25

A COMPARISON OF NON-VERBAL 
CREATIVIT Y BE T WEEN LITHUANIAN  
AND MEXIC AN ADOLESCENTS

Pedro Sánchez-Escobedo1

Universidad Autónoma de Yucatán, México

Jurga Misiūnienė, Eglė Mockaitytė 

Vytautas Magnus University, Lithuania

Abstract. Background. The purpose of this work was to explore non-verbal creativity (free 
from language influences) between Mexican and Lithuanian adolescents. Methods. 
This is a cross comparative study of 354 high school students (average age 17.2 years) 
from Lithuania and Mexico who were asked to complete four tasks from the 2 non-
verbal sections of the Multifactorial assessment of creativity test, EMUC (Evaluación 
Multifactorial de la Creatividad, Sánchez, García, Valdes, 2009). The first section ex-
plored visual-spatial creativity associated with vocational choices such as architec-
ture and graphic design. The second explored inventive associated with realistic vo-
cational choices such as engineering and practical handcraft. Results. No significant 
differences were noticed in the procedures of administering, interpreting and using 
test results in these two countries. In general, Lithuanians show more fluidity and flex-
ibility, whereas Mexicans show more originality. Results indicated a poor relationship 
between high creative potential and vocational choices that demand creativity (i.e. 
design, music, etc.). Consistent gender differences were observed depending upon 
the type of creativity assessed. Women showed higher creativity on visual-spatial 
tasks, while men in originality for inventive tasks. Conclusions. Results underline the 
difficulties in assessing and comparing creative products from different cultures. They 
underline the importance of context specific criteria to judge creativity in an ipsative 
fashion. Furthermore, overall results suggest that visual-spatial tasks in this age range 
should focus on originality and elaboration, whereas the ingenuity task should focus 
on flexibility and fluidity.

Keywords. EMUC, visual-spatial creativity, inventive creativity, culture.

1 Address for correspondence: Address: PO BOX 12o7. Facultad de Educación, UADY.  
Mérida Yucatán Mexico CP97000, Phone: +521478681, E-mail: psanchez@correo.uady.mx.

SCIENTIFIC PUBLICATIONS
International Journal of Psychology:  
Biopsychosocial Approach 2015 / 17 
ISSN 1941-7233 (Print), ISSN 2345-024X (Online) 
http://dx.doi.org/10.7220/2345-024X.17.2



26

Pedro Sánchez-Escobedo, Jurga Misiūnienė, Eglė Mockaitytė

INTRODUC TION

Testing creativity has been considered difficult due to the lack of 
reliable instruments and the inherent difficulty to test divergent think-
ing and originality. Creativity testing has various and solid challenges 
regarding validity issues. Hardship in measuring creativity lies in the fact 
that measurement theory is usually based on comparing a given obser-
vation against a norm, whereas when estimating a degree of creativity, 
one seeks to establish the deviation from the norm or even the existence 
of something that has no norms (originality). Indeed, creativity testing 
has been associated with divergent thinking that requires the test taker 
to use imagination and explore solutions in many different directions, 
sometimes with no single right answer. Thus, efforts to develop a creativ-
ity quotient similar to the intelligence quotient (IQ) have been unsuc-
cessful since they depend on many more contextual factors than intel-
ligence (Ferrando, Prieto, & Sánchez, 2005).

Historically, the most systematic assessment of creativity in elemen-
tary school children has been conducted by Torrance and his associates 
who have developed and administered the Torrance Test of Creative 
Thinking (TTCT, 1960) which included routines on divergent thinking 
and problem-solving skills that were assessed in four dimensions:

1.  Fluency: the total number of interpretable, meaningful, and rel-
evant ideas generated in response to the stimulus;

2. Flexibility: the number of different categories of relevant responses;
3. Originality: the statistical rarity of the responses;
4.  Elaboration: the amount of detail in the responses.
The third edition of the TTCT in 1984 eliminated the Flexibility scale 

from the figural test but added Resistance to Premature Closure and Ab-
stractness of Titles. Torrance called the new scoring procedure Stream-
lined Scoring. Evaluating creativity then involved five norm-referenced 
measures: fluency, originality, abstractness of titles, elaboration, and 
resistance to premature closure, and 13 criterion-referenced measures 
which included emotional expressiveness, story-telling articulateness, 
movement or actions, expressiveness of titles, syntheses of incomplete 
figures, synthesis of lines, of circles, unusual visualization, extending or 
breaking boundaries, humor, richness of imagery, colorfulness of im-
agery, and fantasy. Torrance (1980) grouped the different subtests of 
the Minnesota Tests of Creative Thinking (MTCT) into three categories: 



27

2015, 17, 25–38 p.International Journal of Psychology: 
A Biopsychosocial Approach

(1) verbal tasks using verbal stimuli, (2) verbal tasks using non-verbal 
stimuli, and (3) non-verbal tasks. Despite the fact that Torrance Test of 
Creative Thinking (TTCT) is probably the most commonly used in cross-
cultural studies (Lubart, 1990), other tests claim to be valid also, for ex-
ample, the Creativity Achievement Questionnaire which is a self-report 
that explores creative achievement across 10 domains. Nonetheless, 
despite many efforts to assess creativity around the globe, there is still 
skepticism in the academic community on whether standardized tests 
can be used to measure creativity (Carson, Peterson, Higgins, 2005).

Difficulties in judging creative products have lead researchers to 
use personality traits as indicators of creative potential. For example, 
independence of judgment, self-confidence, attraction to complexity, 
aesthetic orientation and risk-taking have been associated to creativity. 
In this perspective, a meta-analysis by Feist (1999) showed that creative 
people tend to be open to new experiences, unconventional, self-confi-
dent, self-accepting, driven, ambitious, dominant, hostile and impulsive.

Beyond this individual approach based on personality, creativity has 
also been the focus of various cross-cultural studies. As Lubart (1990) 
claimed, creativity must be understood beyond personal variables in-
cluding cultural and environmental variables. According to this author, 
culture interacting with personality regulates the general level of cre-
ativity. In this perspective, Ramos and Puccio (2014) compared Western 
and Eastern cultures and found that both cultures have implicit belief 
that creativity is thinking out of the box, something new, innovative, un-
usual and different, however, Americans link creativity with arts more 
frequently than Singaporeans. Zhou, Shen, Wang, Neber & Johji (2013) 
also observed that creativity is depicted as divergent thinking and linked 
with novelty in Germany and China, but German teachers think creativ-
ity is less likely to be expressed in mathematics, whereas Chinese teach-
ers dispute the potential to exhibit creativity in literature. Hence, the per-
ception of creativity is influenced by cultural factors.

Creativity and intelligence
There has been debate in the psychological literature whether intel-

ligence (as measured by IQ) and creativity are part of the same mental 
process (the conjoint hypothesis) or represent distinct mental processes 
(the disjoint hypothesis). Evidence from correlational studies since the 
1950s has not settled this issue to the satisfaction for most scholars.



28

Pedro Sánchez-Escobedo, Jurga Misiūnienė, Eglė Mockaitytė

While some researchers believe that creativity is the outcome of the 
same cognitive processes as intelligence (Feist & Barron, 2003), others 
believe that creativity is, in fact, a mental process that has to do more 
with emotions, intuition, or spirituality (Janesic, 2001; Corry, Mallet, 
Lewis, & Abdel-Khalek, 2013). Actually, Einstein’s statements regarding 
creativity and intelligence have left this issue open to debate for many 
years: “The true sign of intelligence is not knowledge but imagination” 
and “creativity is intelligence having fun.”

The threshold hypothesis proposed by Torrance posits that a high de-
gree of intelligence appears to be a necessary but not sufficient condi-
tion for high creativity (Torrance, 1988). That is, while there is a positive 
correlation between creativity and intelligence, this correlation disap-
pears for IQs above a threshold of around 120. Such a model has found 
acceptance by many researchers, although it has not gone unchallenged. 

Neurobiology of Creativity
An emphasis on biological factors related to creativity has been 

noted in recent years. Heilman, Nadeau, & Beversdof (2003) assert that 
highly creative people who excel at creative innovation tend to differ 
from others in three functions based in the frontal lobe: they have a high 
level of specialized knowledge, they are capable of divergent thinking, 
and they are able to modulate neurotransmitters such as norepineph-
rine. Thus, the frontal lobe appears to be the part of the cortex that is 
most important for creativity. Flaherty (2005) suggested that the creative 
drive results from an interaction of the frontal lobes, the temporal lobes, 
and dopamine from the limbic system. She asserted that whereas the 
frontal lobes can be seen as responsible for producing ideas, the tempo-
ral lobes are known for their editing and evaluation. Thus, abnormalities 
in the frontal lobe (such as depression or anxiety) generally decrease cre-
ativity, while abnormalities in the temporal lobe often increase it. High 
activity in the temporal lobe typically inhibits activity in the frontal lobe, 
and vice versa. High dopamine levels increase general arousal and goal 
directed behaviors and reduce latent inhibition, and all three effects in-
crease the drive to generate ideas (Cromie, 2007). This new neurobio-
logical evidence further supports the notion that creative potential is  
a construct rather inconsistent in time and context dependent in nature.



29

2015, 17, 25–38 p.International Journal of Psychology: 
A Biopsychosocial Approach

Gender and creativity
Although Hyde & Linn (1998) claimed that there are basically no gen-

der differences in verbal creativity, Runco, Cramond, & Pagnani (2010) 
argued that men are identified as more creative because of their higher 
social recognition and coincided that there are no gender differences  
in the creative potential. Gough (1992) also reported no significant dif-
ferences between men and women. 

Baer and Kaufman (2008) meta-analysis of gender differences in cre-
ativity also argued in favor of a lack of consistent gender differences in 
overall creativity test scores, creative achievements, and self-reported 
creativity. They argued that the debate on gender differences is a dif-
ficult arena in which to conduct a research, with few significant differ-
ences in creativity test scores and accomplishments of boys and girls, 
and in case they are found, they tend to favor girls. As a result, it is dif-
ficult to show how innate gender differences in creativity could possibly 
explain later differences in creative accomplishment. Nonetheless, gen-
der differences should lead investigators to explore environmental and 
cultural influences that explain the observed differences in the creative 
achievement of men and women in many fields. For example, Hender-
son (2003) recognized women that worked in multinational firms and 
had the same publications and conference presentations as men. These 
women depicted their experiences in family, school, community and 
higher education as crucial to enhance their ability to invent. 

In many cases, especially in research on divergent-thinking, there 
are significant numbers of studies in which one sex score higher, but 
these findings are generally counter-balanced by studies showing the 
opposite.

In spite of these previous claims, research continues to evidence gen-
der differences in creative productivity, and these differences represent 
the most significant unanswered questions about gender and creativity. 
It is clear that a large part of those differences are either cultural or envi-
ronmental, including differences in adult expectations of girls and boys, 
differences in opportunities available to male and female children and 
adults, and differences in the kinds of experiences women and men are 
likely to have. There are also differences in how different kinds of creativ-
ity works, including those more typically produced by women and men, 
and how they are valued by different cultures. However, very few studies 



30

Pedro Sánchez-Escobedo, Jurga Misiūnienė, Eglė Mockaitytė

published explored gender differences in creativity considering cultural 
background. Hence, the purpose of this work was to explore non-verbal 
creativity performances in youngsters, men and women, from two dif-
ferent cultural backgrounds. Cross-cultural studies are recognized as 
means to understand the links between creativity and changing mod-
ern cultures (Lubart, 1990), they reveal complexity of creativity in various 
cultures (Ramos, Puccio, 2014) and they provide information helpful to 
gain awareness and understanding of these differences (Corry, Mallett, 
Lewis, Abdel-Khalek, 2013).

ME THODS

Type of study
This is a quantitative, exploratory study that collects information 

from two groups of youngsters between 16 and 19 years of age from 
Mexico and Lithuania. Researchers originally aimed to collect a sample 
of 180 participants per country from high school and college fresh-
men. In each country, a conventional sample of students volunteered 
to participate in the study. It was assumed that each group reflected 
their country´s culture in the performance of two different non-verbal 
creative tasks. 

Par ticipants
Table 1 depicts demographic characteristics of participants finally 

included in each country sample. A total of 354 students completed the 
test with directions in their born language. Six students were discarded 
because their test was either incomplete or they did not follow direc-
tions. 

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of par ticipants

Mexico Lithuania Total

Males 82 95 198

Females 74 103 156

Total 177 177 354

Average age, years 18.4 16.3 17.2



31

2015, 17, 25–38 p.International Journal of Psychology: 
A Biopsychosocial Approach

Instrument
The two non-verbal subscales of the three scales of the Multidimen-

sional Scale for Creativity developed by Sánchez (2011) were used. The 
first subscale attempts to measure visual-spatial creativity by providing 
a stimulus consisting of figures that require the respondent to use in free 
drawing. Two performances are required, the first one timed. Criteria for 
scoring performance consist of frequency of objects drawn (fluency), 
frequency of categories (flexibility), originality and degree of complexity 
of drawings (elaboration).

The second subscale attempts to measure inventive creativity. As 
in the previous subscale, two stimuli are provided and the respondent 
is requested to write as many uses one can think for that object. Scor-
ing criteria are based on and consists of frequency of objects (fluency), 
frequency of categories (flexibility) and originality. Sánchez, García, & 
Valdes (2009) provided evidence of the external validity for this test in 
discerning highly creative students and stated that the instrument has 
good psychometric properties according to content, construct and crite-
rion validity and high reliability (Cronbach’s alpha value is above 0.8) that 
make it usable for measuring creativity in Mexican adolescents. The test 
was translated to Lithuanian and administered in a standardized fashion 
by respecting the timing for each routine. 

This tests does not provide norms to assess creativity and assumes 
ipsative criteria to judge performances in the routines. Both versions of 
the test showed acceptable psychometric properties (see Table 2).

Table 2. Reliability coefficients for each test (Cronbach’s Alfa)

Mexico Lithuania Total

Visual-spatial .71 .67 198

Inventive .74 .64 156

Scoring system to assess fluidity and flexibility consists of the fre-
quency of ideas in the former and of the variation of ideas in the latter. 
Scores ranged from 0 to 10. Scoring for originality and elaboration are 
given in a scale from 0 to 5. 



32

Pedro Sánchez-Escobedo, Jurga Misiūnienė, Eglė Mockaitytė

Procedures
Investigators obtained permission from school authorities, parents 

and students to administer the test. The instruments were administered 
in each country following their own rules. For instance, parental consent 
was not needed in Mexico. Participants were explained the purpose of 
this study and asked to voluntarily participate in it. Data from both coun-
tries was coded and analyzed using SPSS Statistics version 21.

RESULTS

Visual-spatial creativity
The first subscale estimated visual-spatial creativity. The mean scores 

of both performances were compared by gender and country. T-tests for 
independent samples established significant differences as shown in 
Tables 3 and 4.

Table 3. Visual-spatial creativity per countr y

Fluidity Flexibility Originality Elaboration

Mexico Lithuania Mexico Lithuania Mexico Lithuania Mexico Lithuania

Draw 1 2.21 2.55 1.51 1.94 .17 .33 .17 .27
Draw 2 2.71 3.15 1.94 2.45 .29 .16 .24 .16

Average 2.46 2.85* 1.72 3.16* .23 .24 .20 .21

Note: * p ≤ .05

It can be observed that Lithuanian students show more Fluidity and 
Flexibility than Mexican students. Overall, no differences were found in 
Originality and Elaboration. However, cultural differences are manifested 
in the drawings of subjects. For example, Mexican students provided 
more examples and uses about living in the tropics, whereas Lithuanian 
students used situations related to extreme winter situations.

Table 4. Visual-spatial creativity differences by gender

Fluidity Flexibility Originality Elaboration
Males Females Males Females Males Females Males Females

Draw 1 2.28 2.50 1.61 1.87 .21 .27 .12 .30
Draw 2 2.63 3.26 2.00 2.42 .24 .19 .18 .21

Average 2.45 2.88* 1.80 2.14* .33 .36 .15 .25*

Note: * p ≤ .05



33

2015, 17, 25–38 p.International Journal of Psychology: 
A Biopsychosocial Approach

Note the consistent gender differences in favor of women, regard-
less of country in this domain: women show more Fluidity, Flexibility and 
Elaboration.

Inventive creativity
Participants were asked to name as many possible uses of a rope and 

a blanket. They had a time limit of 120 seconds per routine. Results com-
paring creativity according to country and gender are shown in Tables 
5 and 6.

Table 5. Inventive creativity differences by countr y

Fluidity Flexibility Originality
Mexico Lithuania Mexico Lithuania Mexico Lithuania

Rope 6.12 6.84 4.60 6.00 .56 .15
Blanket 5.40 7.12 4.43 6.17 .53 .15
Average 5.76 6.8* 4.51 6.08* .54* .15

Note: * p ≤ .05

It can be observed in the inventive subtest that in general, Lithua-
nian students show more Fluidity and Flexibility than Mexican students. 
However, Mexican students show more Originality. 

In this test, gender differences were explored. Table 6 illustrates dif-
ferences of inventive creativity between different genders.

Table 6. Inventive creativity differences by gender

Fluidity Flexibility Originality

Males Females Males Females Males Females

Rope 6.63 6.41 5.38 5.38 .42 .25
Blanket 6.40 6.32 5.41 5.39 .36 .28
Average 6.51* 6.36 5.39 5.38 .44* .26

Note: * p ≤ .05

In contrast to visual-spatial creativity, men tend to score higher in 
Fluidity and Originality dimensions of inventive creativity.

Participating students were asked to state their future vocational 
aspiration or career choice. Open responses were categorized and com-
puted. Table 7 depicts results by country and gender.



34

Pedro Sánchez-Escobedo, Jurga Misiūnienė, Eglė Mockaitytė

Table 7. Vocational aspirations by countr y and gender

Occupation Mexico Lithuania Total

Males Females Males Females

Arts 21 14 15 21 71

Medicine (health) 5 3 15 15 38

Engineering 18 10 3 1 32

Computer science 10 5 10 0 25

Science 0 2 11 8 21

Commerce 0 3 12 5 20

Finances 3 2 7 5 17

Law 1 2 5 3 11

Education 0 7 0 3 10

Sports 1 0 3 2 6

Communication 3 1 3 1 8

Politics 0 0 1 2 3

Gastronomy 6 6 0 0 12

It can be observed that in general, Lithuanian students have greater 
vocational aspirations, selecting careers such as Law, Medicine and Fi-
nances. Mexican students tend to choose new vocational alternatives 
such as Gastronomy. Gender differences are few regarding vocational 
choice, except for some preference in Engineering and Computer sci-
ence by males in both countries.

Vocational choices were classified by the degree of creativity de-
manded, for example, file keeping (low), legal work (medium) or graphi-
cal design (high). As it can be seen in Table 8, high scores in Originality 
and Elaboration in visual-spatial task and Originality in inventive tasks 
were related to vocational choices with high creative demands. Thus, 
these factors must be considered in vocational guidance. Fluidity or 
Flexibility, however, were not related to vocational desires.



35

2015, 17, 25–38 p.International Journal of Psychology: 
A Biopsychosocial Approach

Table 8. Scores by demand of creativity and vocational choice

Indicators Low Medium High

Visual-spatial

Fluidity 2.88 2.48 2.64

Flexibility 2.09 1.82 1.99

Originality .12 .18 .45

Elaboration .16 .15 .38

Inventive

Fluidity 5.51 6.48 6.73

Flexibility 5.51 5.32 5.23

Originality .26 .32 .63

DISCUSSION

Results show more consistent differences by gender than by coun-
try. These findings should revise the debate on gender differences and 
creativity, and they indeed challenge previous researchers claiming 
there are no gender differences in creative potential (Hyde & Linn, 1998; 
Baer & Kaufman, 2008). Perhaps, the observed patterns between men 
and women in relation to a given creative task lead investigators to infer 
more significant influences due to sex than to culture itself. Women 
showing more creativity in visual-spatial tasks and men in inventive 
tasks should suggest that the analysis of gender differences should in-
clude the type of creativity that is under the study. 

Regarding cultural differences, obvious themes were identified. 
For example, the drawings appeared to be based on demographics, i.e. 
snow landscapes drawn by Lithuanian students vs. beach landscapes –  
by Mexican students. Unlike Zhou et al. (2013) and Ramos & Puccio 
(2014), no major cultural differences in creativity were found. Maybe, the 
fact that both groups had roughly the same school level cancelled cul-
tural effects.

It can be noted that test adaptation in Lithuania was uncomplicated 
since only directions for the test and the demographic questions re-
quired translation. The focus of the measurements was non-verbal tasks. 
This follows Sánchez-Escobedo & Hollingworth (2011) guidelines:

During the translation and/or adaption of any test, it is important 
to make sure that the test is more understandable to the test takers, the 
directions are easy to comprehend, and the items are ordered on an ap-
propriate scale of difficulty (p. 26).



36

Pedro Sánchez-Escobedo, Jurga Misiūnienė, Eglė Mockaitytė

Lessons learned and Recommendations
Testing for creativity remains a challenge for investigators seeking 

to develop instruments that could be useful for schools when screening 
students with high creative potential so they could receive, for exam-
ple, additional instruction or vocational guidance to make the most of 
this potential. Furthermore, comparative studies, such as this one, elicit 
many more questions than those that guided the study. For instance, 
is gender a more compelling influence in different creative tasks than 
culture? This question remains unanswered and should trigger more 
research projects that include not only the usual dependent variables  
of gender and nationality but that consistently include a third element 
of analysis: the type of creative task under scrutiny.

The process of administering, scoring and interpreting results from 
these two very different countries demonstrates once again the concep-
tual, methodological and practical difficulties in assessing creativity.

Maybe, efforts to develop standardized tests that objectively at-
tempt to identify the degree of creativity in students should be replaced 
by more innovative approaches. Researchers should focus on proce-
dures more qualitative in nature, able to identify a student with creative 
potential that it is worth to be cultivated in school.

References

Baer, J., Kaufman, J. C. (2008). Gender differences in creativity. Journal of Creative 
Behavior, 42 (2), 75–105. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.2008.tb01289.x.

Carson, Sh. H., Peterson, J. B., Higgins D. M. (2005). Reliability, validity, and factor 
structure of the creative achievement questionnaire. Creativity Research 
Journal, 17 (1), 37–50. doi: 10.1207/s15326934crj1701_4.

Corry, D. A. S., Mallett, J., Lewis, C. A., Abdel-Khalek, A. M. (2013). The creativity-
spirituality construct and its role in transformative coping. Mental Health, 
Religion & Culture, 16 (10), 979–990. doi: 10.1080/13674676.2013.834492.

Cromie, W.  J. (2007). The brains behind writer’s block: new views of the muse. Retrieved 
October 10, 2015, from http://news.harvard.edu/gazette/2004/01.29/01-
creativity.html.

Feist, G. F. (1999). The influence of personality on artistic and scientific creativity. 
In R. J. Sternberg, Handbook of Creativity (pp. 273–297). Cambridge University 
Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511807916.016.



37

2015, 17, 25–38 p.International Journal of Psychology: 
A Biopsychosocial Approach

Feist, G. F., Barron, F. X. (2003). Predicting creativity from early to late adulthood: 
intellect, potential, and personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 
62–88. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00536-6.

Flaherty, A. W. (2005). Frontotemporal and dopaminergic control of idea gene-
ration and creative drive. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493 (1), 147–153. 
doi: 10.1002/cne.20768.

Gough, H. (1992). Assessment of creative potential in psychology and the 
development of creative temperament scale. In J. C. Rosen, P. McReynolds, 
Advances in Psychological Assessment (pp. 225–257). New York: Plenun Press.

Heilman, K., Nadeau, S., Beversdof, D. (2003). Creative innovations: possible brain 
mechanisms. Neurocase, 9 (5), 369–379. doi: 10.1076/neur.9.5.369.16553.

Henderson, S. (2003). The correlates of inventor motivation, creativity and 
achievement. Unpublished doctortal dissertation. Stanford University.

Hyde, J. S., Linn, M. C. (1998). Gender differences in verbal ability: a meta-analysis. 
Psychological Bulletin, 104 (1), 53–69.

Janesic, V. J. (2001). Intuition and creativity: a pas de deux for qualitative researches. 
Qualitative Inquiry, 7 (5), 531–540.

Lubart, T. I. (1990). Creativity and cross-cultural variation. International Journal of 
Psychology, 25, 39–59. 

Ramos, S. J., Puccio, G. J. (2014). Cross-cultural studies of implicit theories of 
creativity: a comparative analysis between the United States and the main 
ethnic groups in Singapore. Creativity Research Journal, 26 (2), 223–228. doi: 
10.1080/10400419.2014.901094.

Runco, M. A., Cramond, B., Pagnani, A. (2010). Gender and creativity. In J. C. Chrisler, 
D. R. McCreary, Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology (pp. 343–357). 
Springer Press.

Sánchez-Escobedo, P., Hollingworth, L. (2011). Validación y normas para México de 
escala Weschler de inteligencia para niños IV. México: El Manual Moderno.

Sánchez, P. (2011). Evaluacion Multifactorial de la Creatividad EMUC. Universidad 
Autonoma de Yucatan.

Sánchez, P. A., García, A., Valdes, A. A. (2009). Validez y confiabilidad de un 
instrumento para medir la creatividad en adolescentes sobresalientes. Revista 
Iberoamericana de Educación, 6 (50), 17–23.

Torrance, E. P. (1980). Growing up creatively gifted: A 22 year longitudinal study. 
The Creative Child and Adult Quaterly, 25, 148–158.

Torrance, E. P. (1988). SOLAT (Style of learning and thinking) manual. Bensenville, IL: 
Scholastic Testing Service.

Zhou, J., Shen, J., Wang, X., Neber, H., Johji, I. (2013). A Cross-cultural comparison: 
teachers’ conceptualizations of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 25 (3), 
239–247. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2012.730006.



38

Pedro Sánchez-Escobedo, Jurga Misiūnienė, Eglė Mockaitytė

NE VERBALINIO LIE TUVOS IR MEKSIKOS PAAUGLIŲ 
KŪRYBIŠKUMO PALYGINIMAS

Pedro Sánchez-Escobedo
Jukatano autonominis universitetas, Meksika 

Jurga Misiūnienė, Eglė Mockait ytė 
Vytauto Didžiojo universitetas, Lietuva

Santrauka. Tyrimo tikslas. Šio darbo tikslas – įvertinti skirtingose kultūrose (Lietuvoje ir 
Meksikoje) gyvenančių paauglių neverbalinio kūrybiškumo skirtumus. Metodas. 
Tarpkultūriniame tyrime dalyvavo 354 Lietuvos ir Meksikos gimnazijų mokiniai 
(amžiaus vidurkis – 17,2 m.), kurie atliko keturias Multifaktorinio kūrybiškumo 
įvertinimo testo EMUC (Evaluación Multifactorial de la Creatividad, Sánchez, García, 
Valdes, 2009) užduotis, skirtas neverbaliniam kūrybiškumui įvertinti. Iš dviejų 
užduočių sudarytos pirmosios skalės paskirtis – įvertinti vizualinį-erdvinį kūrybiškumą, 
siejamą su tokiais profesiniais pasirinkimais kaip architektūra, grafikos dizainas ir pan. 
Antrosios, kurią sudaro taip pat dvi užduotys, paskirtis – įvertinti išradingumą, siejamą 
su tokiais profesiniais pasirinkimais kaip inžinerija ar amatai. Rezultatai. Rezultatai 
nepatvirtino prielaidos, kad kūrybingesni Meksikos ir Lietuvos paaugliai rinksis 
kūrybiškumo reikalaujančias profesijas (pvz., dizainerio, muziko ir pan.). Analizuojant 
testo adminstravimą, interpretavimą ir rezultatų panaudojimą skirtumų tarp lietuvių 
ir meksikiečių nebuvo rasta, tačiau pastebėta, kad pirmieji pasižymi aukštesniu 
mąstymo laisvumu bei lankstumu, o antrieji – originalumu. Analizuojant skirtingų tipų 
kūrybiškumą patvirtinti lyčių skirtumai: merginos geriau atlieka vaizdines-erdvines,  
o vaikinai išradingumo užduotis. Išvados. Rezultatai atskleidžia sunkumus, kylančius 
vertinant kūrybiškumą ir lyginant kūrybiškumo rezultatus, o tai skatina kūrybiškumą 
vertinti ipsatyviai. Be to, apibendrinti rezultatai rodo, kad vertinant vizualines-
erdvines užduotis turėtų būti labiau akcentuojamas kūrybinio mąstymo originalumas 
ir detalumas, o išradingumą – lankstumas bei laisvumas.

Pagrindiniai žodžiai: daugiafaktorinis kūrybiškumo įvertinimas (EMUC), vizualinis- 
erdvinis kūrybiškumas, išradingumas, kultūra. 

Received: October 14, 2015
Accepted: October 19, 2015