in Turkey appeared to adopt more externalizing (fighting back) or internalizing (self-harm) 

responses to parental arguments and bullying than the other groups. In accordance with this 

finding, one study conducted in Turkey reported that expressing negative emotions such as 

swearing and shouting was a common coping strategy among adolescents (Öngen, 2002), although 

this result was not replicated in a more recent study (Şahin & Hepsöğütlü, 2018). In contrast, 

research in Turkey has been consistent in finding that adolescents commonly seek social supports 

(Altun & Özdemir, 2018; Kuyumcu et al., 2017; Öngen, 2002). 

The group of young people (all female) in care from Pakistan appeared to rely more on 

interpersonal and peer-related resources, but chose not to turn to their residential caregivers for 

help and did not know how to access external supports. These vulnerable young people did not 

consider individual activities, which may have been unavailable because of the structured 

residential regime. They did not use emotional regulation strategies either, which would be 

consistent with their multiple traumatic experiences, disrupted attachment relationships, and 

emotional dysregulation. Worryingly, they did not consider themselves as agents, which could 

reflect their submissive role within the care system. 

Limitations of the research include the relatively heterogenous sampling, and the data 

collection approach that prevented us from fully understanding either the process through which 

young people came up with their strategies or the context of their resources. Developing an 

approach that allows more time and the recording of interactions throughout the small group 

discussions should be a focus of future research. Sampling variables should also be addressed in 

more depth. For example, it was not possible to establish the extent of differences between the 

groups. Age variation was one factor, as the youngest age group from Brazil appeared to suggest 

more concrete or linear, instead of multilevel, responses; whilst the oldest age group in Kenya 

considered political protests in response to environmental concerns. However, thr age range of the 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

106 

sample was developmentally too wide to address this factor, as it varied from the onset to the end 

of puberty. These developmental stages can also be culturally-influenced (Sabageh et al., 2015), 

so future studies should explore children’s resilience-building strategies within more targeted age 

and culture groups. 

A similar limitation was the lack of in-depth consideration of gender-related issues, whilst 

acknowledging that the care home participants were all female Although no gender differences 

were detected from this dataset, the scenarios were possibly too broad to elicit gender-specific 

responses. Children’s first-line responses could have been followed up by further questioning to 

understand whether girls and boys used similar or different strategies in response to the same 

stressor, such as family conflict or school bullying; the participants could also have been asked to 

formulate gender-specific scenarios (Rizvi et al., 2014). Finally, as both traditional and Western 

influences were detected in the data, it would also be interesting to explore young people’s own 

explanations of the impact of these influences. 

Conclusion 

Several implications for practice and services tentatively arise from these findings. Active 

participation from young people, indeed from children, is paramount in influencing decisions on 

their care (United Nations, 2014). Several studies have shown that, when engaged and involved in 

a developmentally appropriate and sensitive way, even young children can formulate 

recommendations based on their experiences of psychosocial interventions and services (Bone et 

al., 2015). In this study, young people responded positively to the participatory research method 

of participants negotiating their answers in small groups rather than having individual interviews. 

The finding that resilience strategies for the same stressor included both informal and 

formal sources of support indicates the importance of establishing pathways and networks within 

and across families, communities, and services for young people to receive prompt and 

comprehensive help. It is recognized that multimodal interventions and scaled service provision 

are particularly appropriate for young people with complex needs (Hussein et al., 2012; O’Connor 

et al., 2018; Reed et al., 2011) living in LMIC without access to substantive specialist resources 

(Vostanis, 2017, 2018; World Health Organization, 2008). Such a service model is consistent with 

both the ecological systems framework and the multitude of views expressed by young people in 

this study. 

Acknowledgements 

We are grateful to all the young people who took part, and their teachers and parents or 

caregivers for facilitating contact with them. We thank the organizations that hosted this study as 

gatekeepers: Friendly Action Network in Kenya; Associação pela Saúde Emocional de Crianças 

(ASEC) in Brazil; and Hussaini Foundation in Pakistan. 



International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

107 

References 

Altun, Z. D., & Özdemir, M. (2018). The role of music in coping with trauma 

experiences. European Journal of Education Studies, 4, 289–302. 

doi:10.5281/zenodo.1328275 

Bone, C., O’Reilly, M., Karim, K., & Vostanis, P. (2015). “They’re not witches…” Young 

children and their parents’ perceptions and experiences of child and adolescent mental 

health services. Child: Care, Health and Development, 15, 450–458. doi:10.1111/cch.12161 

Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in 

Psychology, 3, 77–101. doi:10.1191/1478088706qp063oa 

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and 

design. Harvard University Press. 

Burr, V. (2003). Social constructionism (2nd ed). Routledge. 

Folke, C. (2016) Resilience (Republished). Ecology and Society, 21(4), 44. doi:10.5751/ES-

09088-210444 

Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience in children’s adaptation to negative life events and stressed 

environments. Pediatric Annals, 20, 459–466. doi:10.3928/0090-4481-19910901-05 

Getanda, E., Vostanis, P., & O’Reilly, M. (2017). Exploring the challenges of meeting child 

mental health needs through community engagement in Kenya. Child and Adolescent 

Mental Health, 22, 201–208. doi:10.1111/camh.12233 

Huang, C. Y. (2018). Resilience in Taiwan: The shaping forces of its diverse cultural contexts 

and beliefs about adversity. In G. J. Rich, S. Sirikantraporn, (Eds.), Human strengths and 

resilience: Developmental, cross-cultural, and international perspectives (pp. 75–86). 

Lexington. 

Hussein, S. A., Vostanis, P., & Bankart, J. (2012). Social and educational risk factors for child 

mental health problems in Karachi, Pakistan. International Journal of Mental Health and 

Culture, 5, 1–14. doi:10.1080/17542863.2010.538187 

Johnson, V., Nurick, R., Baker, K., & Shivakotee, R. (2013). Children and young people’s 

participation (CYPP) training workshop guide. Child Hope. 

Kidd, S., & Kral, M. (2005). Practicing participatory action research. Journal of Counselling 

Psychology, 52, 187–195. 

https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1328275
https://doi.org/10.1111/cch.12161
https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
https://doi.org/10.5751/ES-09088-210444
https://doi.org/10.5751/ES-09088-210444
https://doi.org/10.3928/0090-4481-19910901-05
https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12233
https://doi.org/10.1080/17542863.2010.538187


International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

108 

Kieling, C., Baker-Henningham, H., Belfer, M., Conti, G., Ertem, I., Omigbodun, O., Agusto-

Rohde, L., Srinath, S., Ulkuer, N., & Rahman, A. (2011). Child and adolescent mental 

health worldwide. Lancet, 378, 1515–1525. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60827-1 

King, N. (2004). Using templates in the thematic analysis of text. In C. Cassell, & G. Symon 

(Eds.), Essential guide to qualitative methods in organisational research (pp. 256–270). 

Sage. doi:10.4135/9781446280119.n21 

Kuyumcu, B., Yildiz, H., & Böke, Z. (2017). Lise öğrencilerinin stresle başa çıkma tarzları ve 

algılanan öğretmen reddinin disiplin durumlarına dayalı olarak incelenmesi [Investigation 

of high school students coping with stress and perceived teacher rejection based on 

disciplinary situations]. Yüzüncü Yıl Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi [Yüzüncü Yıl 

University Journal of Education], 14, 1289–1318. doi:10.23891/efdyyu.2017.46 

Libório, R. M. C., & Ungar, M. (2014). Resilience as protagonism: Interpersonal relationships, 

cultural practices, and personal agency among working adolescents in Brazil. Journal of 

Youth Studies, 17, 682–696. doi:10.1080/13676261.2013.834313 

Maltby, J., Day, L., Zemojtel-Piotrowska, M., Piotrowski, J., Hitokoko, H., Baran, T., Jones, C., 

Chakravarty-Agbo, A., & Flowe, H. D. (2016). An ecological systems model of trait 

resilience: Cross-cultural and clinical relevance. Personality and Individividual Differences 

doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.100 

Masten, A. (2006). Promoting resilience in development: A general system for framework of 

care. In R. Flynn, P. M. Dudding, & J. G. Barber (Eds.), Promoting resilience in child 

welfare (pp. 3–17). University of Ottawa Press. 

Masten, A. S., & Wright, M. O. (2010). Resilience over the lifespan: Developmental 

perspectives on resistance, recovery and transformation. In J. W. Reich, A. J. Zautra, & J. S. 

Hall (Eds.), Handbook of adult resilience (pp. 213–237). Guilford Press. 

O’Connor, C., Dyson, J., Cowdell, F., & Watson, R. (2018). Do universal school-based mental 

health promotion programmes improve the mental health and emotional well-being of 

young people? A literature review. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 27, e412–e426. 

doi:10.1111/jocn.14078 

Öngen, D. (2002). Ergenlerde sorunlarla başa çıkma davranışları [Coping behaviors of 

adolescents]. Eğitim ve Bilim [Education and Science], 27, 54–61. 

Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2016). Country risk classification. 

Retrieved April, 2019 from http://www.oecd.org/tad/xcred/crc.htm 

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60827-1
https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446280119.n21
https://doi.org/10.23891/efdyyu.2017.46
https://doi.org/10.1080/13676261.2013.834313
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.100
https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.14078
http://www.oecd.org/tad/xcred/crc.htm


International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

109 

Panter-Brick, C. (2015). Culture and resilience: Next steps for theory and practice. In L. C. 

Theron, L. Liebenberg & M. Ungar (Eds.), Youth resilience and culture: Commonalities 

and complexities (pp. 233–244). Springer. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-9415-2_17 

Pargament, K. I. (1997). The psychology of religion and coping: Theory, research, practice. 

Guilford. 

Pargament, K. I., Ano, G. G., Falb, M. D., & Wachholtz, A. (2005). The religious dimension of 

coping: Advances in theory, research, and practice. In R. F. Paloutzian and C. L. Park  

(Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. 498–609). Guilford. 

Patel, V., & Rahman, A. (2015). Editorial commentary: An agenda for global child mental 

health. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 20, 3–4. doi:10.1111/camh.12083 

Reason, P. (1994). Human inquiry as discipline and practice. In P. Reason (Ed.), Participation in 

human inquiry (pp. 40–56). Sage. 

Reed, R. V., Fazel, M., Jones, L., Panter-Brick, C., & Stein, A. (2011). Mental health of 

displaced and refugee children resettled in low-income and middle-income countries: Risk 

and protective factors. The Lancet, 379, 250–265. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60050-0 

Rhodes, S. D., Song, E., Nam, S., Choi, S. J., & Choi, S. (2015). Identifying and intervening on 

barriers of healthcare access among members of a small community in the southern USA. 

Patient Education and Counselling, 98, 484–491. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2015.01.001 

Rizvi, N., Khan, S. K., & Shaikh, B. T. (2014). Gender: Shaping personality, lives and health of 

women in Pakistan. BMC Women’s Health, 14(1). doi:10.1186/1472-6874-14-53 

Rutter, M. (2006). Implications of resilience concepts for scientific understanding. Annals of the 

New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 1–12. doi:10.1196/annals.1376.002 

Sabageh, A. O., Sabageh, D., Adeoye, O. A., & Adeomi, A. A. (2015). Pubertal timing and 

demographic predictors of adolescents in southwest Nigeria. Journal of Clinical and 

Diagnostic Research, 9(8), LC11–LC13. doi:10.7860/JCDR/2015/13582.6349 

Sahar, N., & Muzaffar, N. (2017). Role of family systems, positive emotions and resilience in 

social adjustment among Pakistani adolescents. Journal of Educational, Health and 

Community Psychology, 6,(2), 46–58. doi:10.12928/jehcp.v6i2.6944 

Şahin, S., & Hepsöğütlü, Z. B. (2018). High school students’ psychological resilience levels and 

coping strategies. European Journal of Education Studies, 5, 33–44. 

doi:10.5281/zenodo.1461008 

https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9415-2_17
https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12083
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60050-0
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2015.01.001
https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6874-14-53
https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1376.002
https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2015/13582.6349
https://doi.org/10.12928/jehcp.v6i2.6944
https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1461008


International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

110 

Sirikantraporn, S., Rich, G. R., & Badaracco, J. C. (2018). The concept of posttraumatic growth 

in a sample of undergraduates from India: A mixed-methods study. In G.J . Rich, & S. 

Sirikantraporn, (Eds.), Human strengths and resilience:Developmental, cross-cultural, and 

international perspectives (pp. 1–20). Lexington. 

Tamburino, I., Getanda, E., O’Reilly, M., & Vostanis, P. (2018). “Everybody’s responsibility”: 

Conceptualisation of youth mental health in Kenya. Journal of Child Health Care. Advance 

online publication. doi:10.1177/1367493518814918 

Theron, L. (2018). Championing the resilience of sub-Saharan adolescents: Pointers for 

psychologists. South African Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 325–336. 

doi:10.1177/0081246318801749 

Theron, L., & Phasha, N. (2015). Cultural pathways to resilience: Opportunities and obstacles as 

recalled by Black South African students. In L. C. Theron, L. Liebenberg, & M. Ungar 

(Eds.), Youth resilience and culture: Commonalities and complexities (pp. 51–65). Springer. 

doi:10.1007/978-94-017-9415-2_4 

Theron, L. C., & Theron, A. (2013). Positive adjustment to poverty: How family communities 

encourage resilience in traditional African contexts. Culture and Psychology, 19, 391–413. 

doi:10.1177/1354067X13489318 

Theron, L., & van Rensburg, A. (2018). Resilience over time: Learning from school-attending 

adolescents living in conditions of structural inequality. Journal of Adolescence, 67, 167–

178. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.06.012 

Ungar, M. (2004). A constructionist discourse on resilience: Multiple contexts, multiple realities 

among at-risk children and youth. Youth Society, 35, 341–365. 

doi:10.1177/0044118X03257030 

Ungar, M. (2011). The social ecology of resilience: Addressing contextual and cultural 

ambiguity of a nascent construct. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 81, 1–17. 

doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01067.x 

Ungar, M. (2012). Social ecologies and their contribution to resilience. In M. Ungar (Ed.), The 

social ecology of resilience: A handbook of theory and practice (pp. 13–31). Springer. 

doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-0586-3_2 

Ungar, M. (2015). Resilience and culture: The diversity of protective processes and positive 

adaptation. In L. C. Theron, L. Liebenberg & M. Ungar (Eds.), Youth resilience and 

culture: Commonalities and complexities (pp. 37–50). Springer. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-

9415-2_3 

https://doi.org/10.1177/1367493518814918
https://doi.org/10.1177/0081246318801749
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9415-2_4
https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X13489318
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.06.012
https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X03257030
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01067.x
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-0586-3_2
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9415-2_3
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9415-2_3


International Journal of Child, Youth and Family Studies (2020) 11(1): 92–111 

111 

United Nations. (2014). Mental health matters: Social inclusion of youth with mental health 

conditions. 

https://www.un.org/esa/socdev/documents/youth/IYD2014/reportsocialinclusionofyouthwit

hmentalhealth.pdf 

United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2013). Operational guidance: Mental health 

and psychosocial support. https://www.unhcr.org/protection/health/525f94479/operational-

guidance-mental-health-psychosocial-support-programming-refugee.html  

van Breda, A. D., & Theron, L. C. (2018). A critical review of South African child and youth 

resilience studies, 2009–2017. Children and Youth Services Review, 91, 237–247. 

doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.06.022 

Vostanis, P. (2017). Global child mental health: Emerging challenges and opportunities. Child 

and Adolescent Mental Health, 22, 177–178. doi:10.1111/camh.12246 

Vostanis, P. (2018). Hope for children of trauma: An international perspective. Routledge. 

Vostanis, P. (2019). World awareness for children in trauma: Capacity-building activities of a 

psychosocial program. International Journal of Mental Health. 

doi:10.1080/00207411.2019.1602019 

Vostanis, P., Maltby, J., Duncan, C., & O’Reilly, M. (2018). Stakeholder perspectives on 

children’s psychosocial needs in six low- and middle-income countries. Children and 

Society, 32, 457–469. 

Vostanis, P., O’Reilly, M., Duncan, C., Maltby, J., & Anderson, E. (2018). Interprofessional 

training on resilience-building for children who experience trauma: Stakeholders’ views 

from six low- and middle-income countries. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 33(2), 143–

152. doi:10.1080/13561820.2018.1538106 

Werner, E. E. (1989). High-risk children in young adulthood: A longitudinal study from birth to 

32 years. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 72–81. doi:10.1111/j.1939-

0025.1989.tb01636.x 

Willow, C. (2013) Involved by right: Effective participation of children and young people in 

alternative care settings. World Awareness for Children in Trauma (WACIT). 

www.wacit.org [Accessed April, 2019] 

World Health Organization. (2008). Mental Health Action Gap Programme: Scaling up care for 

mental, neurological and substance use disorders. 

https://www.who.int/mental_health/mhgap_final_english.pdf 

https://www.un.org/esa/socdev/documents/youth/IYD2014/reportsocialinclusionofyouthwithmentalhealth.pdf
https://www.un.org/esa/socdev/documents/youth/IYD2014/reportsocialinclusionofyouthwithmentalhealth.pdf
https://www.unhcr.org/protection/health/525f94479/operational-guidance-mental-health-psychosocial-support-programming-refugee.html
https://www.unhcr.org/protection/health/525f94479/operational-guidance-mental-health-psychosocial-support-programming-refugee.html
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.06.022
https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12246
https://doi.org/10.1080/00207411.2019.1602019
https://doi.org/10.1080/13561820.2018.1538106
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1989.tb01636.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1989.tb01636.x
http://www.wacit.org/
https://www.who.int/mental_health/mhgap_final_english.pdf