Microsoft Word - 2 (18-15) C - Balau


      19 

 
 

 
THE LIMITED CONSUMER RATIONALITY 

AND THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENTAL CUES 
 

Mădălina Bălău 
Danubius University of Galați,  Romania 

 
 

  
Received: July 13, 2018           Accepted: October 7, 2018            Online Published: October 15, 2018             
 
 
 

Abstract 
 
The ideal of rational consumer rests on the assumption that individuals already hold perfect 
information and consumer behavior results from the preferences based on this information. 
Today, consumers enjoy access to more information than ever before, but, on the other hand, 
they frequently reach decisions with limited deliberation. With its flow of information, does the 
age of the Internet encourage the ideal of consumer rationality? Our aim is to explore the role 
of social influences and environmental cues on consumer online behavior and to derive 
implications for improving our understanding of online behaviors. Research findings on 
influence tactics highlight an increasing importance of social cues in assessing online 
information, while experiments in social psychology reveal the role of unconscious processes 
in automatically activating attitudes and behaviors. We discuss implications for developing our 
understanding of online consumer behavior and advancements necessary for consumer 
research. 
 
Keywords: Consumer rationality; Online behavior; Social proof; Influence tactics; Consumer 
decision-making; Limited deliberation. 

1. Introduction 
Decision making and consumer behavior are thought, traditionally, to result from individual 
information processing that leads to certain attitudes influencing subsequent behavior (Ajzen 
& Fishbein, 1980). Consumers will choose a certain product or service after comparing the 
costs and benefits of each alternative. This more elaborated information processing takes place 
for expensive products, with important long-term consequences for the individual (Chaiken, 
1980; Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983). However, more frequently consumers do not 
engage in thorough information processing before making decisions, such as when they make 
daily and repeated decisions, when they decide on impulse or when they act without obvious 
consequences.  

Many online behavioral decisions result from a limited analysis of costs and benefits, since 
they are only a click away and their consequences seem to be inexistent or inexpensive for the 
consumer. This is the case for behaviors such as game and applications downloading, 
subscribing to various online channels or reacting to online content. Nonetheless, these 
behaviors have an increasingly important impact, at aggregate level, determining company 

International Journal of Economic Behavior, vol. 8, n. 1, 2018, 19-31. 



20 

 

investments and having macroeconomic consequences. At individual level, online behaviors 
also impact the consumers’ life by changing their habits and their spending choices in terms of 
money and time, yet these consequences remain mostly invisible to consumers. Thus, online 
behavior is a special kind of consumer behavior characterized by fast decision-making with few 
immediate individual consequences, but with an important aggregate impact. 

Although the ideal of consumer rationality has long been criticized (Scott, 2000, Simon, 
1957) or given various interpretations and developments (Lancaster, 1966; Samuelson, 1938), 
the current context of online consumer behavior challenges this view even more. The rational 
consumer is considered to access and process all information needed, to make a decision 
without social influences (or at best they are internalized as stable set of beliefs on social norms) 
and to hold stable preferences, which are not influenced by context (Jackson, 2005). However, 
today consumers have access to more information than ever before in history, they are 
demanding for e-commerce platforms that embed social interactions and they still exhibit 
impulse buying behaviors. 

The online consumer behavior brings forefront some aspects that were ignored in the past: 
the social influence and the context’s impact. Also the Internet facilitates access to information, 
thus possibly driving consumers closer to that ideal of perfect information. So, what the main 
developments with respect to the ideal of consumer rationality that are facilitated by consumers’ 
use of the Internet in their decisions? This paper aims to explore the role of social influences 
and environmental cues on consumer online behavior and to derive implications for moving 
forward the understanding of consumer behavior in online settings.  

 Multiple tactics are aimed at influencing consumer behavior to make decisions faster and 
thus with reduced information processing. Many of these tactics use various social cues that 
determine impulsive behaviors, but consumers may become aware of them and develop 
defensive strategies, if they wish to do so (Cialdini, 2001). Other times consumers seem to make 
decisions unconsciously, without a deliberate information processing. Dijksterhuis, Smith, van 
Baaren and Wigboldus. (2005) illustrate how consumers simply bypass the deliberation process 
and, instead, are influenced by environmental cues. These may consist of elements related to 
the structure of choice, such as the website features, or may be driven by social cues with direct 
influence on behavior. 

This article is divided into three parts. First, we present an overview of the mechanisms 
that explain consumer decision-making with limited deliberation. We identified two types of 
influence mechanisms: those of which consumers may become aware of and those that function 
only at an unconscious level. Secondly, we summarize research findings on how the influence 
mechanisms impact consumers’ online behavior and how social validation influences their 
assessment of information credibility. Finally, we derive implications for the ideal of consumer 
rationality in online decision-making context and we propose future research directions for 
improving consumer behavior models. 

2. Mechanisms of Individual Decision-Making with Limited Deliberation 
Consumer choice with limited deliberation is explained through various mechanisms and 
mediating concepts, such as habits, the automatic attitude-behavior link, impulse decisions, a 
multitude of social influence tactics and a direct perception-behavior link (Cialdini, 2001; 
Dijksterhuis, Smith, van Baaren, & Wigboldus, 2005; Dittmar, 2008; Fazio & Olson, 2014; 
Ouellette & Wood, 1998;).  

Frequently-made decisions are usually explained by the concept of habit, which is a 
repeated behavior that was the result of a previous information processing stage. The habit 
produces satisfying consequences to the consumer and therefore the behavior that is simply 
replicated in the future and acquires certain automaticity. Ouellette and Wood (1998) defined 



      21 

 
 

habits as behavioral tendencies towards repeating a certain action when the context is stable, 
thus being highly dependent external cues. From a learning process perspective, habits are 
described as a mental mechanism that enables us to free up cognitive capacity for the more 
difficult tasks, while the habitual ones are performed automatically (Maslow, 1954).  

Although, the proponents of consumer behavior models based on the expectancy-value 
theory consider habit as already included in the attitudes towards a behavior (Ajzen & Fishbein, 
1980; Ajzen I., 1985; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975), habitual behaviors often bypass the deliberation 
process. Due to its automatic action initiation mechanism, habit is worth considering in addition 
to intention and attitudes towards a behavior, both for future behavior prediction and for 
behavioral change.  

In their meta-analysis, Ouellette and Wood (1998) tested whether past behavior and 
intention predicted differently subsequent behavior depending on the situation’s characteristics 
in terms of opportunity and stability. Their findings confirm that habit predicts the behavior 
better in stable and favorable contexts, while for shifting contexts, intention performs better as 
predictor of future behavior. Thus, since habits and intentions determine subsequent behavior 
differently depending on the choice context, the environmental cues can trigger habit activation 
in consumers.  

Another mechanism explaining the decision-making with limited deliberation is the 
automatic attitude-behavior link, which is explored by Fazio (1990) in the MODE model. 
Contrary to the popular idea that attitudes determine behavior through the mediation of 
intention, Fazio (1990) proposes that attitudes and behavior are directly linked. This automatic 
attitude-behavior mechanism is used in spontaneous decision-making, a frequently encountered 
type of decision-making. Fazio and Olson (2014) note that the deliberative decision-making 
process, which involves the analysis of costs and benefits is rooted in the ideal of the rational 
consumer, is far less used by individuals, only when they have the opportunity and motivation 
to do so.  

Fazio and Olson (2014) posit that the external stimulus automatically activate consumer 
attitudes, which then trigger a subsequent behavior directly. Thus, consumers do not hold a 
fixed set of attitudes towards behaviors as if they would hold a box of tools and as the Theory 
of Planned Behavior suggests (Ajzen, 1985). Rather, individuals hold an immense variety of 
attitudes that become salient only when triggered by cues in the environment.  

An alternative justification for consumer decision-making with limited information 
processing is that sometimes consumers make impulse decisions and attitudes are completely 
bypassed, thus their cognitive processing. An impulse decision of particular interest in 
consumer behavior is the impulse buying decision, which has three main characteristics: it takes 
little deliberation and planning, it involves high emotional feelings from consumers and it 
makes individuals completely neglect the constraints and consequences of that decision 
(Dittmar & Drury, 2000). Thus, the impulse buying decision is a situation where consumer 
passion overtakes deliberation. Dittmar (2008) suggests that impulse buying decisions occur 
mostly for goods that are symbolically linked to that individual’s self-concept. 

Cialdini (2001) also explored the mechanisms that drive individuals to impulse decisions 
and to accept offers. Instead of looking at a symbolic link with goods, Cialdini highlights a 
series of social influence tactics that lead to impulsive reactions. He identifies six fundamental 
psychological principles behind them: the priciples of scarcity, reciprocity, liking, consistency, 
authority and social proof. These principles are often recognized as the basic elements of 
marketing tools aimed at influencing consumer behavior. Their main characteristics are 
highlighted below: 

1. Scarcity: Individuals value more the offers in short supply and react faster to those which 
highlight an advantage they may lose.  



22 

 

2. Reciprocity: Individuals are inclined to pay back for the favors received, due to a feeling 
of indebtedness towards the person who offered the “gift” first. 

3. Liking: Individuals allow others they like to influence or persuade them. This functions 
mainly through similarity to others or through being praised by others. 

4. Consistency: Individuals will try to stick to their previously reported opinions or 
behaviors. 

5. Authority: Individiuals give in to persuasive messages from experts. Expert opinions 
make valuable and efficient short-cuts for good enough decisions. 

6. Social proof: Individuals follow the behavior of others, especially in ambiguous 
contexts. 

 
These principles of influence have been frequently used in various contexts of social 

interaction. Initially, the sales context was the place where these principles were discovered, 
yet they are ment to function in other social interaction settings as well. Cialdini (2001 b) 
recommended managers to use them in order to persuade colleagues and subordinates to change 
behavior and he highlighted their potential to increase the results of fund-raising initiatives 
through a combination of these principles (Cialdini, 2003).  

Finally, the research on the direct link between perception and behavior illustrates 
situations when behavior is triggered unconsciously for the consumer. Dijksterhuis Smith, van 
Baaren & Wigboldus (2005) review the findings on this perception-behavior link and conclude 
that environmental cues play a major role in influencing subsequent behavior through their mere 
perception. These cues impact directly the behavior, at an unconscious level, by inducing 
certain behaviors or goals on the subjects. In many instances, social perception is involved in 
triggering the automatic behavior, as it activates a representation in the individuals mind with 
direct effect on the social behavior (Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001). 

Imitation represents a direct consequence of the perception-behavior link. Dijksterhuis and 
Bargh (2001) investigated the role played by imitation on the decision to enact a behavior and 
they identified two ways in which imitation intervenes: the low and high road to imitation. The 
low road relates to a simple imitation of the observable behavior, such as when people mimic 
gestiures, facial expressions and speech characteristics. The high road implies a more complex 
imitation effect, when people try to imitate traits, goal and stereotypes of important others.  

Automatic mimicry is recognized as an innate human ability, which plays a role in the 
learning from others’ behavior. The automatic mimicry is enabled by mirror neurons, as the 
findings on their functioning reveal (Decety & Grezes, 1999). Iacoboni (2005) found that 
whenever individuals practice or observe an action the same areas in the brain get activated. 
Thus, if the same brain’s regions are involved in coding own goals and intentions as well as 
those of others, then imitation provides also a means of understanding others’ actions. Yet, even 
though imitation enables individuals to understand others, it also provides an automatic 
behavioral response that the person is not aware of.  

Johnston (2002) tested the influence of mimicry on behavior in an experimental study and 
found a statistically significant effect on behavior. In the experiment, subjects were asked to eat 
ice cream in the presence of another person, who was instructed to eat a small or a large sample 
according to the assigned experimental group. The subjects in the experimental condition 
mimicked the behavior of their peer. 

Chartrand and Bargh (1999) tested experimentally whether mimicry impacts the sympathy 
felt by a peer and found that subjects who were imitated in the experiment liked more their peer 
than those who were not imitated by their partner. Van Baaren, Holland, Kawakami and van 
Knippenberg (2004) found that mimicry also encourages pro-social behavior; in their studies, 
participants were more helpful and generous if they had been mimicked and they directed these 



      23 

 
 

behaviors not only towards the mimicker but also to others, outside the strict context of 
mimicking.  

Several studies on the effects of priming on subsequent behavior indicated a significant 
priming effect through the activation of goals, traits of personality and stereotypes in the mind 
of the participants. Carver, Ganellen, Froming, and Chambers (1983) primed subiects with the 
concept of hostility by exposing participants to an aggressive model and they were influenced 
by displaying a more aggresssive behavior subsequently or by interpreting others’ behaviors as 
more aggressive. Bargh, Chen, and Burrows (1996) tested experimentally the effect of priming 
participants with the concept of rudeness and with certain stereotypes, an elderly stereotype and 
an African American stereotype, and in all cases they observed an automatic bevior inducing 
effect. Dijksterhuis and van Knippenberg (1996) studied as well the effects of stereotype 
activation on the cognitive performance in tests or on the speed of their reactions. In the 
experiments, they asked different groups of participants to read descriptions of older people, 
teachers and hooligans and the priming procedure facilitated or inhibited their performance in 
the tests applied. 

The direct link of perception with automatic behavior activation illustrates that the 
influences on decision-making are subtle and bear an unconscious dimension. Individuals are 
more sensitive to social influences than posit popular consumer behavior models, such as the 
Theory of Planned Behavior of Ajzen (1985). Additionally, social perception has the biggest 
impact on subsequent behavior, as individuals react more frequently towards others’ behaviors 
and less frequently to environmental cues outside the human context. Thus, individuals are even 
more sensitive to the social influences than would suggest Cialdini (2001). The social influence 
tactics he identified are a part of the determinants of behavior that could be observed once the 
consumer becomes aware. However, the direct link between perception and behavior inform us 
that this relationship resembles more a reflex reaction, without any cognitive processing. 

 
3. Influences on Consumer Behavior in Online Social Contexts 
In June 2018 there were over 4 billion Internet users, most of them being located in Asia, 49%, 
followed by Europe, with 16,8% users, and Africa with 11% users of the total number of 
Internet users worldwide (Internet Usage Statistics, 2018). However, the highest penetration 
rate can be found in North America, where 95% of the population uses the Internet, followed 
by Europe with a 85,2% penetration rate. 

The increasing use of the Internet and the intensified online interactions made it an 
interesting plaform for various attempts to influence others. Fogg (2003, p.1) recognized the 
importance of technology in attampts to change consumer attitudes and behaviors and termed 
this class of technologies ‘Persuasive Technology’. Besides being a topic of interest, the online 
social interaction is also charaterized by some particular features that enable the users more 
control over the timing and location of the interaction, the degree of anonimity, the physical 
appearance and the physical distance (Guadagno, Muscanell, Rice, & Roberts, 2013). These 
charasteristics have an impact over the functioning and the effectiveness of the persuasion 
strategies.  

Guadagno and Cialdini (2005) reviewed the influence of the authority and consistency  
principles of persuasion in online contexts and found the principles have different degrees of 
influence compared to the face-to-face interaction. The authority principle in online context has 
encountered a higher compliance to the influence agent, but the influence was diminished 
compared to an interactive context. The consistency principle, however, seemed to be as 
effective in computer-mediated contexts as in other communication means. In following article, 
Guagagno, Muscanell, Rice, & Roberts. (2013) examined how effective are the principles of 
liking and social proof in online influence. The higher the social validation, defined as the 



24 

 

number of individuals agreeing to a online request, the more influence it exerted on individuals 
to agree to the request. However, the likability of the blogger, although it was noticed, didn’t 
influence participants to comply to the request.  

Orji, Mandryk, & Vassileva (2015) tested the impact of Cialdini’s persuasion principales 
in online context and explored the difference in compliance by gender and age. In general, the 
most effective persuasive strategies were consistency and reciprocity. However, females were 
persuaded easier with the principles of reciprocity, consistency and social proof than men, 
suggesting that peer presure impacts more this group. With respect to age, younger adults 
responded more to scarcity, while adults were persuaded more by the consistency principle. 

The social interaction in the online environment is an essential feature that has contributed 
to important changes in the e-commerce activity. This type of online commerce, which is 
termed 'social commerce', incorpoates tools and interfaces that facilitate social interaction and 
help leveraging sales (Huang & Benyoucef, 2013). According to Dennison, Bourdage-Braun, 
& Chetuparambil (2009), implementing social commerce means including the word-of-mouth 
into the e-commerce activity, but the concept also refers to the use of social media applications 
in the interaction with consumers. All these developements impact the marketplace and the 
ways businesses interact with their public, transforming it into a user-driven one (Wigand, 
Benjamin, & Birkland, 2008). 

As we discussed above, impuse behavior is one of the examples when consumers make 
decisions with limited or without cognitive deliberation. Impulse online buying behavior is an 
important phenomenon to study since it is a type of behavior frequently observed in retail sales 
(Hausman, 2000) as well as in online contexts (Li, Kuo, & Russell, 1999). Wells, Parboteeah, 
& Valacich. (2011) conducted two experiments in order to test the impact of online environment 
cues and consumer impulsiveness on impluse buying behaviors. They tested the website quality 
as a determining environmental cue and they found it has a significat role in influencing online 
impulse buying. The website quality was operationalized as a perceptual value with three 
dimensions: the security, the navigability and the visual appearance perceptions of the website. 
They conclude that relevant environmental cue (i.e. a high website quality) stimulates consumer 
impulsiveness and induces impulse buying behaviors, while less relevant cues (i.e. lower 
quality websites) negatively influence impulse buying.  

Amblee & Bui (2011) investigated the effect that online word-of-mouth has on sales and 
on the brand and product reputation. They studied the consumers’ information exchange, 
recommendations, shared thoughts and conversations about books and author quality on 
Amazon.com. Their findings revealed that the amount of shared online information about a 
products doesn’t only help users make evaluations and decisions, but it also directly influces 
the products’s sales performance. The more the product reviews are available, the more product 
sales are generated, and the rating score is less associated with sales as long as the number of 
reviews is smaller. In other words, the more an item is reviewed the better sales it enjoys, no 
matter the review score.  

Consumers interaction with the online content and their usage of online information in 
decision-making also raise the question of consumers’capability to evaluate credibility of online 
information. Individuals have always evaluated the credibility of information through social 
means and not in isolation  (Metzger, Flanagin, & Medders, 2010). Traditionally, the credibility 
of information is assessed by referring to some recognized institution or expert that provides 
the reliable information. This was a good enough solution when people lived in a world where 
the information was scarce. However, as Callister (2000) argues, the current context of 
abundent online information makes this convention for evaluating information credibility 
insufficient for the fast pace in which consumers need to make choices. The online environment 



      25 

 
 

allows individuals to access large amounts of information from a wide variety of sources, so 
they also have a new need: to assess credibility while saving cognitive effort and time.  

Since individuals need to cope with a problem of information overload and uncertainty in 
the online environments (Sundar, 2008; Taraborelli, 2008), they resort to several heuristic 
strategies. Metzger, Flanagin, & Medders (2010) explored how individuals use such strategies 
to  assess information credibility online, so that they can reach a decision fast enough and 
without a high cognitive effort. One dominant characteristic identified is the social arbitration, 
which means that individuals use online social networking, online assessments and online 
reputation systems to judge the information and source credibility. 

In their study, Metzger, Flanagin, & Medders (2010) identified five heuristics used by 
individuals in online contexts: reputation, endorsement, consistency, expectancy violation and 
persuasive intent (p. 425). These heuristics fall in two major categories, namely heuristics based 
on social confirmation and heuristics rooted in expectations from the context: 

 The reputation heuristic – information is credible when it is published on well-known 
websites. 

 The endorsement heuristic – information is credible if others consider it as well (by 
sharing, liking, etc.) without further evaluation of website content or information source.  

 The consistency heuristic – information is credible when individuals can cross-validate 
it through several information sources; this heuristic requires more cognitive effort from 
participant.  

 The expectancy violation heuristic – information is credible if the website meets 
readers’ expectations in terms of appearance, layout, features, functionality and 
comprehensiveness; online sources which fail to meet the reader’s criteria in terms of 
layout in considered less credible.  

 The persuasive intent heuristic – information that is perceived as advertising, 
commercial or persuasive is generally considered not credible. 

 
The main observation of Metzger, Flanagin, & Medders (2010) study is that individuals 

resort to distant information sources that they consider relevant through social evaluation 
mechanisms. This means that relevance and credibility is assessed through social information 
pooling and priviledging personal opinion confirmation, passionate recommendations and 
resources shared by familiar others. Thus, the notion of ‘social proof’ proposed by Cialdini 
(2001) supports the reputaion, endorsement and consistency heuristics, and therefore make 
individuals efficiently evaluate credibility (i.e. they reach a decision on the information at hand 
faster and with less cognitive effort)  but are subject to manipulation influences. Credibility 
assessment through social proof can be erroneous since it is based on crowd behavior and it 
equates popularity with credibility. In addition, individiuals may reject information as not 
credible if it disconfirms their personal opinion or expectations, leading thus to a narrowing of 
information avaliable to the individual though his/her own choice to ignore competeing 
opinions. 

The online visibility of individuals is subject to similar credibility evaluations and the 
social network of the individual influences the importance of his/her online presence. Stoica, 
Riederer & Chaintreau (2018) observe that the online visibility of individuals, which depends 
on their social network, grows mainly with the help of the referral systems embedded in social 
media platforms. Thus, the y tested whether the social recommendation algorithms affect the 
representation opportunities of different demographic groups, and especially if there are 
significant gender gaps enhanced or diminished by these algorithms. 

Indeed, Stoica Riederer & Chaintreau (2018) documented a reinforcement of the 
representation gender gap through the referral function in Instagram’s recommendation 



26 

 

algorithm. They describe the effect observed as ‘an algorithmic glass ceiling’ for women that 
can be explained by the phenomenon of ‘differentiated homophily’ (Stoica, Riederer, & 
Chaintreau, 2018, p. 2). This phenomenon implies that individuals favor interactions with 
similar others, so in this case men support more other men, while women don’t favor other 
women. The algorithm does not create this type of behavior, so it might be considered legitimate 
as it only reflects a pre-existing behavior in the human society. However, combining this finding 
with those related to the limits of online information processing, the effects on limiting 
representation may increase with the information volume and create unwanted social effects. 
Stoica, Riederer & Chaintreau (2018) suggest that a good structural understanding of the causes 
should be encouraged in order to improve referral algorithms and reduce bias towards an over-
representation of male postings. 

The recognized importance of social influence on consumer behavior in online contexts 
raises also a concern over the attempts to manipulate individuals’ decisions and behavior 
through online information and stimulus. An increasingly visible concern comes from the 
activity of social robots, or bots, which are software robots that mimic human behavior online. 

Bots are defined as a computer algorithms that produce content and interact with users on 
social media, in order to influence their behavior (Ferrara, Varol, Davis, Menczer, & Flammini, 
2016). These bots are fulfilling mainly benign tasks and are useful in the achievement of 
different automatic functions. Yet, the activity of social bots may become problematic when 
they are used to share rumours or wrong information. They enable manipulation attempts by 
giving individuals the false impression that some piece of information is popular and endorsed 
by many people, thus increasing its credibility through social proof. 

Social bots can and are used to alter the impression of popularity and support for certain 
political campaigns and candidates (Ratkiewicz, Conover, Meiss, Goncalves, Flammini, & 
Menczer, 2011), they may increase panic feelings during emergencies and they can even alter 
stock market fluctuations (Hwang, Pearce, & Nanis, 2012). Although Ferrara, Varol, Davis, 
Menczer, & Flammini (2016) describe the functioning of several detection systems that could 
be used to identify social bots, the biggest problem for individuals is that they lack the necessary 
abilities to use them on their own. As the proponents argue, each of the described detection 
system has its imperfections and their best use is possible when they are combined.  

Thus, the biggest problem related to the influence of social bots comes from the inability 
of humans to recognize bots in social media. To counter this problem, individuals should 
become knowledgeable of online influences directed at them as well as of their own 
vulnerabilities. Additionally, it might not be enough for consumers do learn how to detect social 
bots but it is necessary to profoundly understand the online and social transformation taking 
place with increased usage of the Internet. The development of social bots implies that there 
are many actors interested in influencing consumer (or individual) actions. However, our level 
of knowledge on factors of social influence on online consumer behavior is at its infancy, and 
today consumers more often feel fooled and puzzled than knowledgeable of the market and 
self-aware of their own needs, wants and rationality.  

 
4. Moving Forward the Understanding of Online Consumer Behavior  
The consumers’ online social interaction enhances some characteristics of consumer behavior 
that were mostly invisible and thus ignored in the past. The influence of social and 
environmental cues is now quite well documented in research results and there are convincing 
arguments to be integrated in models of consumer behavior. As well, the cognitive limitations 
of consumers in relation to the high volume of online information should be translated in 
attempts to better understand consumer decision-making skills and the ways in which they can 
be developed. Since the premises of the rational consumer ideal are contradicted, consumer 



      27 

 
 

researchers should start developing a new view on consumer rationality, adapted to the current 
reality. Consumer rationality is not an inborn ability that everyone posseses, but it is dependent 
on an interaction of factors coming from the social environment and the individual self. Also 
consumers’ abilities in decision-making are prone to multiple errors and vulnerabilities.  

Consumer behavior in the online environment is still an emerging field of research which 
needs further exploration. The behaviors resulting from the online social interaction are  
dependent on the particular features offered by this environment: consumers have more control 
over engaging or not in the interaction and over when and how they will communicate. These 
characteristics of online social interaction apparently encourage an internal focus, which may 
enable consumers to hold back their implusiveness. However, many studies on impluse online 
behaviors confirm that consumers continue to be impulsive online, yet this type of behavior is 
dependent on the features offered by the computer-mediated communication tool.  

Social influence has been proved to play a major role in determining online consumer 
behaviors, yet there are important differences compared to the traditional face-to-face 
interactions that are worth noting. The effectiveness of each of the influence principles proposed 
by Cialdini (2001) differs in the online interaction: the consistency and the social proof 
principles have greater effectiveness, while the authority and liking principles face a reduced 
impact in influencing online behaviors. This might imply that online consumers are more 
concerned with maintaining a coherent self-concept and non-contradicting themselves, on one 
hand, and adjusting their self-concept to the relevant social context, on the other hand. Future 
research should explore the construction and significance of consumers’ online identity as well 
as the importance of self-concept in accepting or rejecting the influence of tactics aimed to 
persuade them. 

Social validation is also highly used by consumers in assessing online information 
credibility. The increasing volume of information available online leads to costs of cognitive 
processing (in terms of effort and time) and in order to cope with them individuals resort to 
heuristics for a faster decision-making. Most of these heuristics are based on social validation 
and confirmation of personal expections, both of which are prone to errors. Online information 
shared or endorsed by many is considered more credible and thus, individuals equate credibility 
with the popularity of an opinion. Also, the online information that corresponds to own 
expections, whether in terms of content or appearance, is considered credible and hence 
narrowing the information variety available to the individual. Online content creators can use 
the knowledge on these heuristics to make their message more credible, however, consumers 
should strive as well to become aware of their limitations and vulnerabilities in making wrong 
credibility judgements. Future research should explore in depth the ways in which these 
heuristics are used depending on the experience of the online user. Are some heuristics favored 
more by experienced Internet users, while less experienced Internet users favor other heuristics? 
Do the frequency and type of social media used influence individuals propensity to use only 
certain heuristics? These questions would be interesting to explore in order to understand better 
consumer vulnerabilities in information processing. 

While the impact of social proof on consumer behavior may be observed, there are also 
more subtle influences illustrated by the research on the direct link between perception and 
behavior. According to their findings, consumers are very sensitive to environmental cues and 
they unconsciously adopt attitudes and behaviors induced by others. Two implications are 
derived from this: automatic processes are not solely the result of repeated actions (habits) and 
the characteristics of the choice context influences behavioral decisions.  

Conscious and unconscious processes both play an important role in determining behavior 
and most often they support one another (Bargh, Schwader, Hailey, Dyer, & Boothby, 2012). 
Usually, unconscious processes determine social behavior and conscious processes alter 



28 

 

uncounscious impulses. Future research should try to include the study of unconscious 
processes in consumer behavior modeling and to find a way to account for the interplay between 
the conscious and unconscious thought processes. This development could be a real game 
changer in consumer behavior research, since the current methodologies based on self-reported 
answers to questionnaires will no longer be appropriate. Although, further debate and analysis 
is needed in order to implement new research methodologies, new ideas could come from 
various other fields like experimental psychology and machine-learning. 

  
5. Conclusions 
Despite an increased access to information, today’s consumer is not necessarily moving towards 
an increased consumer rationality in decision-making, since the increasing volume of online 
information comes with costs of cognitive processing and credibility evaluation. Moreover, the 
social influence on individual choice is becoming increasingly visible, an aspect that was rather 
ignored in the ideal of rational consumer. The online environment highlights particular aspects 
of consumer behavior that were rather hidden and seemingly unimportant in the world 
unconnected by the Internet: the impact of social influence and the cost of information 
processing.  

Today, in order to make good enough choices in a timely manner, consumers resort 
different shortcuts. Social proof is commonly used in decision-making and it mediates the 
credibility and authority given to the online source of information, directly influencing 
consumer trust and the subsequent behavior. This means that the information shared by many 
becomes true, trustworthy and acquires authority. 

Moreover, there are also social and environmental cues that trigger behavior automatically, 
without any cognitive processing. These cues may be used in order to increase consumer 
impulsiveness and lead to immediate behavior performance. Their impact is usually located at 
an unconscious level and for this reason they cannot be accounted for in consumer behavior 
model that concentrate on conscious cognitive processes. Consumer behavior research 
procedures could be changed and new tools developed in order to understand all these changes 
observed in the online social interaction so that consumer knowledge advances with the 
technology available. 

This paper aimed to review of the main influence mechanisms responsible for consumer 
decision-making with limited deliberation and to highlight their functioning in online contexts. 
The paper contributes to the literature on the changes in consumer behavior driven by online 
contexts and it highlights their implications for the ideal of consumer rationality. Another 
contribution is the discussion of social and environmental factors that increase consumer 
vulnerability and are worth considering for advancing consumer awareness and wellbeing. 
However, a major limitation for the paper is that it offers only an overall review of the multiple 
influences that diminish consumer deliberation in online settings. In order to improve our 
understanding and knowledge on the consumer online behavior three future research directions 
are proposed for further study: the impact of consumer experience with Internet on evaluating 
information credibility, the role played by consumer’s self-concept on the effectiveness of 
social influence tactics and the importance of online cues in triggering automatic behaviors.  

Another challenge for future research is also to find methodological solutions to reduce the 
use of the traditional way of testing consumer behavior patterns: through self-reported 
responses to questionnaires. In fact, there is a significant discrepancy between what consumers 
say they do and their actual behavior. This is a serious concern for testing models in online 
contexts since these questinnaires involve a lot of verbalization, while online interaction is 
based primarily on clicks, pictograms and multimedia content, none of which are spoken or 



      29 

 
 

written by the user. In order to adavance consumer research in this area new tools need to be 
developed with inspiration from psychology anf machine-learning research. 

 
References 
1. Aarts, H., Paulussen, T., & Schaalma, H. (1997). Physical exercise habit: On the 

conceptualization and formation of habitual health behaviors. Health Education Research: 
Theory and Practice, Vol. 12, 363-374. 

2. Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl, & J. 
Beckmann, Action-control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 11-39). Heidelberg: Springer. 

3. Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social 
Behaviour. New Jersey: Englewood Cliffs, Prentice-Hall. 

4. Bargh, J., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). The automaticity of social behavior: Direct 
effects of trait concept and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and 
Social Psychology, Vol. 71, 230–244. 

5. Bargh, J., Gollwitzer, P., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trötschel, R. (2001). he 
automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 81, 1014–1027. 

6. Bargh, J., Schwader, K., Hailey, S., Dyer, R., & Boothby, E. (2012). Automaticity in social-
cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 16, No. 12, 593-605. 

7. Carver, C., Ganellen, R., Froming, W., & Chambers, W. (1983). Modeling: An analysis in 
terms of category accessibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 19, 403-
421. 

8. Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of 
source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 
39, 752–766. 

9. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior 
link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 76, 893–
910. 

10. Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W. W., & Lakin, J. L. (2005). Beyond the perception-behavior 
link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In R. 
Hassin, J. S. Uleman, & J. Bargh, The new unconscious (pp. 334-361). New York: Oxford 
University Press. 

11. Cialdini, R. (2001 b, October). Harnessing the Science of Persuasion. Harvard Business 
Review, pp. 72-79. 

12. Cialdini, R. (2001). Influence. Science and Practice. Fourth Edition. Needham Heights: 
Allyn & Bacon. 

13. Cialdini, R. (2003, Summer). The Power of Persuasion. Stanford Social Innovation Review, 
pp. 18-27. 

14. Decety, J., & Grezes, J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human 
actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 3, 172–178. 

15. Dennison, G., Bourdage-Braun, S., & Chetuparambil, M. (2009). Social commerce defined. 
White paper #23747. Research Triangle Park, NC, USA: IBM Corporation. 

16. Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic 
effects of social perception on social behavior. Advances in Experimental Social 
Psychology, 1-40. 

17. Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway: The 
automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In M. Zanna, Advances in 
experimental social psychology, Vol. 33 (pp. 1-40). San Diego, CA: Academic. 



30 

 

18. Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (1996). The knife that cuts both ways: Facilitated 
and inhibited access to traits as a result of stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental 
Social Psychology, Vol. 32, 271–288. 

19. Dijksterhuis, A., Smith, P., van Baaren, R., & Wigboldus, D. (2005). The Unconscious 
Consumer: Effects of Environment on Consumer Behavior. Journal of Consumer 
Psychology, Vol. 15, Nr. 3, 193–202. 

20. Dittmar, H. (2008). Consuming passions? Psychological motives for buying consumer 
goods. In H. Dittmar, Consumer Culture, Identity and Well-Being. The Search for the 
"Good Life" and the "Body Perfect" (pp. 49-70). Psychology Press. 

21. Dittmar, H., & Drury, J. (2000). Self-image - is it in the bag? A qualitative 
comparisonbetween ordinary and "excessive" consumers. Journal of Economic 
Psychology, 109-142. 

22. Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE 
model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna, Advances in experimental social 
psychology Vol. 23 (pp. 75-109). San Diego, CA: Academic. 

23. Fazio, R., & Olson, M. (2014). The MODE model: Attitude-Behavior Processes as a 
Function of Motivation and Opportunity. In J. W. Sherman, B. Gawronski, & Y. Trope, 
Dual process theories of the social mind (pp. 155-171). New York: Guilford Press. 

24. Ferrara, E., Varol, O., Davis, C., Menczer, F., & Flammini, A. (2016). The Rise of Social 
Bots. Communications of the ACM, Vol. 59, Nr. 7, 96-104. 

25. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior. Reading, MA: 
Addison-Wesley. 

26. Fitzsimons, G., & Bargh, J. (2003). Thinking of you: Pursuit of interpersonal goals 
associated with relational partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 84, 
148–164. 

27. Fogg, B. (2003). Persuasive Technology: Using Computers to Change What We Think and 
Do. Morgan Kaufmann. 

28. Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the 
premotor cortex. Brain, Vol 119, 593–609. 

29. Guadagno, R., & Cialdini, R. (2005). Online persuasion and compliance: social influence 
on the Internet and beyond. In Y. Amichai-Hamburger, The Social Net: The social 
psychology of the Internet (pp. 91-113). New York: Oxford University Press. 

30. Guadagno, R., Muscanell, N., Rice, L., & Roberts, N. (2013). Social Influence Online: The 
Impact of Social Validation and Likability on Compliance. Psychology of Popular Media 
Culture, Vol. 2, No. 1, 51-60. 

31. Hausman, A. (2000). Multi-Method Investigation of Consumer Motivations in Impulse 
Buying Behavior. Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 17, Nr. 5, 403-426. 

32. Huang, Z., & Benyoucef, M. (2013). From e-commerce to social commerce: A close look 
at design features. Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, 246-259. 

33. Hwang, T., Pearce, I., & Nanis, M. (2012). Socialbots: Voices from the Fronts. ACM 
Interactions 19, 2, 38-45. 

34. Iacoboni, M. (2005). Understanding others: Imitation, language, empathy. In S. Hurley, & 
N. Chater, Perspectives on imitation: From mirror neurons to memes, Vol. 1 (pp. 77–100). 
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 

35. Internet Usage Statistics. (2018, June). Retrieved from Internet World Stats: 
https://www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htm 

36. Jackson, T. (2005). Motivating Sustainable Consumption. A review of evidence on 
Consumer Behavior and Behavioral Change. Sustainable Development Research Network. 

37. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt. 



      31 

 
 

38. Johnston, L. (2002). Behavioral mimicry and stigmatization. Social Cognition, Vol 20, 18–
35. 

39. Lancaster, K. (1966). A new approach to consumer theory. Journal of Political Economy, 
174, 132-157. 

40. Li, H., Kuo, C., & Russell, M. (1999). The Impact of Perceived Channel Utilities, Shopping 
Orientations, and Demographics on the Consumer’s On-Line Buying Behavior. Journal of 
Computer-Mediated Communication Vol. 5, No. 2, 
http://www.ascusc.org/icmc/vol5/issue2/hairong.html. 

41. Macrae, C., & Johnston, L. (1998). Help, I need somebody: Automatic action and inaction. 
Social Cognition, Vol. 16, 400-417. 

42. Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and Personality. Harper & Row Publishers, Inc. 
43. Metzger, M., Flanagin, A., & Medders, R. (2010). Social and Heuristic Approaches to 

Credibility Evaluation Online. Journal of Communication, Vol. 60, 413-439. 
44. Orji, R., Mandryk, R., & Vassileva, J. (2015). Gender, Age and Responsiveness to 

Cialdini's Persuasion Strategies. In T. MacTavish, & S. Basapur, PERSUASIVE 2015 (pp. 
147-159). Springer International Publishing Switzerland. 

45. Ouellette, J., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and Intention in Everyday Life: The Multiple 
Processes by Which Past Behavior Predicts Future Behavior. Psychological Bulletin Vol. 
124 Nr. 1, 54-74. 

46. Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to 
advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer 
Research, 10, 135–146. 

47. Ratkiewicz, J., Conover, M., Meiss, M., Goncalves, B., Flammini, A., & Menczer, F. 
(2011). Detecting and Tracking Political Abuse in Social Media. Proceedings of the Fifth 
Annual AAAI Conference on Weblogs and Social Media, (pp. 297-304). 

48. Samuelson, P. (1938). A note on the pure theory of consumer behaviour. Economica, 5, 
61-71. 

49. Scott, J. (2000). Rational Choice Theory. In G. Browning, A. Halcli, & F. Webster, 
Understanding Contemporary Society: Theories of The Present (pp. 126-138). Sage. 

50. Shepperd, B., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P. (1988). The Theory of Reasoned Action: A 
Meta-Analysis of Past Research with Recommendations for Modifications and Future 
Research. Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 15, No. 3, 325-343. 

51. Simon, H. (1957). Models of Man. New York: John Wiley. 
52. Stoica, A.-A., Riederer, C., & Chaintreau, A. (2018). Algorithmic Glass Ceiling in Social 

Networks. The effects of social recommendations on network diversity. WWW 2018: The 
2018 Web Conference,. New York, NY, USA: ACM. 

53. Sundar, S. (2008). The MAIN model: A heuristic approach to understanding technology 
effects on credibility. In M. Metzger, & A. Flanagin, Digital media, youth, and credibility 
(pp. 73-100). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 

54. Taraborelli, D. (2008). How the Web is changing the way we trust. In K. Waelbers, A. 
Briggle, & P. Brey, Current issues in computing and philosophy. Amsterdam: IOS Press. 

55. van Baaren, R., Holland, R., Kawakami, K., & van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and 
Prosocial Behavior. Psychological Science, Vol. 15, 71-74. 

56. Wells, J., Parboteeah, V., & Valacich, J. (2011). Online Impulse Buying: Understanding 
the Interplay between Consumer Impulsiveness and Website Quality. Journal of the 
Association for Information Systems Vol. 12 No 1, 32-56. 

57. Wigand, R., Benjamin, R., & Birkland, J. (2008). Web 2.0 and beyond: implications for 
electronic commerce. Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Electronic 
Commerce Innsbruck, Austria, August. New York: ACMPress. 



32 

 

58. Cerruti C., Musso F. (2004), “La logistica nei distretti industriali. Scenari di evoluzione 
per l’integrazione delle filiere”, Finanza, Marketing e Produzione, 4(22), 23-40.  

59. Pepe C., Musso F. (2003), “The International Opening of Small District Firms. 
Flexibility Vs. Consolidation in Channel Relations”, Conference on Clusters, Industrial 
Districts and Firms: The Challenge of Globalization, Modena, September 12-13. 

60. Musso F. (2005), L’impresa commerciale minore in Italia, Cedam, Padova. 
61. Musso F. (2013), Strategie e competitività internazionale delle piccole e medie imprese. 

Un'analisi sul settore della meccanica, Cedam, Padova.  
62. Musso F. (2010), "Small Retailing, Town Centres and Inland Territories. An “Extended Town 

Centre Management” Perspective", Public Administration & Regional Studies, 3rd Year, n. 2 
(6), Galati University Press, pp. 37-58. 

63. Musso F. (1997), “The Changing Role of Trade Co-operation in Regional Economies: A Case 
Study”, 37th European Regional Science Association Congress, Roma, Università degli Studi 
di Roma "Tor Vergata", Facoltà di Economia, 26-29 agosto 

64. Musso F. (2012), “Technology in Marketing Channels: Present and Future Drivers of 
Innovation”, International Journal of Applied Behavioural Economics (IJABE), vol. 1, n. 2, pp. 
41-51. doi:10.4018/ijabe.2012040104. 

65. Palmeira M., Musso F. (2017), "3Rs of sustainability and the impact on consumer behavior”, 
Conference Proceedings, 24th Recent Advances in Retailing & Services Science Conference - 
EIRASS 2017, June 26-29, Vancouver. 

66. Musso F. (1996), "Potere e stabilità nei rapporti di fornitura della grande distribuzione 
britannica", Economia e Diritto del Terziario, n. 3, ISSN: 1593-9464, pp. 971-1011. 

67. Pepe C., Musso F., Risso M. (2010), SME Food Suppliers versus Large Retailers: Perspectives 
in the International Supply Chains, DSI Essays Series, n. 13, McGraw-Hill, Milano.