Ritchie, T. D., & Bryant, F. B. (2012). Positive state mindfulness: A multidimensional model of 

mindfulness in relation to positive experience. International Journal of Wellbeing, 2(3), 150-181. 

doi:10.5502/ijw.v2.i3.1 

 

Timothy D. Ritchie 

University of Limerick 

tim.ritchie@ul.ie 

Copyright belongs to the author(s) 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 

150 

ARTICLE  

 

Positive state mindfulness: 

A multidimensional model of mindfulness in relation to 

positive experience 
 

Timothy D. Ritchie  ·  Fred B. Bryant 

 

 
Abstract: The present research tested Langer’s theory of mindfulness in the context of positive 

experiences: positive state mindfulness. In Study 1 (N1 = 586, N2 = 415) confirmatory factor 

analyses indicated that a three-factor model (Focused Attention, Novelty Appreciation, Open-

Ended Expectations) fit the data well and explained responses better than a one-factor model. In 

support of construct validity, Study 2 (N3 = 239, N4 = 126) suggested that each dimension had a 

different pattern of associations with unidimensional trait measures of mindfulness, savoring 

beliefs, trait absorption, uncertainty tolerance, need for structure, and need for cognition. Study 

3 (N5 = 46) revealed that each dimension correlated uniquely with the positive affect, self-esteem, 

interpersonal connectedness, and the overall rehearsal frequency associated with positive 

autobiographical events. In support of criterion validity in Study 4, in Experiment 1 (N6 = 46) a 

boredom task decreased Novelty Appreciation, and in Experiment 2 (N7 = 92) a problem-solving 

task increased Focused Attention. Our data suggest that positive mindfulness is more than the 

absence of mindlessness and that it includes three distinct dimensions. We discuss the utility of 

positive mindfulness in both research and practice. 

 

Keywords: state mindfulness; positive experience; wellbeing; savoring 

 

 

1. Introduction 

Mindfulness is often conceptualized as paying attention flexibly to the present moment. 

Notwithstanding the different conceptualizations of mindfulness, practitioners and researchers 

generally agree with the idea that mindfulness involves a present-focused time orientation 

(Bishop et al., 2004). However, the definitions and applications of mindfulness vary widely, 

ranging from the philosophy and practices of Buddhism (Chiesa & Malinowski, 2011), to the 

clinical sciences and therapies (Kabat-Zinn, 2005; Rothwell, 2006; Teasdale, Segal, Williams, 

Ridgeway, Soulsby, & Lau, 2000), to cognitive and emotional research (Brown & Langer, 1990; 

Carson & Langer, 2006; Erisman & Roemer, 2010), to educational research and interventions 

(Langer, Hatem, Joss, & Howell, 1989), social and personality research (Langer, 1997; Langer, 

Bashner, & Chanowitz, 1985; Langer & Moldoveanu, 2000; Sternberg, 2000), and to positive 

psychology (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Jacob & Brinkerhoff, 1999).  

A recent definition of mindfulness is:  

a process of regulating attention in order to bring a quality of non-elaborative 

awareness to current experience and a quality of relating to one’s experience 

within an orientation of curiosity, experiential openness, and acceptance< a 

http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/


Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 151 

process of gaining insight into the nature of one’s mind<‛ (Bishop et al., 2004, p. 

234).  

We agree with Bishop et al.’s definition of mindfulness as a process. However, even though an 

impressive amount of research in recent years has helped to elucidate mindfulness in the clinic, 

there remains a paucity of evidence that helps to elucidate everyday mindfulness, such as 

mindfulness that occurs during positive experiences.  

In tandem with efforts to clarify the construct of mindfulness in clinical settings, several 

new measures of mindfulness have emerged, many of them within the past decade. For 

example, these include the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (Buchheld, Grossman, & Walach, 

2001), the Mindful Attention/Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003), the MAAS for 

adolescents (Brown, West, Loverich, & Biegel, 2011), the modified MAAS (Höfling, 

Moosbrugger, Schermelleh-Engel, & Heidenreich, 2011), the Kentucky Inventory of 

Mindfulness Skills (Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004), the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-

Revised (Feldman, Hayes, Kumar, Greeson, & Laurenceau, 2007), and the Toronto Mindfulness 

Scale (Lau et al., 2006), to name a few. These scales have framed mindfulness primarily in terms 

of Buddhist and clinical conceptualizations of mindfulness, examined mindfulness as a 

unidimensional trait, and as a hierarchical model (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 

2006). 

Unique to the present research we grounded our thinking about mindfulness in Langer’s 

social cognitive theory of mindfulness (1992, 1997). We appreciate the integrative approaches 

taken by researchers of mindfulness assessment in the clinic and therapeutic settings; however, 

in the present research we purposefully narrowed our scope for the sake of testing theory-

driven hypotheses about the core characteristics of state mindfulness in the context of pleasant, 

enjoyable experiences. Next, we acknowledge that Bodner and Langer (2001) proposed and 

supported the Mindfulness/Mindless Scale (MMS), designed according to Langer’s theory. 

However, they not only examined trait mindfulness, as many other scales have done, but also 

their data supported a one-factor model. Indeed, recent work by Haigh, Moore, Kashdan, and 

Fresco (2011) found support for a single-factor model of a brief version of the MMS. The 

present research extends Langer’s theory of mindfulness to positive experiences, and views 

mindfulness as a state rather than a trait. Additionally, as we detail later, we expected positive 

state mindfulness to include multiple related but distinct dimensions. 

Our approach is consistent with the notion of ‚everyday mindfulness‛ suggested by 

Thompson and Waltz (2007). That we rooted our model in positive experiences reveals that our 

conceptual basis is essentially consistent with that of clinical psychology. For instance, we 

garnered support for the idea that pleasant and enjoyable engagement oftentimes includes 

attention that waxes mindful and, as a state, mindfulness wanes as an experience transpires. 

Our goal was to capture this empirically, using a variety of statistical and experimental 

techniques. We do not suggest that all positive experiences necessarily involve mindful states; 

rather, we suggest that some positive experiences do.  

This view is conceptually similar to the mindfulness championed in the clinical literatures, 

namely, that the presence of maladaptive cognitive, emotional and behavioral patterns suggests 

the absence of mindfulness. Techniques that build mindfulness skills are desirable. Hence, by 

extension we believe that individuals who exhibit adaptive cognitive, emotional and behavioral 

patterns will sometimes evince the presence of mindfulness spontaneously in their everyday 

positive experiences. 

Related to Langer and her colleagues’ descriptions of mindfulness (1992, 1997, 2000), in the 

present research we posited and tested a measurement model of mindfulness in relation to 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 152 

positive experience, and emphasize the state-like nature of mindfulness rather than 

mindfulness as a disposition. Shared among all relevant conceptualizations of mindfulness, we 

adhere to the idea that mindfulness is a way of paying attention flexibly to what is occurring in 

the moment and accepting that moment as it is, without judging it, or the self, critically. By 

focusing on Langer and her colleagues' conceptualization of mindfulness, and by narrowing 

our focus to positive experiences, the present research fills important conceptual, psychometric 

and practical gaps in the literature. Additionally, in narrowing our conceptualization of 

mindfulness to encompass positive state mindfulness, we tend to avoid references to 

mindlessness. We begin with a brief review of Langer's mindfulness theory, and then later in 

Study 1 we operationalize each of the dimensions that are derived from this theory. 

 

2. Review of mindfulness 

Langer’s (1992) early conceptualization included mindfulness as a ‚state of conscious 

awareness<openness to novelty in which the individual actively constructs categories and 

distinctions‛ (p. 289). From this view mindfulness is a somewhat effortful way of attending to 

the present moment, in contrast to the automatic, shallow processing of mindlessness. 

Together, both are akin to many dual-process models in social psychology (Chaiken & Trope, 

1999).  

In a later conceptualization, Langer and Moldoveanu (2000) revised the definition of 

mindfulness to emphasize that it is a ‚process of drawing novel distinctions,‛ such that a 

perceiver experiences:  

a greater sensitivity to one’s environment, more openness to new information, 

the creation of new categories for structuring perception, and enhanced 

awareness of multiple perspectives in problem solving (Langer & Moldoveanu, 

2000, p. 1). 

Consistent with this view, we conceptualized mindfulness in the context of positive experiences 

as more than merely the absence of mindlessness. In theory, mindfulness may be engaged, 

sustained, and cultivated by the development and rehearsal of distinct perceptual, cognitive 

and meta-cognitive skills. On the one hand, these skills may help a person to suppress, inhibit, 

or eliminate maladaptive habits, heuristics, and automatic thoughts (mindlessness). On the 

other hand, mindfulness also includes other skills that foster the mobilization, sustainment, and 

enhancement of adaptive habits, and controlled processing of information. The latter were the 

primary focus of the present research. 

Extending Langer and Moldoveanu’s (2000) conception, Sternberg (2000) proposed the 

following central components of mindfulness:  

a) openness to novelty; b) alertness to distinction; c) sensitivity to different 

contexts; d) implicit, if not explicit, awareness of multiple perspectives; and e) 

orientation in the present (Sternberg, 2000, p. 12). 

Sternberg also emphasized the importance of conceptualizing and measuring mindfulness as a 

state. His summary of Langer's mindfulness theory, and of the theory’s importance to future 

research, was optimistic:  

To the extent that this state [mindfulness] can be measured successfully, such 

measurement will be a valuable contribution to our understanding of people’s 

interactions with the context in which they live (Sternberg, 2000, p. 18). 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 153 

We agree with this conclusion. Hence, the present research attempted to garner support for the 

idea that state mindfulness is estimable in self-report form, at least indirectly, and can be 

surmised from positive experiences. 

In emphasizing the utility of mindfulness as a psychological construct, Brown and Ryan 

(2003) suggested that mindfulness is a useful concept, because it ‚<may be important in 

disentangling individuals from automatic thoughts, habits, and unhealthy behavior patterns" 

(Brown & Ryan, 2003, p. 823). We concur with Brown and Ryan; however, despite their 

endorsement of mindfulness, our review of the published literature on mindfulness leads us to 

conclude at present that mindfulness researchers continue to partially agree on how 

mindfulness should be conceptualized and measured. Such partial agreement has occurred far 

more in the realm of clinical psychology than in the realm of positive psychology. Furthermore, 

researchers have not yet tested Langer's mindfulness theory in the context of positive 

experiences. Hence, the present research is novel in this regard.  

Also, while the bulk of the extant public literature on mindfulness suggests that 

mindfulness is good, in general, such research has mainly framed mindfulness as something 

lacking; and hence, research is lacking from the view that mindfulness occurs spontaneously, 

especially during pleasant and enjoyable activities. Therefore, to address these important 

conceptual, experiential and measurement gaps in the literature, we tested systematically 

hypotheses that were derived from Langer’s social cognitive theory: mindfulness is a state; it is 

multidimensional; and sometimes occurs spontaneously during positive experiences. 

 

3. Positive mindfulness 

The notion of mindfulness in everyday experience is at the heart of the concept of savoring, a 

process through which people regulate and appreciate positive experience (Bryant, 1989, 2003; 

Bryant & Veroff, 2007). Savoring involves people’s ‚capacities to attend to, appreciate, and 

enhance the positive experiences in their lives‛ (Bryant & Veroff, 2007, p. 2). Thus, savoring 

entails a mindfulness of positive thoughts and feelings in relation to either the past 

(reminiscence), the present (savoring the moment), or the future (anticipation). Further, the 

capacity to attend to positive experience is a fundamental aspect of savoring. For this reason, in 

the present research, we purposefully focus on the construct of ‚positive state mindfulness.‛  

We suggest that positive state mindfulness is central to the notion of savoring the moment. 

Unless one notices and is mindfully aware of an ongoing positive experience, one cannot savor 

it. However, just because one is mindfully aware of an ongoing positive experience does not 

guarantee that one will savor it. Thus, we predict that states of positive mindfulness will be 

positively related to self-reported savoring capacity, though not so strongly as to be equivalent. 

Such theorizing is consistent with recent research that established moderate positive 

correlations between trait mindfulness and positive affect, curiosity, and emotion regulation 

(Haigh et al., 2011). 

Conceptually, it is important to clarify the relationship of temporal orientation to positive 

mindfulness and savoring, both of which fundamentally entail a present-oriented focus. Just as 

mindfulness involves a person’s attention oriented to the present (Farb et al., 2007), so also is 

savoring in the moment, in the here and now (Bryant & Veroff, 2007, p. 8). Yet, it is also 

possible for people to savor memories from the past or anticipate pleasurable experiences. In 

such cases, however, people may not necessarily remember past feelings or imagine future 

feelings, but rather are savoring in the present the way they feel when they think about the past 

or the future (Bryant & Veroff, 2007; Ritchie, Skowronski, Hartnett, Wells, & Walker, 2009; 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 154 

Ritchie, Skowronski, Wood, Walker, Vogl, & Gibbons, 2006). Thus, like mindfulness, savoring 

always involves some form of conscious present-focused attention on positive experience. 

 

4. Research hypotheses 

4.1 The dimensionality of positive mindfulness  

In considering a person’s momentary awareness during a positive experience, the question of 

whether positive state mindfulness is unidimensional or multidimensional arises. Although 

Langer’s theoretical framework highlights multiple conceptual components that should each be 

present at varying levels when people are being mindful, mindfulness has often been assessed 

as a unitary phenomenon, in terms of a total score, with no distinction made among different 

dimensions of mindfulness (Baer et al., 2006; Bodner & Langer, 2001). Therefore, one purpose of 

the present research was to test directly the hypothesis that positive mindfulness is 

multidimensional rather than unidimensional. 

 

4.2 Construct validation  

A second goal of the present research was to assess the convergent and discriminant validity of 

dimensions of positive state mindfulness in connection with related state and trait constructs, 

following validation guidelines proposed by Bryant and colleagues (Bryant, 2000; Bryant et al., 

2006). This work involved examining nomological patterns of correlations between our 

measures of positive mindfulness and measures of other theoretically relevant constructs, 

including trait mindfulness, perceived savoring capacity, need for cognition, trait absorption, 

uncertainty tolerance, and need for structure.  

In assessing convergent validity, we expected that states of positive mindfulness would be 

positively correlated with related constructs, such as perceived savoring capacity, trait 

absorption, and uncertainty tolerance, with which they share common conceptual elements. In 

assessing discriminant validity, we expected positive mindfulness to be distinct from the need 

for cognition and need for structure; and, we expected that different dimensions of positive 

mindfulness would evince different patterns of association with the set of validation criterion 

measures. Based on our position that positive state mindfulness is distinct from other notions of 

mindfulness, state and trait mindfulness are different constructs. Mindfulness is not simply the 

absence of mindlessness. We also expected positive state mindfulness to share relatively little 

variance with measures of general trait mindlessness. 

 

5. Study 1 

Guided by Langer's (1992, 1997) conceptualizations of mindfulness, we identified the following 

five theoretical dimensions presumed to underlie states of positive mindfulness: a) Focused 

Attention, b) Openness to Novelty, c) Novelty Appreciation, d) Categorization and Distinction-

making, and e) Open-Ended Expectations. The top panel of Figure 1 illustrates this model. To 

explore the conceptual structure and validity of self-reports related to Langer's theory of 

mindfulness, we tethered mindfulness items to a pleasant autobiographical event. The present 

study thus conceptualized mindfulness as a state that, in theory, can be recalled and described 

as part of an autobiographical episode or experience. Below we briefly describe each 

hypothesized component of positive mindfulness. 

  



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 155 

Figure 1. Conceptualizations of mindfulness 

 
Note: The top panel illustrates the initial model of positive state mindfulness, five correlated factors 

predicted from Langer’s theory; the bottom panel illustrates our refined, three correlated factor model of 

positive state mindfulness. 

 

5.1 Method 

5.1.1 Item construction 

We developed self-report items following procedures recommended by DeVellis (1993). We 

began by generating an initial list of 37 items, each intended to reflect one of the five 

characteristics of mindfulness that constitute Langer’s theoretical model. All items were 

prefaced with the phrase: ‚During my positive experience<.‛. Responses to each item were set 

to a six-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = disagree somewhat, 4 = agree 

somewhat, 5 = agree, and 6 = strongly agree).1 Rather than clustering items and presenting them in 

their intended conceptual categories, instead we randomly ordered items for administration. 

Focused attention. Consistent with Langer's and many others’ conceptualization of 

mindfulness, we hypothesized that focused attention on the present moment is central to 

mindfulness. Accordingly, we generated a set of items designed to tap the extent to which 

respondents remembered experiencing a heightened focus of attention to the present moment 

during a recent positive event. We interpreted Langer and others’ descriptions of a person who 

is mindfully focused as someone whose attention becomes immersed and absorbed in the 

present moment, akin to but not necessarily the same as, psychological absorption (Tellegen & 

Atkinson, 1974; Wild, Kuiken, & Schopflocher, 1995). A sample item that we created for this 

dimension is: ‚<my attention was focused on what was happening just then.‛ 

                                                
1 The initial 37 items are available from the first author; see Table 2 for a list of final items and the Appendix for further details.  
 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 156 

Openness to novelty. In theory, mindfulness could be indexed through a person’s memory 

for having been cognitively receptive to novel information. Consistent with this prevailing 

theoretical notion, we generated a second set of items intended to reflect openness to novelty. 

The central theme to these items involves being open to learning and being receptive to new 

ideas. If a person’s readiness to perceive novelty reflects mindfulness, then self-reports to such 

items ought to reflect this component of mindfulness. A sample item for this dimension is: ‚< I 

felt open to experience new things.‛  

Novelty appreciation. Another dimension central in many conceptual models of mindfulness 

is a pleasurable appreciation of perceived novelty. We hypothesized that the state of being 

receptive to new experiences is distinct from the state of appreciating novelty. Accordingly, we 

wrote a third set of items intended to reflect the extent to which respondents appreciated a 

sense of novelty during a recent positive episode. A sample item for this dimension is: ‚< I 

enjoyed the newness of the moment.‛ In this item, mindfulness is represented as recalled 

enjoyment of the novelty that a person perceived during a recent pleasant event. We wrote 

separate sets of items reflecting openness to and appreciation of novelty because theory 

suggests both dimensions are related but distinct. 

Categorization and distinction-making. When conceptualizing mindfulness, Langer (1992, 

1997) referred to a dimension involving distinction-making, categorization, and the creation of 

new categories to guide perception. To reflect this particular dimension, we wrote nine items 

that represented perceptions about categorization or about distinction-making. A sample item 

for this hybrid dimension is: ‚<I compared different things that were happening‛. We 

designed these items to tap the extent to which respondents recalled noticing how they 

evaluated, appraised, and judged their perceptions during a recent positive experience. 

Open-ended expectations. Consistent with Langer and her colleagues' research (Langer & 

Moldoveanu, 2000), we generated items intended to reflect a fifth component of mindfulness 

representing a person's experience of momentary uncertainty and self-reported degree of open-

ended situational expectations. As Langer (1997) stated, ‚From a mindful perspective< 

uncertainty creates freedom to discover meaning...if there are meaningful choices, there is 

uncertainty‛ (p. 130). To assess this dimension, we constructed four items. A sample item for 

this dimension is: ‚<I felt like anything could happen from one moment to the next.‛ 

 

5.1.2 Participants 

Study 1 included two samples. We used sample 1 to test and refine a measurement model for 

the set of positive mindfulness items. We split sample 2 in half randomly for use in doubly 

cross-validating model modifications based on the first sample. 

Sample 1 consisted of 586 undergraduate students enrolled in different sections of an 

Introductory Psychology course at Loyola University Chicago. Reflecting the population of 

students in this course, the gender composition was 397 females (68%), 186 males (32%), and 

three respondents who did not indicate their gender (<1%). The mean sample age was 19.0 

years (SD = 3.0; mode = 18). The ethnic composition was Caucasian (63%), Asian (22%), Latin 

(10%), Black (4%), and unspecified (1%). The final sample consisted of 569 respondents who 

provided complete data for all 37 items. 

Sample 2 consisted of 415 undergraduate students enrolled in different sections of an 

Introductory Psychology courses at the same university. Prior to analyses, the data were split 

into two sub-samples—sub-sample 2A (na = 208) and sub-sample 2B (nb = 207)—according to 

random sampling, stratifying on gender. Sub-sample 2A consisted of 162 females (78%) and 46 

males (22%). Sub-sample 2B consisted of 161 females (78%), 45 males (22%), and one 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 157 

respondent (<1%) who did not report gender. Each random sub-sample included a sufficient 

number of observations for measurement modeling.2 

 

5.1.3 Procedure 

Participants volunteered to complete the research materials in scheduled sessions. They were 

instructed first to reflect on the past week, and then to describe briefly in writing a positive 

experience that had occurred during the past week. To permit freedom in selecting positive 

experiences, we prompted respondents to choose what they deemed to be a positive 

experience, rather than asking them to report a particular kind of positive experience or asking 

them to choose from a list of pre-selected events and activities. We intended to elicit relatively 

ordinary positive experiences rather than momentous, peak experiences. We also wanted to 

hold constant the recent time frame; hence, we instructed respondents to remember as far back 

as only one week. After responding to the instrument, participants were debriefed, provided an 

explanation of the objectives of our study, thanked, and dismissed. 

 

5.1.4 Analytic strategy 

Using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). In Study 1, we followed three common analytic 

strategies used in CFA (Bryant et al., 2006; Jöreskog, 1993). According to Jöreskog (1993), 

researchers who formulate and then test a single measurement model a priori employ a strictly 

confirmatory (SC) approach. We used an SC approach to test a priori measurement models 

using four independent samples. Jöreskog also referred to a second analytic strategy, model 

generating (MG), in which the researcher posits a tentative initial model, and then uses 

statistical and theoretical criteria to modify and retest post hoc refinements to improve the 

model’s fit to the data. In the MG scenario, the model to be tested is tentative and not fully 

established at the outset. Consistent with this approach, we committed one full sample in 

implementing an MG strategy, adopting both theoretical and empirical criteria in model 

development. We also adopted a third analytic approach that Jöreskog (1993) termed the 

alternative models (AM) strategy. Adopting this AM approach in four independent samples, 

we tested the goodness-of-fit of an alternative one-factor model versus our final 

multidimensional measurement model of positive mindfulness.3 

Assessing model fit. We used five different statistical criteria to judge the goodness-of-fit of 

each CFA model: the goodness-of-fit index (GFI), the non-normed fit index (NNFI), the 

comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the 

standardized root mean residual (SRMR). Analogous to R2 in multiple regression, GFI reflects 

the proportion of available variance-covariance information in the data that the given model 

explains, with larger GFI values representing better model fit. NNFI and CFI indicate how 

much better the given model fits the data relative to a ‚null‛ model, which assumes sampling 

error alone explains covariation among observed measures (i.e., no common variance exists 

among measured variables). Bentler and Bonett (1980) recommended that measurement models 

have GFI, NNCI, and CFI of at least .90. RMSEA reflects the size of the residuals that result 

when using the model to predict the data, adjusting for model complexity, with smaller values 

indicating better fit. According to Browne and Cudeck (1993), RMSEA < .05 represents ‚close 

                                                
2 With this sample, our final nine-item three-factor model included 21 estimated parameters: nine factor loadings, nine unique 

variances, and three factor correlations. Each random sample thus provided roughly 10 observations per estimated model 

parameter (na: 208 / 21 = 9.90; nb: 207 / 21 = 9.86). 
3 We followed a similar model-testing strategy using exploratory factor analysis with principal axis factor extraction and a promax 

(oblique) rotation method, and found similar results. They are available upon request from the first author.  



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 158 

fit;‛ RMSEA between .05 and .08 represents ‚reasonably close fit;‛ and RMSEA > .10 represents 

‚an unacceptable model.‛ SRMR reflects the average standardized absolute value of the 

difference between the elements of the observed covariance matrix and the covariance matrix 

elements implied by the given model, with smaller values indicating better fit. Hu and Bentler 

(1998) suggested that SRMR <.08 represents acceptable model fit. Finally, we used the 

difference in degrees of freedom and chi-square values (i.e., the likelihood ratio test; Bollen, 

1989), to test hypotheses about differences in goodness-of-fit between nested CFA models. 

 

5.1.5 Hypotheses 

Following the SC strategy, we initially specified a five-factor mindfulness model (Focused 

Attention, Openness to Novelty, Novelty Appreciation, Categorization and Distinction-

Making, Open-Ended Expectations). However, we acknowledged the possibility that the full 

five-factor model might be too broad to tap the state mindfulness associated with a recalled 

positive experience and might need to be reduced to a smaller set of core subscales. We also 

expected that some items from the initial pool would need to be eliminated because of 

redundancy or low reliability. 

Following the MG strategy, we expected to re-specify the initial measurement model if it 

achieved inadequate goodness-of-fit, using both theoretical and empirical criteria during model 

refinement. We hypothesized that our more parsimonious measurement model would fit the 

data better than both the initial five-factor and alternative one-factor models. 

Following the AM strategy, we used data from multiple independent samples to test the 

hypothesis that the more parsimonious measurement model would again fit the data better 

than a one-factor model, and would generalize across independent samples. All of the 

structural analyses in Study 1 were aimed at developing and confirming a theory-grounded 

measurement model, whose construct validity would be assessed in Study 2. 

 

5.2 Results and discussion 

5.2.1 Strictly confirmatory: Testing the a priori measurement model 

Initial screening of the pool of items suggested they were reasonably normally distributed. To 

test the first hypothesis, we subjected the initial 37-item, oblique five-factor model to 

maximum-likelihood CFA using LISREL 8.54 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996) to analyze the 

covariance matrix. Not surprisingly, we found that the initial full model fit the data 

unsatisfactorily, χ2 (619, N = 569) = 2551.03, GFI = .77, NNFI = .71, CFI = .73, RMSEA = .08, SRMR 

= .08 (see Table 1). Thus, the data did not confirm the a priori five-factor model for the initial 

pool of 37 items. Accordingly, we adopted the MG strategy and tested a series of modified 

measurement models for the purpose of model refinement. 

 

5.2.2 Model generating: Refining the initial factor structure 

We used established measurement modeling procedures (Bryant, King, & Smart, 2006) to refine 

this initial model by iteratively eliminating items with squared multiple correlations < .30 and 

combining factors that were highly inter-correlated (i.e., r >.90), while simultaneously requiring 

factors to have acceptable reliabilities (i.e., Cronbach’s α >.70). This approach yielded: a) first, a 

five-factor model for 20 items that fit the data better but not acceptably; b) then, a three-factor 

model for 20 items that still fell short of acceptable fit; and, c) eventually a three-factor model 

for 9 items that fit the data well (see Table 1). The final refined model consisted of three 

factors—Novelty Appreciation, Focused Attention, and Open-Ended Expectations—each with  



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 159 

Table 1. Results from confirmatory factor analyses of the positive state mindfulness 

measurement models 

Strategy Model N # items χ2 df GFI NNFI CFI RMSEA SRMR 

SC 5-factor 569 37 2551.03 619 .77 .71 .73 .084 .081 

MG 5-factor 569 20 803.63 160 .87 .79 .82 .088 .068 

MG 3-factor 569 20 864.89 167 .86 .78 .81 .089 .071 

MGa 3-factor 569 9 103.82 24 .96 .91 .94 .076 .059 

AM 1-factor 569 9 534.62 27 .81 .50 .62 .193 .115 

SCa 3-factor 208 9 74.48 24 .93 .90 .93 .097 .062 

SCa 3-factor 207 9 71.52 24 .93 .87 .91 .095 .066 

SCa 3-factor 415 9 114.87 24 .94 .91 .93 .095 .060 

AM 1-factor 415 9 397.58 27 .80 .68 .70 .199 .109 

SCa 3-factor 239 9 101.10 24 .91 .87 .90 .116 .071 

AM 1-factor 239 9 318.76 27 .76 .60 .61 .220 .134 

SCa 3-factor 1361 9 252.91 24 .96 .93 .94 .085 .052 

AM 1-factor 1361 9 1215.13 27 .81 .67 .67 .194 .109 

a Our refined final model. 

Note. SC = Strictly Confirmatory analysis, MG = Model Generating analysis, AM = Alternative Models 

analysis. GFI = Goodness of Fit Index. NNFI = Non-Normed Fit Index. CFI = Comparative Fit Index. 

RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation. SRMR = Standardized Root Mean Square Residual. 

Results obtained using LISREL 8.54 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996). 

 

three items. The bottom panel of Figure 1 illustrates this model. The resulting measurement 

model provided an acceptable goodness-of-fit to the data: χ2 (24, N = 569) = 103.82, GFI = .96, 

NNFI = .91, CFI = .94, RMSEA = .08, SRMR = .06. 

 

5.2.3 Testing alternative models: Is positive mindfulness multidimensional? 

We next tested whether this multidimensional model explained responses to the nine items 

better than a unidimensional model. As hypothesized, a one-factor model provided a poor fit to 

the data: χ2 (27, N = 569) = 534.62, GFI = .81, NNFI = .50, CFI = .62, RMSEA = .19, SRMR = .11. 

Thus, the three-factor model fit the data better than a one-factor model, Δχ2 (3, N = 569) = 430.80, 

p <.0001. These results indicate that positive mindfulness is multidimensional and reflects at 

least three basic dimensions.  

 

5.2.4 Strictly confirmatory: Testing the cross-sample generalizability of the refined model 

The next set of analyses assessed how well the goodness-of-fit of the nine-item three-factor 

model replicated across two independent samples (Ns = 207 and 208). We ran identical analyses 

for each independent sample, and for the pooled data from both samples combined (N = 415). 

Results revealed that the refined three-factor model fit the data of each independent sample 

adequately: χ2 (24, N = 207) = 74.48, GFI = .93, NNFI = .90, CFI = .93, RMSEA = .09, SRMR = .06; 

and, χ2 (24, N = 208) = 71.52, GFI = .93, NNFI = .87, CFI = .91, RMSEA = .09, SRMR = .06, 

respectively. 

Assessing the cross-sample generalizability of the refined three-factor model, additional 

tests of factorial invariance using multi-group CFA (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996) revealed that the 

factor loadings, factor variances-covariances, and unique variances of the three-factor model 

were equivalent for the two independent samples, χ2 (21, N = 415) = 30.83, p < .08. In addition, 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 160 

the three-factor model provided an acceptable goodness-of-fit to the data of the two 

independent samples combined, χ2 (24, N = 415) = 114.87, GFI = .94, NNFI = .91, CFI = .92, 

RMSEA = .09, SRMR = .06 (see Table 1). These findings strongly confirm the cross-sample 

generalizability of the three-factor CFA model. 

 

5.2.5 Testing alternative models 

In contrast, a unidimensional one-factor model fit the pooled data relatively poorly: χ2 (27, N = 

415) = 397.58, GFI = .80, NNFI = .68, CFI = .70, RMSEA = .20, SRMR = .10, and provided a 

significantly worse fit compared to the three-factor model, Δχ2 (3, N = 415) = 282.71, p < 

.0001. Thus, the data suggest that the intended three-factor measurement model provides a 

reasonable representation of state mindfulness in relation to positive experience. This refined 

three-factor model incorporates the a priori mindfulness dimensions of Novelty Appreciation, 

Focused Attention, and Open-Ended Expectations as originally hypothesized. However, the 

refined three-factor model excludes the initial mindfulness dimensions of Categorization and 

Distinction-Making and Openness to Novelty. Table 2 displays the completely standardized 

factor loadings from the confirmatory factor analyses. Table 3 displays descriptive statistics and 

reliability estimates for the refined model. 

 

Table 2. CFA factor loadings for the nine-item, three-factor positive state mindfulness model 

  Sample 

Factor Items 1 2 3 4 

Focused Attention My attention was focused on what 

was happening just then. 
.67 .65 .64 .66 

 My attention was absorbed in the 

moment. 
.78 .73 .70 .84 

 I was aware of nothing else except 

what I was going through right 

then. 

.65 .62 .69 .72 

Novelty Appreciation I enjoyed the newness of the 

moment. 
.68 .71 .78 .62 

 I took great pleasure in 

experiencing new things. 
.75 .80 .76 .79 

 Everything happening seemed 

unique to me. 
.63 .62 .57 .73 

Open-Ended Expectations I wasn't sure of what to expect. .74 .77 .75 .72 

 
I couldn't tell how things were 

going to unfold. 
.79 .80 .77 .82 

 
I felt like anything could happen 

from one moment to the next. 
.64 .70 .58 .66 

Note: CFA = confirmatory factor analysis. Tabled above are completely standardized factor loadings 

from maximum-likelihood CFA via LISREL 8.54 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996). Sample 1: pooled N = 1361. 

Sample 2: N = 569. Sample 3: N = 415. Sample 4: N = 239. One smaller sample (n = 126) was not separately 

subjected to confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) but was included in the pooled Sample 1. 

  



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 161 

Table 3. Descriptive Statistics for the Positive State Mindfulness Scales 

Sub-scale N αa Mean SD Min. Max. 

Focused Attention 

(3 items) 583 .71 4.75 0.98 1.00 6.00 

 415 .73 4.84 0.99 1.00 6.00 

 239 .77 4.97 0.88 2.00 6.00 

 126 .79 4.76 1.04 1.00 6.00 

 1363 .73 4.82 0.97 1.00 6.00 

Novelty Appreciation 

(3 items) 582 .73 4.60 1.02 1.00 6.00 

 415 .70 4.41 1.07 1.00 6.00 

 239 .75 4.51 1.10 1.33 6.00 

 126 .74 4.32 1.11 1.00 6.00 

 1362 .72 4.50 1.06 1.00 6.00 

Open-Ended Expectations 

(3 items) 585 .73 3.78 1.20 1.00 6.00 

 415 .80 3.77 1.28 1.00 6.00 

 239 .77 3.78 1.28 1.00 6.00 

 126 .76 3.87 1.27 1.00 6.00 

 1365 .76 3.78 1.24 1.00 6.00 

a Cronbach’s alpha, an index of item internal consistency. 

 

Additionally, to ascertain the unique proportion of variance explained by a second-order factor 

and unique variance explained by each of the three first-order factors (N = 415), a Schmid-

Leiman solution was examined to assess the proportion of variance explained by a higher-order 

factor compared to the proportion of variance explained by each of the three hypothesized 

factors. Results from Wolff and Preising’s (2005) macro for SAS, version 9.1 suggest that the 

second-order factor (i.e., recalled positive mindfulness) accounted for only 19.4% of the 

variance and the three factors jointly accounted for 80.6% (Focused Attention = 34%, Novelty 

Appreciation = 26.4%, and Open-Ended Expectations = 20.2%). These results converge with the 

previous findings that suggest a multidimensional model is a better fit with the data than a 

unidimensional or higher-order factor model. 

 

5.2.6 Correlations among dimensions of mindfulness 

Finally, we used equality constraints via CFA (Bryant et al., 2006) to directly compare 

correlations among the three dimensions, using the data of the pooled independent samples (N 

= 415). These analyses revealed that: a) Focused Attention was equally correlated with Novelty 

Appreciation (r = .61) and Open-Ended Expectations (r = .67), Δχ2(1) = 0.70, p < .41; b) Novelty 

Appreciation was more correlated with Focused Attention (r = .61) than it was with Open-

Ended Expectations (r = .36), Δχ2(1) = 17.10, p <.0001; and, c) Open-Ended Expectations had 

more to do with Focused Attention (r = .67) than with Novelty Appreciation (r = .36), Δχ2(1) = 

26.05, p <.0001. These results lend initial support to the divergent validity of the three 

dimensions that compose our refined measurement model. 

  



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 162 

6. Study 2 

In Study 1 we developed and confirmed a multidimensional factor model of recalled state 

mindfulness during a recent pleasant experience, and we established its structural validity 

using confirmatory factor analytic techniques. In Study 2, we collected and analyzed two 

additional data sets for the purpose of testing the construct validity of each of the three 

mindfulness components with existing constructs. 

The goal of the second study was to investigate further our refined nine-item three-factor 

model of positive mindfulness, and extend our construct validation efforts to include more 

systematic tests of the model’s convergent, discriminant, and nomological validity. Toward this 

goal, we widened the conceptual net by administering the refined positive mindfulness 

measurement instrument along with a) existing measures of trait mindfulness, b) scales 

conceptually related to mindfulness, and c) scales conceptually distinct from mindfulness. 

Thus, in addition to administering our refined three-factor measure, we administered two 

other self-report measures of mindfulness: the dispositional Mindfulness/Mindlessness Scale 

(Bodner & Langer, 2001), and the dispositional Mindful Attention/Awareness Scale (Brown & 

Ryan, 2003). Here, we sought to identify points of conceptual convergence and divergence 

between our state measure of positive mindfulness and trait measures of general mindfulness. 

In addition, we assessed five other constructs: a) savoring beliefs assessed in terms of the 

perceived capacity to savor positive experiences through anticipation, through appreciating the 

moment, and through reminiscence; b) trait absorption; c) need for cognition; d) uncertainty 

tolerance; and, e) personal need for structure.  

As a global test of discriminant validity, we hypothesized that each of the three dimensions 

of positive mindfulness would show a different pattern of associations with the set of criterion 

measures. As a global test of convergent validity, we hypothesized that trait absorption would 

be positively related to all three dimensions of positive mindfulness and that the need for 

structure would be negatively correlated with all three dimensions of positive mindfulness. We 

hypothesized that: a) all three temporal forms of savoring beliefs would be positively related to 

the mindful appreciation of novelty and to positive focused attention, but less strongly related 

to open-ended expectations; b) the capacity to savor positive experiences prospectively through 

anticipation would be negatively related to open-ended expectations; and c) tolerance of 

uncertainty would be positively related to the appreciation of novelty and open-ended 

expectations, but less strongly related to positive temporal focus. Below we provide more 

specific theoretical foundations for our hypotheses concerning each of these criterion measures. 

 

6.1 Method 

6.1.1 Participants and procedure 

Sample 3 (N = 239) included 165 females (69%) and 74 males (31%) with one respondent who 

did not report gender (<1%). Sample 4 (N = 126) included 54 women (42.9%), 33 men (26.2%), 

and 39 respondents (31%) for whom gender information was not collected. The survey 

administration procedure used in Study 2 was essentially the same as the procedure described 

in Study 1. The only change was that respondents in Study 2 needed more time to complete the 

additional validation measures we administered. To counteract any order effects, three 

different item orders were arranged and then randomly assigned to respondents. As noted 

below, not all instruments were administered to all respondents. 

 

  



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 163 

6.1.2 Measures 

MMS. We measured dispositional mindfulness, or the stable trait of proneness to mindfulness 

in Samples 3 and 4, using Bodner and Langer’s (2001) Mindfulness/Mindlessness Scale (MMS). 

The MMS consists of 21 items, each assessed by a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly 

agree, with middle point = neutral). The MMS composite score was reliable (Sample 3 

Cronbach’s α = .80, Sample 4 Cronbach’s α = .87). Bodner and Langer (2001) presented evidence 

suggesting that their scale taps into a person's self-reported characteristic propensity to be 

mindful. Even though the MMS is a trait measure and our instrument is a state measure, both 

measures are based on Langer’s conceptualization of mindfulness. Therefore, we hypothesized 

positive correlations between the MMS and at least one of our scales. 

MAAS. In Sample 4, we also measured dispositional mindfulness using the Mindful 

Attention/Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003), a 15-item unidimensional measure 

of trait mindfulness, with items assessed using a 6-point scale (1 = almost always to 6 = almost 

never). The MAAS is worded such that higher total scores suggest the absence of mindlessness 

in respondents’ lives (Sample 4 Cronbach’s α = .88). Given that the MAAS items were only 

indirectly based on Langer's conceptualization of mindfulness, and that the items tapped into 

the absence of mindlessness rather than the presence of mindfulness, we expected that the total 

score of this trait measure would not correlate positively with our three scales.  

SBI. Both Samples 3 and 4 completed the Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI; Bryant, 2003), a 

self-report measure assessing a person's perceived ability to enjoy positive events by: a) 

Anticipating the Future (Sample 3 Cronbach’s α = .72, Sample 4 Cronbach’s α = .81); b) Savoring 

the Moment (Sample 3 Cronbach’s α = .72, Sample 4 Cronbach’s α = .81); and, c) Reminiscing 

about the Past (Sample 3 Cronbach’s α = .83, Sample 4 Cronbach’s α = .81). Each scale has 8 

items (4 positively-worded, 4 negatively-worded) assessed using a 7-point scale (1 = strongly 

disagree to 7 = strongly agree). Given that savoring involves mindfulness of positive stimuli, we 

expected that our present-oriented Focused Attention subscale would correlate positively with 

the present-oriented Savoring the Moment subscale of the SBI. We also expected that Novelty 

Appreciation would positively correlate with Savoring the Moment. Based on the notion that 

anticipation requires a fixed positive expectation regarding the future, we expected that 

savoring through Anticipation would correlate negatively with the positive mindfulness 

dimension of Open-Ended Expectations.  

TAS. We assessed trait absorption in Sample 4 using Tellegen’s Absorption Scale (TAS; 

Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974), consisting of 34 items (Sample 4 Cronbach’s α = .79) assessed by a 

7-point Likert scale (0 = never to 7 = always). Although trait absorption has primarily been linked 

to hypnotic susceptibility, absorption is also associated with experiential involvement (Wild et 

al., 1995). Thus, we expected that our Focused Attention scale would correlate positively with 

the TAS, because both measures are present-time oriented and suggest sensory-perceptual and 

experiential engagement. 

UTS. We assessed individual differences in the capacity to tolerate situational uncertainty 

in Sample 4 using the Uncertainty Tolerance Scale (UTS; Dalbert, 1999), an 8-item measure of 

the extent to which a person tolerates spontaneity, surprise, and disrupted routines. The UTS 

contains both present- and future-focused items and uses a 6-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 

6 = strongly agree). The version we used was translated by C. Dalbert from German into English 

(using back-translation). UTS total score is used in our analyses (Sample 4 Cronbach’s α = .62). 

Although this reliability coefficient is somewhat low, we expected that UTS total score would 

correlate positively with each of our subscales. 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 164 

PNS. We measured individual differences in the need for routine and structure in Sample 4 

using the Personal Need for Structure Scale (PNS; Neuberg & Newsome, 1993), consisting of 12 

items each assessed by a 6-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree). A higher PNS 

total score suggests a greater need for simple structure in life (Sample 4 Cronbach’s α = .81). 

Because people less inclined to be spontaneous are likely to approach positive experiences with 

fixed plans and clear-cut future expectations, we would expect them to report lower levels of 

present-focused attention, appreciation of novelty, and open-ended expectations. From this 

view, individuals’ personal need for structure, in theory, should correlate negatively with all 

three of our components of mindfulness. 

NFC. Finally, we measured individual differences in the motivation to think and 

understand in Sample 4 using the 18-item version of the Need for Cognition scale (NFC; 

Cacioppo & Petty, 1982), with each item assessed using a 5-point scale (1 = extremely 

uncharacteristic to 5 = extremely characteristic). In Sample 4, Cronbach’s α for NFC was .87. We 

hypothesized that NFC has little to do with mindfulness in relation to positive situations, and 

hence would be uncorrelated with our subscales. 

 

6.2 Results and discussion 

We first computed unit-weighted average scores for each of our three subscales, along with 

scale and subscale scores for each of the criterion measures. We next used meta-analytic 

statistical methods to organize and simplify construct validation across the two samples. When 

the same constructs were assessed in both Sample 3 and 4, we calculated and reported an 

average r weighted by sample size (D-Stat 1.11; Johnson, 1995). When a bivariate correlation 

was assessed in only one sample, we reported the Pearson product-moment correlation. Table 4 

displays the correlations observed among our positive mindfulness subscales, the measures of 

trait mindfulness, and each of the criterion measures in Study 2. 

 

Table 4. Meta-analytic and bivariate correlations among positive state mindfulness scales, 

trait mindfulness scales and related constructs 

Measures N 
Focused 

Attention 

Novelty 

Appreciation 

Open-Ended 

Expectations 
MMS MAAS 

MMS 365a .07 .32** .15* -- -- 

MAAS 126 .04 .13 -.32** .25** -- 

SBI Anticipation 365a .30** .28** -.11 .32** .30* 

SBI Savor the Moment 365a .41** .27** -.08 .34** .46** 

SBI Reminiscence 365a .21** .17* -.19** .35** .49** 

TAS 126 .17* .15 .18* .30** -.29** 

UTS 126 .25** .20* .16 .36** .02 

PNS 126 -.19* -.14 -.10 -.31** -.18* 

NFC 126 .07 -.06 -.08 .61** .26** 

*p < .05. **p < .01. a Meta-analytic correlations. 

Note: MMS = Mindfulness/Mindlessness Scale (Bodner & Langer, 2001). MAAS = Mindful 

Attention/Awareness Scale (Brown & Ryan, 2003), higher scores reflect lower trait mindlessness. SBI = 

Savoring Beliefs Inventory (Bryant, 2003). TAS = Tellegen’s Absorption Scale (Tellegen & 

Atkinson, 1974). UTS = Uncertainty Tolerance Scale (Dalbert, 1999). PNS = Personal Need for Structure 

(Neuberg & Newsome, 1993). NFC = Need for Cognition (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982). 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 165 

In general, the validation results support the construct validity of our measure of mindfulness 

in relation to positive experience. Confirming hypotheses, each of our three subscales of 

positive mindfulness demonstrated a different pattern of correlations with the criterion 

measures. These results support the discriminant validity of the three positive mindfulness 

subscales. Below we consider the specific relations of each criterion measure with our subscales 

of positive mindfulness. 

 

6.2.1 Trait mindfulness/mindlessness 

As predicted, the data suggest that our positive state mindfulness subscales were more similar 

to the MMS than to the MAAS; both of the latter are trait measures of mindfulness. Supporting 

our hypotheses, Novelty Appreciation and Open-Ended Expectations each evidenced modest 

positive correlations with the MMS. Our items were based entirely on Langer’s 

conceptualization of mindfulness, which has consistently included the notions of seeking and 

producing novelty (i.e., novelty appreciation) and a tolerance for uncertainty (i.e., open-ended 

situational expectations). 

Unexpectedly, our Open-Ended Expectations subscale correlated negatively with the MAAS. 

What is it about being mindless that might actually predispose people to have more open-

ended expectations in positive situations? We speculate that people who are characteristically 

unaware of their surroundings tend to lack expectations about what will happen next in 

positive situations; that is, characteristically mindless individuals are typically unsure about 

what to expect, cannot tell how things are going to unfold, and feel like anything could happen 

from one moment to the next (i.e., the three items comprising our Open-Ended Expectations 

subscale). Although this clearly seems to be the case, it leads to the paradoxical conclusion that 

being characteristically mindless actually helps people avoid closed-ended expectations during 

positive experiences; being mindless, in general, predisposes a person to be mindful with 

respect to remaining open to unlimited possibilities in positive situations. This ironic 

interpretation not only supports the discriminant validity of our Open-Ended Expectations 

subscale, but also illustrates an important way in which mindfulness is not simply the opposite 

of mindlessness. Moreover, that trait mindlessness is associated with positive state 

mindfulness, specifically concerning open-ended expectations, demonstrates the importance of 

distinguishing general trait mindlessness from mindfulness in relation to positive situations. 

 

6.2.2 Savoring beliefs 

Concerning the criterion measures of savoring beliefs, the data confirmed the hypothesized 

pattern of correlations. Specifically, as hypothesized our Focused Attention and Novelty 

Appreciation subscales were positively correlated with the perceived capacity to savor positive 

experiences through anticipation, savoring the moment, and reminiscence (see Table 4). Also as 

predicted, Open-Ended Expectations correlated negatively with Anticipation, although this 

effect did not reach statistical significance. Contrary to predictions, however, Open-Ended 

Expectations correlated negatively with savoring through reminiscence (r = -.19, p < .01). This 

latter finding suggests that being better at savoring retrospectively predisposes people to be 

more certain about what to expect, how things will unfold, and what will happen from one 

moment to the next in ongoing positive situations. Given that stronger savoring beliefs via 

reminiscence are associated with more active memory-building during unfolding positive 

experiences (Bryant & Veroff, 2007), the tendency to savor through reminiscence may 

predispose people to be more closed-ended with respect to expectations in positive situations, 

so as to facilitate memory encoding for later recall. 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 166 

6.2.3 Absorption 

Further supporting our a priori hypotheses, the measure of trait absorption (TAS) correlated 

positively with two out of three of our subscales: Focused Attention and Open-Ended 

Expectations (see Table 4). These results suggest that individuals with a tendency to become 

absorbed in the moment also tended to report being mindful during a recent positive event. 

 

6.2.4 Uncertainty tolerance 

Also consistent with our hypotheses, Uncertainty Tolerance correlated positively with each of 

our subscales, though only two of these correlations (Focused Attention and Novelty 

Appreciation) were statistically significant. Individuals with a proclivity to respond favorably 

to surprise and who tolerate disruption in daily routines tend to report being mindful during a 

recent positive experience. 

 

6.2.5 Need for structure 

Further supporting our hypotheses, Personal Need for Structure correlated negatively with our 

subscales, though only one of these negative correlations, Focused Attention (r = -.19, p < .05), 

was statistically significant. Thus, the more need for structure, the less reported Focused 

Attention during recent positive experiences. Alternatively, the less strongly individuals 

endorsed a personal need for structure, the more strongly they reported attending to the 

present during a recent positive experience. 

 

6.2.6 Need for cognition 

Finally, supporting the discriminant validity of positive mindfulness, as hypothesized all three 

of our scales were uncorrelated with need for cognition (|rs| < .09, ns). The lack of a relation 

between NFC and state mindfulness suggests that the need for mental activity and mindfulness 

during positive activity are conceptually distinct. Notice, in contrast, that both of the trait 

measures of mindfulness—the MAAS (r = .26, p < .01) and MMS (r = .61, p < .01)—were 

correlated positively with need for cognition (see Table 4). These results provide further 

evidence of the discriminant validity of positive state mindfulness, as distinct from trait 

mindfulness/mindlessness. 

 

7. Study 3 

In Study 1 we developed and confirmed a multidimensional factor model of state mindfulness 

during a recent pleasant experience, and we established its structural validity using 

confirmatory factor analytic techniques. In Study 2, we collected and analyzed two additional 

data sets that garnered evidence of the construct validity of each of the three mindfulness 

components with existing constructs. The goal of Study 3 was to investigate further our refined 

nine-item, three-factor model of positive mindfulness, and extend our construct validation 

efforts to include more tests of the model’s convergent, discriminant, and nomological validity. 

Toward this goal, we widened the conceptual net by again administering the refined 

positive mindfulness scale along with scales that would help distinguish our three sub-scales 

from each other. We were concerned with the unique pattern of correlations between positive 

mindfulness and conceptually related pleasant memorial phenomena, such as a) nostalgia, b) a 

sense of purpose in life, c) self-esteem, d) self-continuity, and e) interpersonal connectedness. 

Research has established a solid link between nostalgia and subjective wellbeing (Hepper, 

Ritchie, Sedikides, & Wildschut, 2012; Wildschut, Sedikides, Arndt, & Routledge, 2006). As our 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 167 

notion of positive mindfulness is itself grounded in subjective wellbeing, we anticipated that 

our scales would correlate positively with nostalgia and its correlates. Additionally, we 

included items that assessed common features of positive events in memory, such as overall 

event rehearsal frequency, affect pleasantness at an event’s occurrence, and affect pleasantness 

at event recall (Ritchie et al., 2006; Ritchie et al., 2009; Walker, Skowronski, Gibbons, Vogl, & 

Ritchie, 2009).  

We were concerned with if, and the extent to which, our positive mindfulness scales 

correlated uniquely with event-related affect, such as the perceived affect felt at a positive 

event’s occurrence and the affect prompted by thinking about the event at recall. We reasoned 

that Novelty Appreciation, for example, could correlate more strongly with affect at recall than 

with affect at occurrence, in part because the pleasant affect could be used to broaden and build 

upon a person’s current skills that could be useful for emotion-regulation (Frederickson, Cohn, 

Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008). Finally, in relation to positive events, we wondered if people 

would report thinking about some aspects of mindfulness more often than others; hence, we 

examined the extent to which rehearsal frequency correlates positively with each dimension of 

positive mindfulness. 

 

7.1 Method 

7.1.1 Participants  

Participants were undergraduate students (N = 46) from the University of Limerick; half were 

women, half were men. Their average age was 21.8 years (SD = 5.82). Over 90% were Irish. 

 

7.1.2 Procedure 

Research Assistants contacted students via email using class rosters. Volunteers were emailed 

an informed consent form, the instructions and a spreadsheet file, in which they entered their 

autobiographical events and event ratings. Participants were prompted to recall and type a 

brief description of two positive autobiographical events: ‚Please recall a moment from your 

life that felt pleasant to you at the time.‛ Following this, they were prompted to simply describe 

‚What happened?‛ in a text space in the spreadsheet. They dated each of their events by 

indicating how old they were at the time the event occurred. Event age was computed by 

subtracting their ages at each event from their current age. Next, each returned their completed 

file to the research assistant, who then emailed each participant a debrief form, thanked them 

for participation and offered to deliver them a small token of appreciation for their 

contributions to the study. 

 

7.1.3 Measures 

Participants rated both of their positive events using a number of scales that have been used to 

study autobiographical event characteristics (Ritchie et al., 2006) and the functions of nostalgia 

(Wildschut et al., 2006). Each of the items that follow was prefaced with ‚Thinking about this 

event<‛ on which participants rated both of their events along 6-point Likert scales (strongly 

disagree = 1 to 6 = strongly agree), following the general instruction, ‚please indicate your 

agreement or disagreement by typing a number next to each statement.‛  

The sample sizes reported below are the event sample sizes, not the person sample sizes; 

hence, the partial correlations preserve the multi-level data structure. Such sizes vary due to 

skipped responses on some items for some events per person. The mean of each item 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 168 

characteristic was used in the analysis. Higher ratings reflect strong agreement that the items 

characterize what it was like for participants to think about a positive event. 

 

7.1.3.1 Positive mindfulness scales 

Focused Attention was assessed with two items: ‚focuses my attention on what is happening 

right now‛ and ‚absorbs my attention in the moment.‛ Responses to these items were 

uncorrelated, r(91) = .13, p = .21. Novelty Appreciation was assessed with two items: ‚makes me 

enjoy the newness of this moment‛ and ‚reminds me to take pleasure in experiencing new 

things.‛ Responses to these items correlated positively, r(90) = .38, p < .0005. Open-Ended 

Expectations was assessed with two items: ‚makes me unsure of what to expect‛ and ‚reminds 

me that I cannot tell how things are going to unfold.‛ Responses to these items correlated 

positively, r(91) = .47, p < .0005. 

 

7.1.3.2 Event characteristics scales 

Nostalgia was assessed with two items: ‚makes me feel nostalgic‛ and ‚is a nostalgic experience 

for me.‛ Responses to these items correlated positively, r(91) = .93, p < .0005. Purpose in Life was 

assessed with two items: ‚makes me feel life is meaningful‛ and ‚makes me feel life has a 

purpose‛. Responses to these items correlated positively, r(91) = .79, p < .0005. Self-Esteem was 

assessed with two items: ‚makes me like myself better‛ and ‚makes me feel good about 

myself.‛ Responses to these items correlated positively, r(91) = .71, p < .0005. Self-Continuity was 

assessed with two items: ‚makes me feel connected with who I was in the past‛ and ‚makes me 

feel connected with my past.‛ Responses to these items correlated positively, r(91) = .85, p < 

.0005. Interpersonal Connectedness was assessed with two items: ‚makes me feel I can trust 

others‛ and ‚makes me feel connected to loved ones.‛ Responses to these items correlated 

positively, r(91) = .54, p < .0005. Overall Rehearsal Frequency was assessed with one item: ‚How 

often have you rehearsed or reviewed this event since it occurred?‛ (never = 1 to 6 = very 

frequently). Finally, Event Affect was assessed with two items: ‚When it happened, how pleasant 

did this event make you feel?‛ and ‚When you remember the event now, how pleasant does 

the event make you feel?‛ (not at all = 1 to 6 = exceptionally). Responses to these items correlated 

positively, r(86) = .32, p < .005. 

 

7.2 Results and discussion 

Participants tended to recall and rate positive events from their lives that were, on average, 6.79 

years old (SD = 7.24). This approach differs from our initial studies because in the present study 

we did not constrain positive event age to one week. Nevertheless, event age was uncorrelated 

with each of the three scales of positive mindfulness. As in all of the previous studies, the 

composite averages each correlated positively with each other: Focused Attention with Novelty 

Appreciation, r(91) = .33, p =.002; Focused Attention with Open-Ended Expectations, r(91) = .21, 

p = .04; and, Novelty Appreciation with Open-Ended Expectations, r(91) = .36, p = .001. 

Only Focused Attention correlated positively with event-related affect, specifically with the 

affect prompted at recall, r(86) = .35, p < .0005; and, none correlated with affect felt at an event’s 

occurrence. Further, only Focused Attention correlated positively with overall rehearsal 

frequency, r(90) = .21, p = .04. Next, Novelty Appreciation correlated positively with self-

esteem, r(91) = .46, p < .0005. Both Focused Attention and Novelty Appreciation correlated 

positively with interpersonal connectedness, r(91) = .25, p = .02, and r(91) = .33, p = .002, 

respectively. Finally, Focused Attention, Novelty Appreciation, and Open-Ended Expectations 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 169 

each correlated positively with purpose in life, r(91) = .33, p = .002; r(91) = .39, p < .0005; and, 

r(91) = .34, p = .001, respectively. Finally, none of the dimensions of positive mindfulness 

correlated with nostalgia or self-continuity. 

These findings add to the convergent validity of positive mindfulness. First, in Studies 1 

and 2 we examined the relations between our scales and a variety of existing individual 

difference measures, whereas in Study 3 we examined the relations between our scales and 

conceptually related ratings about positive autobiographical events. Despite this difference in 

scope, the fact that all positive event characteristics did not correlate positively with all of our 

scales suggests that each of the dimensions represents a related but distinct facet of positive 

mindfulness.  

Second, up to this study we had prompted participants each for a single positive event that 

was constrained temporally to within the past week, a relatively recent event. In the present 

study we widened the scope of positive events by prompting not only two of such events per 

person but also placed no temporal constraints on their recollection task. Despite these 

differences in approach, results across each of the studies produced a converging pattern of 

correlations. 

Third, the validity estimates garnered in the present study support our notion that positive 

mindfulness is relevant to the study of a range of memorial and emotional phenomena. When 

thinking about their positive experiences our participants’ ratings evidenced a positive 

correlation between Focused Attention and pleasant affect, event rehearsal frequency, and 

interpersonal connectedness; thus, this dimension of positive mindfulness is relevant to several 

domains of experience: emotional, cognitive, and social, respectively.  

Further, only Novelty Appreciation correlated positively with the self-esteem boosting 

quality of recalling past positive experiences. Such reminiscence may make a person feel good 

about the self, which in turn may stimulate curiosity and an explorative use of attention, 

resulting in an appreciation for novelty. Such a finding is consistent with the broaden-and-

build framework of emotion regulation (Fredrickson et al., 2008). 

Finally, all three of our scales correlated positively with a sense of meaning to life that 

thinking about past positive experiences can provide; consistent with findings from nostalgia 

research (Routledge et al., 2011). Indeed, reflecting on past positive events may prompt a 

person to reflect on the meaning and purpose of their own lives. The data suggest that doing so 

might actually prevent goal-oriented, future thinking, forecasting, and planning. One can 

experience a sense that one’s expectations can, for a moment, be suspended, as reliving a 

positive experience by itself imbues the self with a sense of existential meaning. 

Interestingly, none of our scales of positive mindfulness correlated with nostalgia or self-

continuity. One reason for this might be that nostalgia, while a predominantly positive 

emotion, is sometimes bittersweet (Hepper et al., 2011). When we had asked participants to 

reflect on purely positive events, nostalgia may not have been as dominant a feature as it could 

have been. Prompting nostalgic events or events associated with mixed feelings might provoke 

positive mindfulness. This is an empirical issue for future research.  

So far, our data support Langer’s theory of mindfulness in the context of recalled positive 

experiences. In fact, the theory offers more dimensions than were needed to model positive 

experiences in our samples. Theory predicted five dimensions; however, our data suggested 

that only three dimensions were necessary. Across multiple samples our three-factor model fit 

the data better than a one-factor model, which distinguishes positive mindfulness from the one-

factor trait models. To garner more validation evidence for our model, we next turned away 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 170 

from subjective self-reports of autobiographical accounts to present-oriented, experimental 

manipulations. 

 

8. Study 4 

We conducted two experiments to garner evidence for the criterion validity of positive 

mindfulness. In parallel we aimed to strengthen the argument that our measurement model is 

sensitive to fluctuations of state mindfulness, particularly with regard to experiential positivity. 

Hence, we manipulated theory-relevant contextual elements within a controlled setting and 

then assessed the degree to which these manipulations influenced participant ratings on our 9-

item instrument (Experiment 1), and on a single item from each of the three dimensions of our 

instrument (Experiment 2). To minimize mono-operational bias in evaluating the construct 

validity of positive mindfulness (Cook & Campbell, 1979), the experimental tasks in both 

experiments were affectively neutral and relatively mundane compared to the recalled recent 

positive experiences we used in prior studies. To minimize mono-method bias in evaluating the 

construct validity of positive mindfulness (Cook & Campbell, 1979), Experiment 1 was 

designed to decrease positive mindfulness; Experiment 2 was designed to increase it.  

 

8.1 Experiment 1 

In Experiment 1, we aimed to induce boredom in participants via a repetitive task that has been 

shown to provoke boredom in the laboratory (Van Tilburg & Igou, 2011). We reasoned that 

because the nature of the boredom task is inherently unpleasant, any or all of our three positive 

mindfulness dimensions might evidence lower scores during boredom. Participants who 

experience only mild boredom, compared to those who experience greater boredom, may a) 

appreciate the novelty of the task to a greater degree, b) more strongly focus on the present, 

and c) formulate fewer expectations about what is going to happen in the near future. 

Therefore, we hypothesized that participants in the high boredom condition would score lower 

than participants in the low boredom condition on one or more of our subscales of positive 

mindfulness.  

 

8.1.1 Method 

8.1.1.1 Participants and procedure 

Participants included 34 women and 12 men enrolled in an undergraduate psychology course 

at the University of Limerick. The mean sample age was 18.78 years (SD = 2.19, range = 17-27). 

Each participant was randomly assigned to a Low Boredom condition (n = 24) or to a High 

Boredom condition (n = 22). Participants in the former transcribed via typing two Wikipedia 

entries from a web site into a text editor; participants in the latter group did the same task for 

10 entries (Van Tilburg & Igou, 2011). They next each rated an item on their level of state 

boredom: ‚To what extent did the task you just completed make you feel bored?‛ (1 = not at all 

to 7 = very much). Following this, they completed the 9-item Positive State Mindfulness Scale 

(see Appendix A). 

 

8.1.2 Results and discussion 

The state boredom manipulation proved successful: participants in the Low Boredom condition 

scored significantly lower (M = 3.79, SD = 1.84) on the boredom index than those in the High 

Boredom condition (M = 4.91, SD = 1.57), t(44) = -2.20, p < .05. Next, as hypothesized, 

participants in the Low Boredom condition evidenced higher Novelty Appreciation ratings (LS 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 171 

Mean = 4.81, SE = 0.23) than those in the High Boredom condition (LS Mean = 4.07, SE = 0.26), 

controlling for state boredom, F(1, 38) = 4.57, p < .05, partial ε2 = .11, model R2 = .14. Further, the 

effect of boredom on Novelty Appreciation remained significant, controlling for state boredom 

and with both Focused Attention and Open-Ended Expectations entered as covariates (due to 

the correlations between these ratings and the outcome rating), such that participants in the 

Low Boredom condition evidenced higher Novelty Appreciation ratings (LS Mean = 4.81, SE = 

0.17) than those in the High Boredom condition (LS Mean = 4.15, SE = 0.19), F(1, 36) = 6.93, p = 

.01, partial ε2 = .16, model R2 = .64. Although the means were in the same direction as above, 

Focused Attention and Open-Ended Expectations did not reach significance between boredom 

conditions. 

In sum, participants who engaged in the more repetitive and boring task of text 

transcription of web site passages into a text editor had a more difficult time maintaining a 

sense of Novelty Appreciation than those who engaged in a similar but less repetitive task. This 

makes sense, because what likely seemed novel at first to those in the High Boredom condition 

became routine and a bigger challenge to appreciate compared to the Low Boredom condition. 

Such a finding suggests that the more repetitive, unchallenging and boring a task becomes the 

harder it was for many individuals to find and appreciate novelty in the moment. Indeed, the 

more mindless a task becomes to a person the less mindful they will remain during the task, in 

general (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975). These data show that increased boredom diminished positive 

mindfulness, especially the appreciation of novelty.  

 

8.2 Experiment 2 

Experiment 2 differed from the previous experiment in two ways. First, in Experiment 2 we 

sought to engage participants’ attention in a task in order to increase positive mindfulness 

rather than decrease it. Second, as our outcome measures we assessed each dimension of 

positive mindfulness using only a subset of items from the full measurement instrument used 

in Experiment 1. Specifically, we chose the item with the highest CFA factor loading for each 

subscale in our earlier studies, to represent the dimensions of Novelty Appreciation, Focused 

Attention, and Open-Ended Expectations. Paralleling Experiment 1, we reasoned that engaging 

in a pleasant task that afforded the opportunity to experience positive mindfulness would 

boost levels of one or more of our three dimensions, compared to engaging in the same task but 

with less opportunity to experience positive mindfulness.  

Participants in each of two conditions first read the same riddle, and were then asked to 

solve the riddle (see Appendix B). Participants in the Delayed Answer condition were not told 

the correct response before prompted to solve it; whereas participants in the Immediate 

Answer condition were told the correct response before prompted to solve it. The former 

condition contained each of the three elements of positive mindfulness. For instance, being a 

mildly pleasant task, a) the novelty of the riddle was there to appreciate (it was unfamiliar to 

most participants), b) focused attention to the present was possible because they were given the 

goal to solve the riddle, and c) open-ended expectation was possible because they were 

informed that they would learn the correct response in a few minutes.  

In contrast, we reasoned that spoiling the mystery immediately by telling participants the 

correct response before prompting them for their response would not only stifle the 

opportunity to appreciate novelty, but would also shift these participants’ attention away from 

the present (toward the future, end of the study) and lead to closed-expectations (i.e., knowing 

the answer eliminated the suspense of discovering the correct response). Thus, the Delayed 

Answer participants, compared to the Immediate Answer participants, were expected to find 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 172 

an opportunity to appreciate the novelty of the task, sustain their focus to the present, and 

suspend their expectations about what should or could happen in the near future as they 

waited for the riddle’s outcome. In sum, we hypothesized that the Delayed Answer condition 

would produce higher scores on measures of positive mindfulness than would the Immediate 

Answer condition. 

 

8.2.1 Method 

8.2.1.1 Participants and procedure 

Participants included 92 individuals (52 women, 36 men and 4 who did not specify their 

gender) who were enrolled in graduate programs at the University of Limerick. The mean 

sample age was 29.09 years (SD = 8.32, range = 22-60).  

Participants first read and then gave their informed consent to participate in a study 

entitled ‚What it’s like when you pay attention‛. Each participant was randomly assigned to 

either the Immediate Answer condition (n = 51), who read a brief riddle1 and was immediately 

told the answer to it, or to the Delayed Answer condition (n = 41), who read the same riddle, 

then were prompted to solve it by typing a response, and later shown the correct response. 

Following the riddle, participants in both conditions indicated their familiarity with the riddle 

(0 = no; 1 = no, but it seemed familiar; 2 = yes), and then rated the extent to which they were certain 

they knew the correct response (1 = not at all certain, 2 = a little certain, 3 = moderately certain, 4 = 

certain).  

Following these ratings, both conditions completed a selection of three items from each of 

the three positive mindfulness subscales, one item from Focused Attention, Open-Ended 

Expectations and Novelty Appreciation, respectively: ‚At the moment my attention is absorbed 

in the moment‛, ‚Right now I cannot tell how things are going to unfold‛, and ‚At the moment 

I take great pleasure in experiencing new things‛ (6 = strongly agree, 5 = moderately agree, 4 = 

agree a little, 3 = disagree a little, 2 = moderately disagree, 1 = strongly disagree). 

Participants then reported their gender and age. On the penultimate page, those in the 

Delayed Answer condition were then shown the correct response to the riddle; those in the 

Immediate Answer condition were reminded of the correct response, which they had seen 

earlier. Finally, students were debriefed and thanked for their participation.  

 

8.2.2 Results and discussion 

The riddle manipulation proved successful: the participants who were told the answer before 

reading the riddle (Immediate Answer) more frequently solved the riddle correctly (40) than 

incorrectly (11) and those who were not told the answer before reading the riddle (Delayed 

Answer) more often responded incorrectly (24) than correctly (17), χ2 (1, N = 92) = 13.18, p < 

.0001, Cramer’s V = .38, p < .0001. As expected, participants in the Delayed Answer condition 

evidenced higher Focused Attention ratings (LS Mean = 4.66, SE = 0.18) than those in the 

Immediate Answer condition (LS Mean = 4.14, SE = 0.16), F(1, 90) = 4.51, p < .05, partial ε2 = .05, 

model R2 = .05.  

This effect also occurred after controlling for the main effects of whether or not participants 

solved the riddle correctly, whether they had heard the riddle before the experiment, the extent 

to which they were certain that they knew the outcome, and with ratings of Novelty 

Appreciation and Open-Ended Expectations included as covariates (due to the correlations 

between these ratings and the outcome rating). Under these controlled conditions, Focused 

Attention ratings were higher for those in the Delayed Answer condition (LS Mean = 4.55, SE = 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 173 

0.17) than those in the Immediate Answer condition (LS Mean = 3.98, SE = 0.19), F(1, 80) = 5.11, p 

< .05, partial ε2 = .06, model R2 = .47. 

Participants in the Immediate Answer condition evidenced a larger Open-Ended 

Expectation mean (LS Mean = 4.94, SE = 0.25) than those in the Delayed Answer condition (LS 

Mean = 4.35, SE = 0.21), however, this difference was statistically non-significant, F(1, 83) = 3.30, 

p = .07. Finally, participants in the Immediate Answer condition evidenced nearly identical 

Novelty Appreciation means (LS Mean = 4.41, SE = 0.23) to those in the Delayed Answer 

condition (LS Mean = 4.48, SE = 0.20), F(1, 83) < 1, ns. 

These results suggest that the participants who learned the riddle’s outcome immediately 

allowed their present focus of attention to wander more than those who had to wait to learn the 

riddle’s outcome. Those in the delayed answer condition had a legitimate reason to sustain 

their attention to the present moment. Such a result supports our hypothesis that attentional 

engagement increases positive mindfulness.  

 

9. General discussion  

The present research makes several important contributions to the field of positive psychology. 

First, our data clearly demonstrate that reported states of mindfulness during positive 

experiences are multidimensional (see Baer et al., 2006). Although social cognitive theorizing 

(Langer, 1992, 1997) strongly supports a multifaceted conceptualization of mindfulness, 

existing measures of this construct exclusively provide only a unidimensional total score. 

Further, existing measures of mindfulness focus on disposition and trait, whereas we posit and 

supported a state, positive state mindfulness.  

Across four independent samples, our data unequivocally demonstrate that a theory-

grounded three-factor model (Focused Attention, Novelty Appreciation, Open-Ended 

Expectations) provided an appropriate and generalizable model for assessing states of positive 

mindfulness, whereas a one-factor model did not. Thus, our data strongly confirm a priori, the 

multidimensionality of mindfulness, and they support the conclusion that states of positive 

mindfulness are meaningfully conceptualized in terms of focused attention, appreciation of 

novel stimuli, and open-ended expectations in relation to an unfolding experience. 

As a second important contribution to the literature, our multidimensional instrument is a 

valuable measurement tool for researchers who wish to operationalize mindfulness as a 

multifaceted state construct. Indeed, doing so is consistent with existing theoretical 

frameworks. The data from Study 1 demonstrate clearly the structural validity and ability of 

our three-factor model to replicate positive state mindfulness. Across a combined sample of 

1,363 respondents, the three-factor measurement model not only provided an acceptable 

psychometric representation of responses to the set of nine self-report items, but also 

consistently fit the data significantly better than a unitary, one-factor model and yielded brief 

subscales that had reasonable reliabilities. 

Providing a third valuable contribution, the data from Study 2 supported the construct 

validity of our three-factor model of positive mindfulness. In support of discriminant validity, 

each of the three positive mindfulness subscales correlated differently with the set of criterion 

measures. When controlling for differences in reliability, the three subscales were differentially 

inter-correlated, such that Focused Attention was equally related to both Novelty Appreciation 

and Open-Ended Expectations, Novelty Appreciation was more strongly related to Focused 

Attention than to Open-Ended Expectations, and Open-Ended Expectations was more strongly 

related to Focused Attention than to Novelty Appreciation. This pattern of results supports the 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 174 

conclusion that positive mindfulness is multidimensional, and that these three underlying 

dimensions are related but distinct. 

As a fourth contribution to the literature, our data support the conclusion that specific 

states of positive mindfulness are conceptually distinct from both general trait mindfulness and 

general trait mindlessness (see Sternberg, 2000). In Study 2 (Table 4), including trait measures 

of both mindfulness (MMS) and mindlessness (MAAS) enabled us to demonstrate that: a) trait 

mindfulness and trait mindlessness share only about 6% of their variance (r = .25); b) traits of 

high mindfulness and low mindlessness are more strongly associated with individual 

differences in need for cognition, absorption, uncertainty tolerance, and need for structure than 

are states of positive mindfulness; c) high trait mindfulness (MMS total score) predisposes 

people toward higher levels of Novelty Appreciation and Open-Ended Expectations, but is 

unrelated to Focused Attention during positive experiences; and, d) high trait mindlessness 

(MAAS total score) predisposes people toward Open-Ended Expectations during positive 

experiences. This last finding is important because it illustrates a specific way in which trait 

mindlessness is distinct from the absence of mindfulness—being characteristically mindless 

may actually help people avoid closing their minds to future possibilities while positive 

experiences are unfolding. 

Confirming specific nomological hypotheses about convergent and discriminant validity, 

the subscales of Focused Attention and Novelty Appreciation demonstrated reliable 

associations with self-assessed capacities to savor positive experiences prospectively (by 

anticipating), concurrently (by savoring the moment), and retrospectively (by reminiscing), and 

with tolerance for uncertainty. Further confirming our hypotheses, responses to Focused 

Attention correlated positively with trait absorption, but were unrelated to the need for 

structure; and all three positive mindfulness subscales were unrelated to the need for cognition. 

Yet our data are not entirely consistent with a priori predictions. Somewhat unexpectedly, 

Open-Ended Expectations were unrelated to self-assessed capacity to savor prospectively and 

concurrently, and were negatively correlated with perceived capacity to savor retrospectively. 

Consistent with past theorizing (Bryant & Veroff, 2007), we suggest that savoring is most 

strongly associated with focused attention on pleasurable stimuli, and that the tendency to 

savor through reminiscence may predispose individuals to restrict expectations during ongoing 

positive experiences, in order to facilitate encoding of memories for later recall. Also 

inconsistent with hypotheses, Open-Ended Expectations were unrelated to uncertainty 

tolerance, and Novelty Appreciation was unrelated to trait absorption. Nevertheless, the 

overall pattern of correlations of the three positive state mindfulness subscales with the set of 

criterion measures supported the convergent and discriminant validity of our 

multidimensional model of positive mindfulness. 

Despite these conceptual and psychometric contributions, the present research is not 

without its limitations. The types of experiences that we sampled represent emotionally 

pleasant but relatively mundane and ordinary recent experiences. We did not intend to sample 

respondents’ self-reported peak experiences, such as memories associated with intense levels of 

positive emotional arousal. We acknowledge, however, that some individuals did report such 

events, and these events were inter-mixed with the ordinary pleasant events. Perhaps other 

dimensions of mindfulness that we initially posited (e.g., Categorization and Distinction-

Making) would have emerged had we examined a wider range of positive experiences.  

In addition, although we included large samples of respondents, they were predominantly 

undergraduate students (an exception was Study 4, Experiment 2). Although we see no 

theoretical reason to believe college students’ states of positive mindfulness should differ from 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 175 

those of older adults, it remains important to demonstrate that our multidimensional measure 

of positive mindfulness generalizes to a wider community population. 

Furthermore, Studies 1, 2 and 3 assessed states of positive mindfulness retrospectively by 

asking respondents to report what they remembered experiencing during a recent positive 

event. Although we minimized the potentially distorting effects of the passage of time by 

restricting respondents to a positive experience that occurred during the past week (Studies 1-

2), we have no way of validating the accuracy of these retrospective self-reports. While it would 

be advantageous in some respects to assess mindfulness concurrently during ongoing positive 

events (i.e., experience sampling methodology), doing so might well alter the very states that 

participants are experiencing. Nonetheless, experience sampling methods could assist in 

validating positive state mindfulness. 

In Study 4 we manipulated state boredom and task engagement. Both experiments added 

to the validity of positive mindfulness using our scale. Indeed, Experiment 1 supported the 

notion that a repetitive and boring task impacted on positive mindfulness negatively, 

particularly affecting appreciation for novelty. Experiment 2 supported the notion that limited 

engagement with a simple cognitive task interfered with a person’s ability to focus their 

attention on the present. Stated differently, giving people the opportunity to engage in a 

puzzling task prompted them to focus their attention longer throughout the experiment, 

compared to those whose opportunity to engage was limited. These findings support the 

criterion validity of our multidimensional measure of positive state mindfulness. 

We hope that researchers seek to identify specific characteristics of positive events that 

promote states of positive mindfulness. For example, situational elements of uncertainty, 

spontaneity, novelty, complexity, surprise, inspiration, awe, nostalgia, and the absence of 

distraction may enhance the tendency to be mindful as positive experiences are unfolding 

(Bryant & Veroff, 2007). It would be valuable to manipulate such characteristics experimentally 

and measure participants’ responses using our states of positive mindfulness subscales. 

Related to the applications of savoring (Bryant & Veroff, 2007), we believe there are 

numerous applications of positive state mindfulness. We began by modeling spontaneous 

positive mindfulness in ordinary positive experience. Our data suggest that even a simple 

reminder about recent positive past events can rekindle positive mindfulness. We speculate 

that our dimensions of positive state mindfulness will be useful for practitioners of many 

disciplines. Indeed, increasing positive state mindfulness bolsters daily enjoyment.  

For example, in education our measure will help educators examine students’ engagement 

with novel information, and will help elucidate why some students lose interest in a subject 

while others retain their interest. Learning requires considerable effort; the expenditure of such 

effort can be a source of enjoyment and personal meaning. We think that assessing positive 

state mindfulness in the classroom will offer insights about the learning process, and ultimately 

assist educators to improve the quality of their instruction, as well as their students’ learning 

satisfaction and performance.  

As wellbeing researchers, we focus on the psychological processes that maintain health. 

Positive state mindfulness offers new possibilities for understanding the construction and 

maintenance of wellbeing; at the same time, we believe that it also offers new possibilities to 

understand what happens when wellbeing is disrupted and subdued. For example, clinicians 

and counselors benefit from assessing not only their clients’ maladaptive symptoms, such as 

anhedonia and rumination, but also benefit from assessing the lack of their clients’ adaptive 

indicators, such an appreciation for novelty in the present moment. We view the goals to 

increase positive mindfulness and savoring as facilitative with mindfulness-based programs 



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 176 

(e.g., stress management, dialectical behavioral therapy). Such goals include intrapersonal and 

interpersonal processes. In sum, we hope that positive state mindfulness and our measure will 

assist professionals in their efforts to improve the quality of life of those for whom they care. 

 
Acknowledgements 

We would like to thank Ellen Langer, Todd Bodner, and Claudia Dalbert for sharing their measures with 

us. We are grateful to Shannon Archuleta, Adam DeHoek, Carrie Ericksen, Scott King, Oisín Ryan, 

Colette Smart, and Wijnand Van Tilburg for their efforts in data collection, coding and entry, and to 

Elaine L. Kinsella, Brent D. Robbins, and Sarah E. Wood for their comments on earlier drafts of this 

manuscript.  

 

Authors 

Timothy D. Ritchie 

University of Limerick 

tim.ritchie@ul.ie 

 

Fred B. Bryant 

Loyola University 

 

Publishing Timeline 

Received 14 April 2012 

Accepted 9 July 2012 

Published 29 August 2012 

 

References 

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The Kentucky 

inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment, 11, 191-206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191104268029 

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment 

methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27-45. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191105283504  

Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness-of-fit in the analysis of covariance 

structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588  

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Carmody, J., Segal, Z. V., Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., 

& Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and 

Practice, 11, 230-241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph077  

Bodner, T. E., & Langer, E. J. (2001). Individual differences in mindfulness: The 

Mindfulness/Mindlessness Scale. Poster presented at the 13th annual American Psychological Society 

Convention, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. 

Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley. 

Brown, J., & Langer, E. (1990). Mindfulness and intelligence: A comparison. Educational Psychologist, 25, 

305-335.  

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in 

psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822-848. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822  

Brown, K. W., West, A. M., Loverich, T. M., & Biegel, G. M. (2011). Assessing adolescent mindfulness: 

Validation of an adapted Mindful Attention Awareness Scale in adolescent normative and 

psychiatric populations. Psychological Assessment, 23, 1023-1033. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021338  

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long 

(Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191104268029
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191105283504
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph077
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021338


Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 177 

Bryant, F. B. (1989). A four-factor model of perceived control: Avoiding, coping, obtaining, and savoring. 

Journal of Personality, 57, 773-797. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00494.x  

Bryant, F. B. (2000). Assessing the validity of measurement. In L. G. Grimm & P. R. Yarnold (Eds.), 

Reading and understanding more multivariate statistics (pp. 99-146). Washington, DC: American 

Psychological Association. 

Bryant, F. B. (2003). Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI): A scale for measuring beliefs about savoring. Journal 

of Mental Health, 12, 175-196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0963823031000103489  

Bryant, F. B., & Veroff, J. (2007). Savoring: A new model of positive experience. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 

Erlbaum Associates. 

Bryant, F. B., King, S. P., & Smart, C. M. (2006). Multivariate statistical strategies for construct validation 

in positive psychology. In A. G. Ong & M. Van Dulmen (Eds.), Oxford handbook of methods in positive 

psychology (pp. 61-82). New York: Oxford University Press. 

Buchheld, N., Grossman, P., & Walach, H. (2001). Measuring mindfulness in insight meditation 

Vipassana and meditation-based psychotherapy: The development of the Freiburg Mindfulness 

Inventory (FMI). Journal of Meditation and Meditation Research, 1, 11-34. 

Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 

116-131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116  

Carson, S. H., & Langer, E. J. (2006). Mindfulness and self-acceptance. Journal of Rational-Emotive & 

Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 24, 29-43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10942-006-0022-5  

Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York, NY: The Guilford 

Press. 

Chiesa, A. & Malinowski, P. (2011). Mindfulness-based approaches: Are they all the same? Journal of 

Clinical Psychology, 67, 404-424. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20776  

Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings. 

Chicago, IL: Rand McNally. 

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety: Experiencing flow in work and play. San Francisco, 

CA: Jossey-Bass. 

Dalbert, C. (1999). Die ungewissheitstoleranzskala: Skaleneigenschaften und validierungsbefunde [The 

Uncertainty Tolerance Scale: Reliability and validation]. Hallesche Berichte zur Pädagogischen 

Psychologie, 1, Halle, Germany. 

DeVellis, R. F. (1991). Scale development: Theory and application. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 

Erisman, S. M., & Roemer, L. (2010). A preliminary investigation of the effects of experimentally induced 

mindfulness on emotional responding to flip clips. Emotion, 10, 72-82. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017162  

Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J. McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). 

Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation reveal distinct neural modes of self reference. 

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 313-322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm030  

Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J. (2007). Mindfulness and emotion 

regulation: The development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness 

Scale—Revised (CAMS–R). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,29, 177–190. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10862-006-9035-8  

Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts build lives: 

Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness mediation, build consequential personal 

resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1045-1062. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013262  

Haigh, E. A. P., Moore, M. T., Kashdan, T. B., & Fresco, D. M. (2011). Examination of the factor structure 

and concurrent validity of the Langer Mindfulness/Mindlessness Scale. Assessment, 18, 11-26. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191110386342  

Hepper, E. G., Ritchie, T. D., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, T. (2012). Odyssey’s end: Lay conceptions of 

nostalgia reflect its original homeric meaning. Emotion, 12, 102-119. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025167  

http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00494.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0963823031000103489
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10942-006-0022-5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20776
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017162
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm030
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10862-006-9035-8
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013262
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191110386342
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025167


Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 178 

Höfling, V., Moosbrugger, H., Schermelleh-Engel, K., & Heidenreich, T. (2011). Mindfulness of 

mindlessness? A modified version of the Mindful Attention and Awareness Sc ale (MAAS). European 

Journal of Psychological Assessment, 27, 59-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000045  

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to 

underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3, 424-453. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.424  

Jacob, J. C., & Brinkerhoff, M. B. (1999). Mindfulness and subjective well-being in the sustainability 

movement: A further elaboration of multiple discrepancies theory. Social Indicators Research, 46, 341-

368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1006941403481  

Johnson, B. T. (1995). D-Stat: Software for the meta-analytic review of research literature (Version 1.11). 

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 

Jöreskog, K. G. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K. A Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing 

structural equation models (pp. 294-316). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 

Jöreskog, K., & Sörbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8: User’s reference guide. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software 

International, Inc. 

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Coming to our senses: Healing ourselves and the world through mindfulness. New York, 

NY: Hyperion. 

Langer, E. J. (1992). Matters of mind: Mindfulness/mindlessness in perspective. Consciousness and 

Cognition, 1, 289-305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/1053-8100(92)90066-J  

Langer, E. J. (1997). The power of mindful learning. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, 

Inc. 

Langer, E. J., & Moldoveanu, M. (2000). The construct of mindfulness. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 1-10. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00148  

Langer, E. J., Bashner, R., & Chanowitz, B. (1985). Decreasing prejudice by increasing discrimination. 

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 113-120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.49.1.113  

Langer, E. J., Hatem, M., Joss, J., & Howell, M. (1989). Conditional teaching and mindful learning: The 

role of uncertainty in education. Creativity Research Journal, 2, 139-150. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400418909534311  

Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z.V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D., Carlson, L., & Devins, G. (2006). The 

Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 1445-1467. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20326  

Neuberg, S. L. & Newsome, J. T. (1993). Personal need for structure: Individual differences in the desire 

for simple structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 113-131. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.113  

Ritchie, T. D., Skowronski, J. J., Hartnett, J. L., Wells, B. M., & Walker, W. R. (2009). The fading affect bias 

in the context of emotion activation level, mood, and personal theories of emotion change. Memory, 

17, 428-444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210902791665  

Ritchie, T. D., Skowronski, J. J., Wood, S. E., Walker, W. R., Vogl, R. J., & Gibbons, J. A. (2006). Event self-

importance, event rehearsal, and the fading affect bias in autobiographical memory. Self and Identity, 

5, 172-195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298860600591222  

Rothwell, N. (2006). The different facets of mindfulness. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior 

Therapy, 24, 79-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10942-006-0023-4  

Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Hart, C., Juhl, J., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., & Schlotz, 

W. (2011). The past makes the present meaningful: Nostalgia as an existential resource. Journal of 

Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 638-652.  http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024292  

Sternberg, R. J. (2000). Images of mindfulness. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 11-26. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00149  

Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau. M. A. (2000). 

Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. 

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615-625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.615  

http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000045
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.424
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1006941403481
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/1053-8100(92)90066-J
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00148
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.49.1.113
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400418909534311
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20326
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.113
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210902791665
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15298860600591222
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10942-006-0023-4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024292
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00149
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.615


Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 179 

Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences ("absorption"), a 

trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 268-277. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0036681  

Thompson, B. L., & Waltz, J. (2007). Everyday mindfulness and mindfulness meditation: Overlapping 

construction or not? Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1875-1885. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.017  

Van Tilburg, W. A. P., & Igou, E. R. (2011). On boredom and social identity: A pragmatic meaning-

regulation approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211418530  

Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., Gibbons, J. A., Vogl, R. J., & Ritchie, T. D. (2009). Why people rehearse 

their memories: Frequency of use and effects on the intensity of emotions associated with 

autobiographical memories. Memory, 17, 428-444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210903107846  

Wild, T. C., Kuiken, D., & Schopflocher, D. (1995). The role of absorption in experiential involvement. 

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 569-579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.569  

Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Arndt, J., & Routledge, C. (2006). Nostalgia: Content, triggers, functions. 

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 975-993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.975  

Wolff, H-G., & Preising, K. (2005). Exploring item and higher order factor structure with the Schmid-

Leiman solution: Syntax codes for SPSS and SAS. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 48-58. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03206397  

  

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0036681
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.017
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211418530
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210903107846
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.569
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.975
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03206397


Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 180 

Appendix A. The positive state mindfulness scale 

Permission to use. Permission for use of the Positive State Mindfulness Scale (PSMS) is not 

necessary when use is for academic research or educational purposes. Reference the authors by 

citing the present article. Use of the PSMS in profit-making ventures requires permission and a 

nominal charge. Inclusion of the PSMS within a larger scale that will be copyrighted also 

requires permission. Send a request to the first author. 

Usage and scoring. We staggered the item presentation order; a Focused Attention item, 

followed by a Novelty Appreciation item, followed by an Open-Ended Expectation item, and 

so on. We set each item to a 6-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = disagree 

somewhat, 4 = agree somewhat, 5 = agree, and 6 = strongly agree). Score each subscale by 

computing either the average or the sum. Each subscale is grouped below.  

Instructions. Phrase each item to fit your research question and design. For instance, in 

Studies 1-3 we assessed recalled positive events; hence, our instructions were phrased in the 

past tense: ‚Recall a recent positive experience that occurred sometime within the past five 

days.‛ In Study 4, we assessed positive state mindfulness following different experimental 

manipulations; hence, we phrased the items in the present tense.  

 

Dimension Past Tense Present Tense 

Focused 

Attention 

During my positive experience my 

attention was focused on what was 

happening just then. 

My attention is focused on what 

is happening right now.  

 
<my attention was absorbed in the 

moment. 

My attention is absorbed in the 

moment. 

 
<I was aware of nothing else except 

what I was going through right then. 

I am aware of nothing else 

except what I am going though 

right now. 

Novelty 

Appreciation 

During my positive experience I enjoyed 

the newness of the moment. 

I enjoy the newness of this 

moment. 

 
<I took great pleasure in experiencing 

new things. 

At the moment I take great 

pleasure in experiencing new 

things. 

 
<everything happening seemed unique 

to me. 

Right now everything 

happening seems unique to me. 

Open-Ended 

Expectations 

During my positive experience I wasn't 

sure of what to expect. 

Right now I am not sure what to 

expect. 

 
<I couldn't tell how things were going 

to unfold. 

Right now I cannot tell how 

things are going to unfold. 

 
<I felt like anything could happen from 

one moment to the next. 

I feel like anything could 

happen from one moment to the 

next. 

 

  



Positive state mindfulness 

Ritchie & Bryant 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 181 

Appendix B  

The riddle used in Study 4, Experiment 2: ‚A boy was at a carnival and went to a booth where a 

man said to the boy, ‘If I write your exact weight on this piece of paper then you have to give 

me €50, but if I cannot, I will pay you €50.’ The boy looked around and saw no scale, so he 

agrees thinking no matter what the man writes the boy will just say that he weighs more or 

less. In the end the boy ended up paying the man €50. How did the man win the bet?‛ 

http://www.blurtit.com/q627108.html  
 

http://www.blurtit.com/q627108.html