Kim, H., Doiron, K., Warren, M. A., & Donaldson, S. I. (2018). The international landscape of positive 

psychology research: A systematic review. International Journal of Wellbeing, 8(1), 50-70. 

doi:10.5502/ijw.v8i1.651 

 

Heejin Kim 

University of California, Irvine 

heejin.kim@uci.edu 

Copyright belongs to the author(s) 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 

50 

ARTICLE  

 

The international landscape of positive psychology 

research: A systematic review 
 

Heejin Kim  ·  Kathryn Doiron  ·   Meg A. Warren  ·   Stewart I. Donaldson 
 
 

Abstract:  Since positive psychology originated in 1998 as an organized stream of inquiry in the 

United States, it has inspired new theory and research on human flourishing across the world. The 

current systematic review presents an overview of (a) the prevalence of scientific research in 

positive psychology across five continents and 63 countries, (b) the characteristics of the research, 

including methodology and topics, and (c) the influence of positive psychology in expanding 

established lines of research in new ways. Through an analysis of 863 peer-reviewed positive 

psychology articles, this review attempts to map the international landscape of positive 

psychology research. Further, it responds to relevant critiques of the field, confirming some and 

dispelling others. Finally, recommendations are shared for future directions to build a more 

culturally responsive field of positive psychology that is committed to the advancement of 

flourishing and wellbeing in the global context. 

 

Keywords: positive psychology, wellbeing, optimal functioning, happiness, international, global, 

cross-cultural, diversity 

 

 

1. Introduction 

Since the inception of the positive psychology movement in 1998, scholarship in the area has 

grown in leaps and bounds in breadth of research, as well as reach across the world. The positive 

psychology perspective advocates a shift in the lens with which the world is viewed, i.e., one 

characterized by opportunity for growth (e.g., post-traumatic growth: Tedeschi, Blevins, & Riffle, 

2017) rather than mired by problems (e.g., post-traumatic stress: Yehuda, 2002). In contrast to 

predominant psychological research that is preoccupied with depression and turmoil, it looks to 

what causes individuals to thrive. Positive psychology has begun to create a roadmap for 

building positive qualities (e.g., resilience: Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000) as a complement to reducing 

negative influences (e.g., peer pressure: Borsari & Carey, 2001).  

This broad shift in perspective has opened up the space to pose new research questions, 

investigate previously ignored positive phenomena, and approach well-established areas of 

research with fresh eyes. Consequently, over the last two decades, this movement has inspired a 

plethora of research across a wide range of areas of scholarship. As this movement has made 

ripples around the world, we attempt to take stock and ask the question: how have scholars 

across the globe interpreted and engaged this perspective? What are the unique characteristics 

of the extant research influenced by this perspective? What are some of the impactful 

contributions that have emerged?  

The purpose of this review is to address these questions by examining the research in positive 

psychology across the world in its first 17 years, from 1998 to 2014. To do so, we map the global 

landscape of positive psychology research using three main foci: prevalence, characteristics, and 

http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/


International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 51 

research topics. The prevalence of positive psychology research is operationalized as the extent 

to which peer-reviewed empirical articles have been published in and across regions. The 

characteristics of this research are mapped by examining the broad trends of the research 

methodologies employed. Finally, we sample the prominent topics framed under the positive 

psychology umbrella that have made meaningful contributions around the world. We partially 

anchor our analyses around the early criticisms about positive psychology being an elite Western 

phenomenon (e.g., Christopher & Hickinbottom, 2008), to examine the extent to which these 

critiques have been addressed. These first 17 years signify positive psychology’s nascence when 

the field had about 2,000 articles in our database of positive psychology articles and we believe 

that understanding the trend in the initial years can provide reflections on the past, as well as 

guidance for the future. 

The current review is the first attempt to offer a bird’s eye view of the broad trends in positive 

psychology research across the world. Thus, it facilitates understanding of the role of regional 

contributions (where the research was conducted) in the larger international landscape of 

positive psychology. Such an analysis of the state of positive psychology promises to be 

instructive on trends and key areas of development, and encourage new research questions and 

directions. While the current study does not compare the status of positive psychology research 

across cultures, it provides a snapshot of the state of positive psychology in various geographic 

regions, which, in turn, serves to map the global presence and progress of the field as a whole. 

Thus, the current review hopes to spark conversations across regions in further advancing the 

field.  

 

2. Background 

Before we assess the influence of positive psychology, it is useful to revisit the scope of the field. 

While the early and most prominent definition of positive psychology emphasized the study of 

wellbeing and happiness (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000), the positive psychology 

perspective has influenced a broad range of scholarship, across multiple levels of analysis, 

inspiring research on virtue (e.g., Dahlsgaard, Peterson, & Seligman, 2005), meaning (e.g., King, 

Heintzelman, & Ward, 2016), gratitude (e.g., Watkins & Bell, 2017), positive emotions (e.g., 

Shiota, Yee, O’Neil, & Danvers, 2017), resilience (e.g., Richardson, 2002), positive relationships 

(e.g., Warren, Donaldson, & Lee, 2017), positive youth development (e.g., Shek, Sun, & Merrick, 

2013), and positive organizations (e.g., Warren, Donaldson, & Luthans, 2017), among many 

others. Therefore, despite a relatively short history, positive psychology has influenced research 

across most sub-areas of psychology and beyond, including education, management, coaching, 

therapy, public health, social services, philosophy, and political science (Donaldson, 

Csikszentmihalyi, & Nakamura, 2011).  

Global presence: what is known and what is not. Although the positive psychology movement 

originated in the United States (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000), there is now a vibrant 

community of researchers across the world. A recent review showed that positive psychology 

scholarship was found to be published in over 46 countries (Donaldson, Dollwet, & Rao, 2015). 

The global influence and popularity of positive psychology has grown to such a large extent that 

in addition to contributions in mainstream psychology journals, new journals dedicated to 

indigenous positive psychology scholarship are being developed (e.g., the Middle East and 

North Africa: Middle East Journal of Positive Psychology; India: Indian Journal of Positive 

Psychology).  

 

  



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 52 

Table 1. Illustrative sample of associations and degree programs by continent 

 Associations Courses and Degrees 

Europe • European Network of Positive 

Psychology 

• German-Speaking Association of Positive 

Psychology (German speaking areas) 

• German Society for Positive Psychology 

Research 

• French and Francophone Positive 

Psychology Association (Francophone 

areas) 

• Czech Positive Psychology Center 

• Hellenic Association of Positive 

Psychology 

• Italian Society of Positive Psychology 

• Polish Positive Psychology Association 

• Portuguese Association for Studies and 

Intervention in Positive Psychology 

• Spanish Society for Positive Psychology 

(SEPP) 

• Swiss Positive Psychology Association 

• Turkish Positive Psychology Association 

• Oslo Summer School (Norway) 

• Aarhus University (Denmark) 

• Universiteit Twente; Maastricht 

University (The Netherlands) 

• Universidade de Lisboa (Portugal) 

• IE University (Spain) 

• University of East London; City 

University of London; University 

of Glasgow; Middlesex University 

London; Anglia Ruskin 

University; Buckinghamshire 

New University (UK) 

Asia • Asian Center for Applied Positive 

Psychology 

• Global Chinese Positive Psychology 

Association 

• National Positive Psychology 

Association, India 

• Japan Positive Psychology Association 

• Informal groups in South Korea 

• Lebanese American University 

(Lebanon) 

• The Chinese University of Hong 

Kong; Hong Kong Shue Yan 

University (Hong Kong) 

• Jerusalem University (Israel) 

• School of Positive Psychology 

(Singapore) 

Americas • Associação de Psicologia Positiva da 

América Latina (Latin America) 

• Western Positive Psychology Association 

• Canadian Positive Psychology 

Association 

• Informal groups in Mexico and Brazil 

• Instituto Chileno de Psicologia 

Positiva (Chile) 

• Universidad Iberoamericana 

(Mexico) 

• TechMillenio University (Mexico) 

• Claremont Graduate University; 

University of Pennsylvania; 

University of Utah; Harvard 

University; Stanford University; 

University of California Los 

Angeles [UCLA] Extension; 

University of Michigan, Case 

Western Reserve University, 

University of Missouri (US) 

Oceania • New Zealand Association of Positive 

Psychology 

• University of Sydney; University 

of Melbourne: RMIT University; 

TAFE South Australia 

Africa  • North-West University (South 

Africa) 

International/ 

Global 

• International Positive Psychology 

Association 

 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 53 

Reviews of positive psychology literature in specific regions and cultures (e.g., South Africa: 

Coetzee & Viviers, 2007; Africa: Eloff, Achoui, Chireshe, Mutepfa, & Ofovwe, 2008; the Middle 

East and North Africa: Rao, Donaldson, & Doiron, 2015; non-Western cultures: Selin & Davey, 

2012; Latin America: Solano, 2014) have attempted to capture the scope and influence of positive 

psychology within these regions. However, these reviews, while commendable, tell us little 

about the differences and similarities in positive psychology research across regions in terms of 

prevalence, methodologies, and research topics. Some broad reviews and volumes have hinted 

at the growing research across geographic boundaries, but it was beyond their scope to capture 

the nuances across regions, and therefore, their analysis did not highlight the contributions of 

regional voices and contextual nuances (Donaldson et al., 2015; Rusk & Waters, 2013; Sheldon, 

Kashdan, & Steger, 2011). Consequently, while previous reviews have made important 

contributions, they do not illustrate and consolidate the scholarship on an international scale. 

The current review attempts to address this gap.  

Is positive psychology an elite Western phenomenon? Despite its popularity and widespread 

interest, positive psychology has been criticized for pandering to hegemonic voices and lacking 

sensitivity to real issues. Critics and theorists point out that the strong North American influence 

on positive psychology has resulted in placing the individual as the primary focus of research, 

to the exclusion of historical, cultural, political, and societal contexts (Becker & Marecek, 2008; 

Christopher & Hickinbottom, 2008). Therefore, there has been a need to decenter Western, 

particularly, North American positive psychology, and make visible contributions across the 

world. Indeed, in addition to the International Positive Psychology Association (IPPA) that 

serves a global audience, there are actively engaged scholars and practitioners fueling positive 

psychology through regional associations (See Table 1 above). The current review will shed light 

on these regional contributions across the world by examining research trends on a global scale.  

 

3. Method 

This review examines the prevalence, characteristics, and research topics of the extant empirical 

research published in positive psychology since the inception of the field in 1998, to 2014. This 

review builds on and extends a now well-established archive in the field that has been employed 

in other reviews (e.g., Ackerman, Warren, & Donaldson, 2017; Donaldson & Ko, 2010; Donaldson 

et al., 2015; Rao & Donaldson, 2015; Rao et al., 2015). This archive consists of 1,628 peer-reviewed 

English-language articles that include theory, research, reviews, and critiques that identify with 

positive psychology, published between 1998 and 2014. As described in those reviews, the 

archive consists of peer-reviewed English-language journal articles gathered by using the search 

term positive psychology (in quotation marks, with no Boolean operators, searching the full text of 

the articles) in five electronic research databases: Academic SearchTM Premier, Business Source 

Premier®, ERIC®, PsycINFO®, and PsycARTICLES®.  

Similar to the past reviews, we acknowledge that there may be a considerable number of 

articles published in various languages that have made significant contributions to the field. 

However, it was beyond the scope and ability of the research team to access and analyze a 

comprehensive set of articles that were written in languages other than English. Therefore, this 

archive has been restricted to English-language articles. We also acknowledge the risk of losing 

valuable research in the field by only reviewing articles that use positive psychology as a term in 

their full text. However, the epistemological underpinning for this search-term choice (i.e., 

“positive psychology”) was to minimize our own bias in labeling others’ articles as positive 

psychology and to use an objective, albeit restrictive, criterion. Therefore, we maintain this 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 54 

explicit criterion used in previous reviews and limit the analysis to literature that explicitly 

identified with the field of positive psychology.  

 

3.1 Screening and coding  

The current review focuses on the empirical contributions relevant to the international landscape 

of positive psychology. Given the importance of geography for this study, a key dilemma that 

arose was whether to use geographical locations of empirical study samples or locations of 

author affiliations to determine the location associated with the articles. Location of author 

affiliation was less practical for two reasons: 1) some authors indicated multiple institutional 

affiliations, sometimes in different countries; 2) some authors conducted research outside the 

country of their institutional affiliation. Thus, it was decided that the sample location would 

serve as a concrete indicator of the region and country in which the research took place.  

As a consequence, this limited the review to the examination of empirical articles that clearly 

reported sample locations. Consequently, the archive of 1,628 articles was screened to eliminate 

non-empirical articles (k = 656). In order to facilitate an analysis by geographical location, articles 

that did not report a sample location were also screened out (k = 109) to create a dataset (k = 863; 

henceforth referred to as such; see Figure 1). 

 

Figure 1. Results of literature search (based on PRISMA standards, Moher, Liberati, 

Tetzlaff, & Altman, 2009) 

 
Note: aThe numbers in “Records excluded” do not add up to 299 because these categories are not 

mutually exclusive. 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 55 

After screening, each empirical article was coded to capture prevalence, characteristics and 

research topics. To determine prevalence, the location of the sample, including continent (i.e., 

Europe, the Americas, Africa, Asia, and Oceania), region (e.g., Northern Europe), and country 

were coded. The U.S. was separately coded from the rest of the Americas, as the U.S. had a 

disproportionately large number of articles, which skewed the analyses when included. Since 

the positive psychology movement originated in the U.S. (Gable & Haidt, 2005), it is not 

surprising that there has been considerable research published therein (see Figure 2), particularly 

in the first 17 years (from 1998 to 2014) of the movement.  

 

Figure 2. The number of articles published outside the U.S. versus in the U.S. by year 

 
 

For the analysis of characteristics, we coded methodology, research design, and variables of 

interest. Five domains of methodology and research design were coded: 1) research methods, 

including qualitative, quantitative, or mixed methods; 2) level of analysis, including individual, 

dyad, group, organization, nation, activity/event, or multi-level; 3) study design, including 

correlational, experimental, or quasi-experimental; 4) study type, including cross-sectional or 

longitudinal; 5) measurement, including the article’s purpose as scale development, scale 

validation, or both.  

Next, articles were coded for key constructs, i.e., predictors, outcomes, and mechanisms, with 

an eye to emergent topics examined in local contexts. Further, each article was coded for whether 

it took a positive, deficit, or a combination of both perspectives. In their review, Rao and 

colleagues (2015) found two main motivations that tend to inspire and guide the direction of 

positive psychology research. The “positive” perspective refers to an abundance-based approach 

that focuses on topics related to optimal functioning, wellbeing, and quality of life. Articles that 

take a “deficit” perspective tend to introduce inherently positive constructs such as hope and 

benefit-finding as mechanisms and interventions to combat physical or psychological disorders, 

engage in a redemptive narrative, or help individuals cope with adversity (e.g., conducting self-



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 56 

compassion and optimism exercises for the benefit of depressed individuals (Shapira & 

Mongrain, 2010)). While most articles took one perspective or the other, some used a combination 

of perspectives to investigate the impact of positive constructs on a range of outcomes (e.g., 

wellbeing as well as depression as intervention outcomes). These articles were coded as 

“neutral.”  

Finally, the influence of positive psychology research across the world was examined using 

a qualitative analysis of the articles in the dataset. The main themes were drawn and 

interpretations are discussed. 

 

4. Results 

4.1 Prevalence  

Of all the positive psychology articles in the dataset that reported sample locations, 41% (k = 350 

articles) were from the U.S. Therefore, in some analyses in this review, the U.S. is used as a 

reference point, and at other times it is excluded from the analysis to facilitate comparison and 

fine-grained examination of research conducted in regions outside of the U.S. Accordingly, 

research conducted outside of the U.S. may be referred to as “non-U.S.” research hereafter.  

The number of articles published outside of the U.S. encompassed 52% of the articles in this 

dataset, including those from five continents, namely, Europe (k = 209), Asia (k = 114), the 

Americas (k = 52), Oceania (k = 46), and Africa (k = 25). The remaining 7% (k = 67) was comprised 

of multi-national articles. Although these numbers are smaller than the articles produced in the 

U.S., the percentage of research taking place outside of the U.S. tends to increase every year. (See 

Figure 2; though there are a few dips in the line graphs, the number of articles published 

generally increases every year.) Examination of the dataset revealed that empirical research was 

produced in 62 countries other than the U.S., and the top locations were the United Kingdom (k 

= 78), Canada (k = 57), and Australia (k = 55). China (k = 31) was ranked fifth and was the top 

country in Asia, and South Africa (k = 25) was ranked seventh and was the top country in Africa 

(see Table 2 below).  

While this review shows the growth of positive psychology research around the world, there 

is also evidence that some regions and countries tend to be hubs within a continent. For example, 

79% of the positive psychology articles using samples from Africa were from South Africa. In the 

Americas, apart from the U.S., 85% of the articles were published in North America (e.g., 

Canada), compared with only 15% in South and Central America. Europe and Asia showed 

similar trends. In Europe, countries in the Northern Europe region (e.g., the U.K., Ireland, 

Norway, Sweden, Denmark, the Netherlands, and Finland) published 49% of the entire 

publications from the continent, while the remaining 51% were spread between Southern 

Europe, Western Europe, and Eastern Europe. In Asia, countries in the East Asia region (e.g., 

China, Hong Kong, Japan, South Korea, and Taiwan) held 52% of the Asian sample locations 

within the dataset, while the remaining 48% were spread between the Middle East, South Asia, 

and Southeast Asia. 

Interest in positive psychology research has been spreading in new regions and countries. In 

the dataset, 24 countries were represented only once by participant samples (e.g., Kenya, 

Ukraine, Thailand), and 14 out of these 24 were included only in multinational studies as a 

comparative research site.   



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 57 

Table 2. Number of publications by country 

Country Region 
Number of 

Publications 

UK Northern Europe 78 

Canada Northern America 57 

Australia Oceania 55 

Germany Western Europe 36 

China East Asia 31 

Spain Southern Europe 25 

Israel Middle East/North Africa 25 

South Africa South Africa 25 

Netherlands Northern Europe 23 

Switzerland Western Europe 20 

Italy Southern Europe 16 

India South Asia 14 

Sweden Northern Europe 13 

Japan, Taiwan East Asia 11 

Norway Northern Europe 11 

Belgium Western Europe 9 

Hong Kong East Asia 8 

South Korea East Asia 8 

Turkey Eastern Europe 7 

Iran Middle East/North Africa 7 

New Zealand Oceania 6 

Singapore Southeast Asia 6 

Russia Eastern Europe 5 

Denmark Northern Europe 5 

Brazil South & Central America 5 

Romania Eastern Europe 4 

Ireland Northern Europe 4 

Austria Western Europe 4 

Croatia Eastern Europe 3 

Finland Northern Europe 3 

France Western Europe 3 

Poland Eastern Europe 2 

Argentina South & Central America 2 

Indonesia. Malaysia Southeast Asia 2 

Portugal, Greece Southern Europe 2 

Cameroon, Nigeria Central & Western Africa 1 

Kenya, Malawi, Tanzania, Uganda, Zimbabwe East Africa 1 

Hungary, Kosovo, Serbia Eastern Europe 1 

Kuwait, Afghanistan, Algeria, Jordan, Egypt Middle East/North Africa 1 

Northern Ireland Northern Europe 1 

El Salvador, Nicaragua, Bolivia South & Central America 1 

Lesotho South Africa 1 

Pakistan South Asia 1 

Thailand Southeast Asia 1 

Malta Southern Europe 1 

 

 

   



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 58 

4.2 Characteristics  

To understand the characteristics of positive psychology across regions, we coded the 

methodologies used in the articles by continents (see Table 3). We analyzed various aspects of 

methodologies, including research method, level of analysis, study design, and scale research. 

Table 3 summarizes the use of methods and design of research in each continent. The results 

largely confirmed the trend of conducting much quantitative, individual-level, correlational and 

cross-sectional research but also show the effort of using various other methods and designs in 

each region.  

 

Table 3. Methodologies by continent (%) 

 Africa 

n=25 

Americas 

n=52 

Asia 

n=114 

Europe 

n=209 

Oceania 

n=46 

US 

n=350 

Multi 

n=67 
Total (%) 

Research Method        

Quantitative 15 (60a)   34 (65) 96 (84) 171 (81) 28 (60) 287 (82) 49 (73) 680 (78.8b) 

Qualitative 7 (28) 8 (15) 8 (7) 22 (11)  9 (20) 26 (7) 10 (15) 90 (10.4) 

Mixed 3 (12) 9 (17) 9 (8) 16 (8)  9 (20)  34 (10)   8 (12) 88 (10.2) 

Missing - 1 (2) 1 (1) - -  3 (1) - 05 (0.6) 

Level of Analysis        

Individual 21 (84)    44 (85) 92 (81) 175 (84) 37 (80) 313 (89) 48 (72) 730 (84.6) 

Dyad 0 1 (2) 4 (4) 3 (1) 01 (2) 07 (2)  2 (3) 18 (2.1) 

Group 1 (4) 3 (6) 11 (10) 17 (8) 04 (9) 13 (4)  2 (3) 51 (5.9) 

Organization    - 1 (2)    - 3 (1)    0 - 05 (1) 0  - 9 (1) 

Nation 1 (4)       -    -     -     0- 01 (0) 0 8 (12) 10 (1.2) 

Activity/Event    - 1 (2) 1 (1) 5 (1) 01 (2) 04 (1)  2 (3) 14 (1.6) 

Multi-level 2 (8)      - 5 (5) 3 (1) 03 (7) 04 (1)  4 (6) 21 (2.4) 

Missing    - 2 (4) 1 (1) 3 (1)    0 - 03 (1)  1 (3) 10 (1.2) 

Study Design 1         

Correlational 19 (76) 35 (67) 84 (74) 157 (75) 24 (52) 253 (72) 55 (82) 627 (72.7) 

Experimental 1 (4) 4 (7) 12 (11)  28 (13) 13 (28) 044 (13) 2 (3) 104 (12.1) 

Quasi-

Experimental 
2 (8)   6 (12) 13 (11) 17 (8) 4 (9) 038 (11) 5 (7) 85 (9.8) 

Missing 03 (12)   7 (13)  5 (4)  7 (3)   5 (11) 15 (4) 5 (7) 47 (5.4) 

Study Design 2         

Cross-Sectional 20 (80)   34 (65) 90 (79) 152 (73) 28 (61) 260 (74) 62 (93) 646 (74.9) 

Longitudinal 1 (4) 9 (17) 15 (13) 40 (19) 10 (22)  55 (16) 2 (3) 132 (15.3) 

Others 2 (8)     4 (8) 8 (7) 016 (8)  6 (13)    28 (8) 0 0 64 (7.4) 

Missing 2 (8) 5 (10) 1 (1) 1 (0) 2 (4)  7 (2)  3 (4) 21 (2.4) 

Scale Research         

Scale 

Development 
1 (4) 3 (6)   0 04 (2) 2 (4) 8 (2) 2 (3) 20 (2.3) 

Scale Validation 03 (12) 3 (6) 13 (11) 06 (3) 1 (2)   15 (4) 7 (10) 48 (5.6) 

Both 1 (4) 1 (2) 3 (3) 06 (3) 000 9 (3) 4 (6) 24 (2.8) 

Neither 20 (80) 43 (83) 97 (85) 191 (91) 42 (91) 315 (90) 52 (90) 760 (88.1) 

Missing    - 00  2 (4) 0 1 (1) 0v 2 (1) 001 (2) 003 (1) 0 2 (3)  11 (1.3) 

Note: a Percentage of number of each method out of total number of articles within the continent. 

Percentages may not add up to 100 within each continent, as we rounded up .5 or above for individual 

scores; b percentage of number of each method out of total number of articles (k = 863).  

 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 59 

4.3 Topics 

We analyzed the topics of research as another characteristic of positive psychology research in 

regions. The independent and dependent variables of articles were coded and analyzed to 

identify topics and constructs studied across continents. Themes that emerged across regions and 

continents were used to create extensive lists of topics studied as predictors or outcomes, and 

their frequency was calculated. Popular themes and topics unique to each continent were 

identified. 

Predictors. Topic analysis across continents indicated that gratitude (k = 35) and personality 

variables (e.g., Big Five; k = 15) were the most commonly studied predictors among all regions 

(see Table 4 below).  

In Asian studies, emotional intelligence (k = 3), stability and balance (k = 2), and savoring (k = 2) 

were the main predictors of interest beyond the variables commonly used in positive psychology 

research across regions. In African studies, violence and trauma (k = 3) were most commonly 

studied. In the Americas, compassion (k = 2) and positive behavior support (k = 2) were commonly 

studied, while in Europe engagement (k = 5) was. In Oceania, coaching interventions (k = 7) were a 

commonly studied predictor, and in the U.S., leisure (k = 9), courage (k = 8), and autonomy (k = 6) 

were among the unique top ten predictors beyond the variables commonly studied in positive 

psychology in general.  

Outcomes. The dependent variables again showed some unique patterns in each region. 

Predictably, the dependent variables found in common across all regions are among the most 

commonly investigated topics in positive psychology overall (Donaldson et al., 2015; see Table 4 

above), including wellbeing (k = 149), life satisfaction (k = 91), positive/negative affect (k = 87), happiness 

(k = 60), and resilience (k = 28). In research from Asia, social acceptance (k = 2), self-efficacy (k = 2), 

and post-traumatic stress (k = 2) were unique among the top dependent variables. In Africa, coping 

and recovery (k = 2) were commonly studied, while in Europe, anxiety (k = 11) and the concept of 

flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; k = 5) were common outcomes of interest. In the Americas, values (k 

= 2) and humor (k = 2) were commonly studied outcomes, in Oceania, positive youth development (k 

= 6) and positive relationships (k = 2) were commonly studied, and in the U.S., achievement (k = 8), 

attenuating of risk behaviors (k = 7), purpose (k = 6), and altruism (k = 5) were relatively common 

outcome variables.  

Positive or Deficit Perspective. Overall, 46% of the articles in the dataset (k = 397) took a positive 

perspective, 29% (k = 251) took a deficit perspective, and 25% used both positive and deficit 

perspectives (k = 215). The highest percentages of a deficit approach (40% of articles) were from 

Africa and the Americas (k = 10, k = 21 respectively). The highest percentages of positive 

approaches were from Oceania (56%; k =26) and multinational studies (57%, k = 38). See Table 5 

below for exact frequencies.  

 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 60 

Table 4. Frequencies of predictors and outcomes across continents/regions 

  Predictors Outcomes 

Common 

Gratitude (35) 

Personality (15) 

Wellbeing (149) 

Life Satisfaction (91) 

Positive or negative affect (87) 

Happiness (60) 

Depression (48) 

Health (39) 

Resilience (28) 

Asia 

Emotional intelligence (3) 

Neuroticism (3) 

Resilience (3) 

Positive external-concept (2) 

Savoring (2) 

Self-concept (2) 

Stability/Balance (2) 

Trust (2)  

Attributional style (2) 

Employability (2) 

Social acceptance (2) 

Post-traumatic stress (2) 

Self-efficacy (2) 

Africa Violence/Trauma (3) Coping/Recovery (2) 

Americas 

Motivation (3)  

Compassion (2) 

Psychological resources (2)  

Positive behavior support (2) 

Moral excellence (2) 

Self-acceptance (2) 

Values (2)  

Humor (2) 

Turnover (2)  

Europe 
Engagement (5) Anxiety (11)  

Flow (5) 

Oceania 

Coaching (7) 

Calling (2)  

Positive youth development (6) 

Floundering (2)  

Parenting style (2)  

Positive relationships (2) 

Flourishing (2) 

Multi-national 

Power/Powerlessness (3) 

Eudaimonic and hedonic dispositions (2) 

 

Approach/avoidance (3) 

Emotional disclosure (3) 

Meditation/mindfulness (3) 

US 

Leisure (9)  

Courage (8)  

Humility (7) 

Attachment (7) 

Self-regulation (7)  

Autonomy (6) 

Post-traumatic growth (5) 

Achievement (8) 

Organizational commitment (7) 

Attenuating risk behavior (7) 

Courage (6)  

Purpose (6) 

Altruism (5) 

Creativity (5) 

Effort (5) 

Adjustment (5) 

Prosocial activities (5) 

 

 
  



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 61 

Table 5. Frequencies of positive, neutral, and deficit approaches (%) 

 Africa 

(k=25) 

Americas 

(k=52) 

Asia 

(k=114) 

Europe 

(k=209) 

Oceania 

(k=46) 

US 

(k=350) 

Multi 

(k=67) 
Total (%) 

Positive 10 (40) 18 (35) 55 (48) 88 (42) 26 (57) 162 (46) 38 (57) 397 (46) 

Neutral 4 (16) 13 (25) 22 (19) 52 (25) 9 (20) 97 (28) 17 (25) 215 (24.9) 

Deficit 10 (40) 21 (40) 37 (32) 69 (33) 11 (24) 91 (26) 12 (18) 251 (29.1) 

 

Indigenous Issues. The results from the analysis of indigenous issues demonstrated that some 

positive psychologists are addressing pertinent and socially relevant local events. This shows the 

potential of positive psychology to address the pressing events that affect people’s lives all over 

the world, although the actual number of articles including local topics is small in this analysis 

(k = 44; see Table 6 for frequency of topics).  

 

Table 6. Frequencies of local topics studied 

Asia 

Post-traumatic stress (Israel: 2) 

Burnout (China: 1) 

Migrant workers (China: 1) 

Economic transition (China: 1)  

Earthquake survivors (Indonesia: 1) 

Children exposed to war (Afghanistan: 1) 

Cumulative adversity (Israel: 1) 

Missile attacks (Israel: 1) 

Mystical experiences of Muslims (1) 

Africa 

Cultural groups in South Africa (4) 

HIV/AIDS (South Africa: 2) 

African-centered coping (South Africa: 1) 

Americas 
Trash pickers (Nicaragua: 1) 

First Nations cultural trauma (Canada: 1) 

Europe 

Terrorism (Spain: 2) 

HIV/AIDS (Russia: 1) 

Football supporters (England: 1) 

Oceania Indigenous students (Australia: 1)  

Multi-National - 

US 

Minority issues of races (9) 

September 9/11 (4) 

Hurricane Katrina (3) 

College campus shooting (1) 

Low-income Housing in NYC (1) 

No Child Left Behind (1) 

War refugees (1) 

 

For example, in Asia, some research in the Middle East had focused on trauma and post-

traumatic stress in response to chronic violence. Some studies in China had examined the 

response of migrant workers and their children to the large economic transition that has taken 

place over the past decade, as well as the associated stigma. In the Americas, study samples 

included trash pickers in Nicaragua and Aboriginal students in Canada. In Europe, research 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 62 

investigated the impact of terrorism in Spain. In South Africa, a commonly studied topic of 

research involved the comparison of various ethnic groups and tribes that live together in the 

country. Studies in the U.S. included research on race and ethnicity issues (e.g., African 

Americans, immigrants) as well as the impact of the terrorist attack on September 11, 2001 and 

Hurricane Katrina in 2005. 

 

4.4 Influences 

We closely examined articles that demonstrated how positive psychology research is influencing 

the lives of individuals, groups, and nations. Accordingly, the findings from our qualitative 

review highlighted main themes that show how positive psychology research is making 

influential contributions and shifting the discourse across the world.  

Resilience after traumatic events. Positive psychology has inspired research on the study of 

unexpected positive outcomes that emerge in the aftermath of man-made and natural disasters. 

One such area of research emerging in many locations around the world is the examination of 

resilience and benefit-finding after large-scale traumatic events such as terrorist attacks and 

earthquakes. Weeks after a terrorist attack in Spain, Vázquez and Hervás (2010) found that 

citizens were finding potential benefits (e.g., learning from the experience, meaning in life) in the 

aftermath of the attack, as well as experiencing positive reactions such as solidarity and a feeling 

of belonging to the nation. In a related vein, Hobfoll and his colleagues (2009; 2012) have focused 

on understanding how one can cultivate resilience and the ability to stay engaged and vigorous 

in life while facing the constant threat of trauma and violence on the West Bank and the Gaza 

Strip.  

Another area of contribution of positive psychology research is the development of new 

constructs, and contextualized investigation of established constructs to capture how human 

strengths can provide a buffer against the effects of adversity. In a study of survivors of the 2009 

earthquake in Indonesia, Lies, Mellor, and Hong (2014) found that hardiness and gratitude were 

negatively related to global distress and posttraumatic stress symptoms five and eight months 

after the earthquake, respectively.     

Resilience after disease or injury. Experiencing a serious disease or a catastrophic injury is often 

traumatic for individuals and their families. Positive psychology research in this area has 

investigated strengths-based interventions and practices to buffer the impact, increase resilience, 

and actively cope with these conditions. The review by Casellas-Grau, Font, and Vives (2014) of 

16 studies on positive psychology interventions for breast cancer patients demonstrated that 

well-designed interventions can help individuals with making meaning and sense of their 

condition, foster hope, and cultivate positive emotions. Similarly, there is a growing body of 

research on interventions fostering posttraumatic growth and benefit-finding with cancer 

patients and individuals with other traumatic health problems all over the world, including in 

China, Romania, and the UK (Ho, Chan, & Ho, 2004; Kállay, 2008; Oaksford, Frude, & Cuddihy, 

2005). 

Another key contribution of positive psychology research is on understanding the 

complexities of people living with HIV and AIDS, and developing interventions to alleviate their 

problems. Pecoraro et al. (2014) found that HIV patients facing stigma and discrimination were 

more likely to stay in treatment if desire to live, social support, spirituality, and positive thinking 

were present in their lives. In another instance, Wood, Theron, and Mayaba (2012) found that 

reading culturally relevant stories to children who had been orphaned by AIDS led to increased 

resilience.  



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 63 

Positive experiences of marginalized populations. A small but promising body of positive 

psychology research is on recognizing positive emotions and strengths in marginalized 

populations. In dominant scholarly research and popular literature, marginalized populations 

have often been deemed as unhealthy, depressed, and frustrated (e.g., Choi, Miller, & Wilbur, 

2009). However, contrary to popular notions, initial evidence suggests that marginalization from 

society itself does not necessarily predict negative affect and depression. Vázquez (2013) found 

that an impoverished and stigmatized population (i.e., trash pickers) also experienced high levels 

of happiness and optimism. According to their findings, these individuals tended to find 

pathways to happiness through familial relationships and having time to participate in relatively 

inexpensive leisure activities like reading, rather than through having a high income. This aligns 

with Biswas-Diener and Diener’s (2006) findings on experience of happiness among slum 

dwellers in India.  

Moreover, the positive psychological perspective has served as a unifying mechanism to 

organize and combine interventions for marginalized populations. Brendtro, Brokenleg, and Van 

Bockern (2005) suggested the Circle of Courage as a theme for interventions that promote 

significance, competence, power, and virtue among native people suffering from 

intergenerational cultural traumas. They stressed the importance of a strengths-based approach 

rather than deficit-focused approach to youth development and created a new space for 

development and implementation of positive-oriented programs and policies. 

Indicators of happiness across cultures. Another way that positive psychologists are making an 

impact is through cross-cultural research that demonstrates the similarities and differences in 

indicators of happiness and wellbeing around the world. For example, Delle Fave, Brdar, Friere, 

Vella-Brodrick, and Wissing (2011) investigated the components of happiness across seven 

countries. The authors found that happiness was commonly defined as psychological balance or 

harmony and that social and familial relationships are the largest predictor of happiness. 

Brannan, Biswas-Diener, Mohr, Mortazavi, and Stein (2013) looked at the relationship between 

social support and wellbeing in Iran, Jordan, and the U.S. and found that family support 

predicted wellbeing in all three countries, while friend support was predictive only in the U.S. 

and Jordan. These findings highlight the nuances in the role of interaction between social 

relationships and culture on wellbeing. In other words, while social support is a common 

predictor of happiness and wellbeing across nations, there are cultural differences in the type 

and nature of social support necessary for greater wellbeing.  

In another instance, Kormi-Nouri, Farahani, and Trost (2013) observed different relationships 

between affect and wellbeing in Sweden and Iran. As increased positive affect led to increased 

wellbeing among Swedish participants, a balance of positive and negative emotions led to higher 

wellbeing for the Iranian participants. This shows that while people around the world may have 

some similar predictors of happiness, there are often cultural differences in how these indicators 

operate in predicting happiness and wellbeing. 

Balance between positive and negative. Positive psychology is often misunderstood as being 

concerned only with rosy aspects, while neglecting harsh realities of life (e.g., Fineman, 2006). 

Early proponents of positive psychology have insisted that the purpose of positive psychology 

is to balance positive and negative perspectives in the psychological sciences, and our review 

supports this. Positive psychology research has made important contributions in finding ways 

to strike a balance between the inherently positive and negative aspects of human experience 

and explain both. For example, Vallerand et al. (2008) showed that football supporters may 

experience both positive and negative consequences from their acts of team-support. Previous 

research encountered a “paradox,” where supporters of sports teams are related to both positive 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 64 

(e.g., psychological health) and negative (e.g., aggression) outcomes. Vallerand et al. (2008) 

adopted the Dualistic Model of Passion (Vallerand et al., 2003) and explained the paradox, such 

that individuals’ passion types (i.e., harmonious or obsessive) predicted positive and negative 

consequences. This research presents a case where positive psychology not only argues for 

positive aspects of life, but equally encompasses both positive and negative aspects of a 

phenomenon in a single model. (For another example, see Keyes’s (2003) Model of Languishing 

and Flourishing.) 

 

5. Discussion 

The purpose of this review was to understand the overall landscape of positive psychology 

research that has emerged in various regions across the world in the initial 17 years of the field 

and to examine the validity of some of the critiques of positive psychology. Thus, the review 

examined the prevalence, characteristics, and influence of empirical research in positive 

psychology across the world.   

 

5.1 Global presence 

Positive psychology was found to have a large and growing global presence and visibility. The 

analysis reveals that positive psychology is entering and making important contributions in the 

scholarly conversations and debates across a range of important societal issues around the world. 

Further, positive psychology is no longer a United States and Western phenomenon as much as 

a psychological science in general. Past reviews of research in psychological science have 

criticized that peer-reviewed psychological research is dominated by the United States and 

Westernized samples. For instance, Arnett (2008) found that 68% of samples of studies published 

in top journals of six sub-disciplines of psychology were from the United States, and 96% from 

Westernized countries. In contrast, in the current review, 41% of the samples in positive 

psychology studies were from the United States, and 70% from Westernized countries. Thus, 

while positive psychology has been criticized for ethnocentrism (Christopher & Hickinbottom, 

2008), this review shows that it fares better than the psychological research included in Arnett 

(2008).  

 

5.2 Methodological relevance  

The analysis of methodologies used in the articles in the dataset reveals that the field is 

dominated by studies using quantitative, correlational, and individual-level analyses. While 

these tendencies are still observed, geographical analyses by continent indicate that preferences 

for methodologies show some variations within and across regions, depending on their contexts 

and regional concerns. For instance, research emerging from Africa has several qualitative 

studies that may be explained in part due to a strong history of storytelling and the resulting 

influence on the choice of research methods (e.g., Reitmaier, Bidwell, & Marsden, 2011). The 

research published in the U.S. showed a high percentage of individual-level analysis, and in Asia 

there was a good number of group-level analyses, potentially reflecting the cultural differences 

in individualism and collectivism (Hofstede, 1984). Finally, compared to other regions, in 

Oceania, a higher percentage of empirical studies employed mixed methods, longitudinal 

designs, experimental methods, and multi-level analyses. These are prima facie considered to be 

more desirable methodological practices in psychology, although the current analysis does not 

assess quality and rigor of individual studies. Future research should examine the determinants 

of the use of research methods among scholars. 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 65 

5.3 Conceptual balance 

One of the most noteworthy findings from the analyses of topics and constructs studied was that 

research in positive psychology not only includes investigation of positive topics but also their 

interaction with and impact on psychological dysfunction (e.g., depression) and social issues 

(e.g., terrorism). This growing balance directly addresses one of the common concerns of positive 

psychology: that the field solely focuses on positivity, neglecting the negative reality of human 

life and society (Hackman, 2009). Further, cultural, social, and political issues and priorities in 

the regions influenced which constructs were studied and how this balance was achieved. A 

research interest in stability in Asian research might reflect the values of religious traditions (e.g., 

Buddhism, Hinduism), while predictors such as trauma and violence, and outcomes such as 

coping and recovery in African research point to the historical and political struggles in the 

region (e.g., civil war, drought, and famine). Similarly, the prevalence of posttraumatic stress and 

resilience as outcome variables in Asian research may reflect research on managing adversity 

arising from wars in the Middle East.  

 

5.4 Global relevance 

Positive psychology scholars have added value to past and established research on posttraumatic 

stress and recovery through exploration of the human aftermath of large-scale traumatic events 

(e.g., Vázquez & Hervás, 2010) and the resilience of individuals who have been affected by 

disease or injury (e.g., Casellas-Grau et al., 2014). These studies show that evidence-based 

interventions are being applied to the most pressing issues of our time, such as combating the 

effects of terrorism and the HIV/AIDS epidemic. The research indicates that happiness and 

strengths exist in human beings in any circumstance, not exclusively among wealthy, privileged 

individuals (e.g., Vazquez, 2013). Further, while happiness and wellbeing may be experienced 

and expressed through different means across populations, positive psychology research and 

interventions can help understand and improve the quality of life for all, regardless of their 

current life circumstances (Delle Fave et al., 2011).  

 

5.5 Strengths, limitations, and future directions  

The purpose of this review was to examine the global landscape of research in positive 

psychology in its initial years, and, as such, is the first large-scale analysis of its kind. As the 

articles for this review were extracted from several large electronic databases, it is expected that 

it provides a fairly reliable estimate of the state of the field. As positive psychology gets close to 

the end of its second decade of existence, this review provides a summary of the first 17 years of 

the field across the world and highlights the emergence of what might be termed as the next 

generation of positive psychology that is increasingly diverse, culturally responsive, contextual, 

and influential.  

However, it is important to view this study in light of its limitations. The current 

investigation faces the limitations of previous reviews that have used similar methods 

(Donaldson & Ko, 2010; Donaldson et al., 2015; Rao & Donaldson, 2015; Rao et al., 2015). 

Specifically, it includes only research published in English, and therefore underestimates the 

breadth and depth of positive psychology’s prevalence and impact around the world. Similarly, 

only research that explicitly identified with or situated itself in relation to positive psychology 

was included in this analysis. Thus, authors’ identification with alternative labels for positive 

psychology, such as psychofortology in South Africa (Strümpfer, 2005), may have caused an 

underestimation of the global influence of positive psychology. In addition, the paper does not 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 66 

exhaustively cover all topics in positive psychology, and instead focuses on highlighting 

prominent trends, thereby inadvertently excluding some (e.g., wisdom).   

Although the current analytical strategy emphasizes the research participants (i.e., where 

they are situated) so that it allows for commentary on the psychological phenomena of the people 

in the region, we acknowledge that using a different criterion (e.g., author location) would have 

enabled us to conduct other interesting analyses, such as patterns of multinational collaboration 

in scholarship. Future studies could explore this. Finally, the current study did not account for 

the impact of articles, which may be reflected in the impact factors of the journals or the number 

of citations of articles. Weighing the count of articles with a measure of impact may highlight 

regions that have few articles yet may have high impact on the field. 

While the current study focused on presenting an international landscape of the field of 

positive psychology, future research can delve into some of the trends observed in this study to 

investigate the “why” and “how” of those trends. Future research can also conduct reviews of 

the later years of the field to compare and contrast the new trends to these initial years and 

examine the changes. As a broader suggestion based on our findings, we recommend increased 

focus on the development of a culturally responsive positive psychology. A key area for growth 

of the field is the development, measurement, and testing of constructs that are grounded in the 

social, economic, and political context of the region. Further, we call for increased cross-cultural 

comparative research that showcases the complex nature and multi-dimensionality of positive 

constructs and their relevance across cultures and contexts.  

 

6. Conclusion 

This study summarized the first 17 years of research in the field of positive psychology. While 

the field is still in nascence, scholars in positive psychology have made important progress across 

the world, giving rise to some general trends and regional perspectives. We anchored the 

analyses partially around the early criticisms of the field, which provide guidance for the future 

of positive psychology as an established field in psychology. Csikszentmihalyi (1993) noted that 

an individual or system develops and becomes richer through successfully balancing 

differentiation and integration. Differentiation involves pursuing uniqueness and distinguishing 

oneself from others, while integration entails unifying with others by creating something beyond 

oneself. Positive psychology as a field will benefit from this approach by cultivating cultural 

sensitivity regarding human life in different contexts and integrating a wide range of 

perspectives and approaches into a unified view. 

 
Acknowledgments 

Special thanks to Maria McManus and Annelise Austill, Claremont Graduate University, and Russell 

Donaldson, Redlands University, for assistance with coding and database management, and Noah Boyd,  

Joo Young Lee, and Scott Donaldson, Claremont Graduate University, for their helpful review and edits. 

A portion of this paper was awarded 3rd place by the American Psychological Association’s International 

Psychology Division (Division 52) at the Western Psychological Association Convention 2016. 

 

Authors 

Heejin Kim 

University of California, Irvine 

heejin.kim@uci.edu 

 

Kathryn Doiron 

Claremont Graduate University 



International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 67 

 

Meg A. Warren 

Claremont Graduate University 

Western Washington University 

 

Stewart I. Donaldson 

Claremont Graduate University 

 

Publishing Timeline 

Received 30 July 2017 

Accepted 5 February 2018  

Published 13 July 2018  

 

References 

Ackerman, C., Warren, M. A., & Donaldson, S. I. (2018). Scaling the heights of positive psychology: A 

systematic review of measurement. Unpublished Manuscript. 

Arnett, J. J. (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less 

American. American Psychologist, 63, 602-614. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602 

Becker, D., & Marecek, J. (2008). Positive psychology: History in the remaking? Theory & Psychology, 18, 

591–604. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354308093397 

Biswas-Diener, R., & Diener, E. D. (2006). The subjective well-being of the homeless, and lessons for 

happiness. Social Indicators Research, 76, 185-205. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-005-8671-9 

Borsari, B., & Carey, K. B. (2001). Peer influences on college drinking: A review of the research. Journal of 

Substance Abuse, 13, 391-424. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0899-3289(01)00098-0 

Brannan, D., Biswas-Diener, R., Mohr, C. D., Mortazavi, S., & Stein, N. (2013). Friends and family: A 

cross-cultural investigation of social support and subjective well-being among college students. The 

Journal of Positive Psychology, 8, 65-75. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2012.743573 

Brendtro, L., Brokenleg, M., & Van Bockern, S. (2005). The circle of courage and positive psychology. 

Reclaiming Children and Youth, 14, 130-136. 

Casellas‐Grau, A., Font, A., & Vives, J. (2014). Positive psychology interventions in breast cancer. A 

systematic review. Psycho‐Oncology, 23, 9-19. https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.3353 

Choi, J., Miller, A., & Wilbur, J. (2009). Acculturation and depressive symptoms in Korean immigrant 

women. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health, 11, 13-19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10903-007-9080-8 

Christopher, J. C., & Hickinbottom, S. (2008). Positive psychology, ethnocentrism, and the disguised 

ideology of individualism. Theory & Psychology, 18, 563-589. 

https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354308093396 

Coetzee, S., & Viviers, R. (2007). An overview of research on positive psychology in South Africa. South 

African Journal of Psychology, 37, 470-490. https://doi.org/10.1177/008124630703700307 

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal performance. New York, NY: Cambridge 

University Press. 

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). Evolving self. New York, NY: HarperCollins. 

Dahlsgaard, K., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. P. (2005). Shared virtue: The convergence of valued human 

strengths across culture and history. Review of General Psychology, 9, 203-213. 

https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.9.3.203 

Delle Fave, A. D., Brdar, I., Freire, T., Vella-Brodrick, D., & Wissing, M. (2011). The eudaimonic and 

hedonic components of happiness: Qualitative and quantitative findings. Social Indicators Research, 

100, 185-207. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-010-9632-5 

Donaldson, S. I., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (2011). Applied positive psychology: Improving 

everyday life, health, schools, work, and society. London, England: Routledge Academic. 

https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602
https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354308093397
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-005-8671-9
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0899-3289(01)00098-0
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2012.743573
https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.3353
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10903-007-9080-8
https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354308093396
https://doi.org/10.1177/008124630703700307
https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.9.3.203
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-010-9632-5


International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 68 

Donaldson, S. I., Dollwet, M., & Rao, M. A. (2015). Happiness, excellence, and optimal functioning 

revisited: Examining the peer-reviewed literature linked to positive psychology. The Journal of 

Positive Psychology, 10, 185-195. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.943801  

Donaldson, S. I., & Ko, I. (2010). Positive organizational psychology, behavior, and scholarship: A review 

of the emerging literature and evidence base. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 177-191. 

https://doi.org/10.1080/17439761003790930 

Eloff, I., Achoui, M., Chireshe, R., Mutepfa, M., & Ofovwe, C. (2008). Views from Africa on positive 

psychology. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 18, 189-194. 

Fineman, S. (2006). On being positive: Concerns and counterpoints. Academy of Management Review, 31, 

270-291. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2006.20208680 

Gable, S. L., & Haidt, J. (2005). What (and why) is positive psychology?. Review of General Psychology, 9, 

103-110. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.103 

Hackman, J. R. (2009). The perils of positivity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30, 309-319. 

https://doi.org/10.1002/job.587 

Ho, S. M., Chan, C. L., & Ho, R. T. (2004). Posttraumatic growth in Chinese cancer survivors. Psycho‐

Oncology, 13, 377-389. https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.758 

Hobfoll, S. E., Johnson, R. J., Canetti, D., Palmieri, P. A., Hall, B. J., Lavi, I., & Galea, S. (2012). Can people 

remain engaged and vigorous in the face of trauma? Palestinians in the West Bank and Gaza. 

Psychiatry, 75, 60-75. https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.2012.75.1.60 

Hobfoll, S. E., Palmieri, P. A., Johnson, R. J., Canetti-Nisim, D., Hall, B. J., & Galea, S. (2009). Trajectories 

of resilience, resistance, and distress during ongoing terrorism: The case of Jews and Arabs in Israel. 

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 138-148. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014360 

Hofstede, G. (1984). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values (Vol. 5). Newbury, 

CA: Sage. 

Kállay, É. (2008). Investigation of the relationship between religious growth, positive affect, and meaning 

in life in a sample of female cancer patients. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 12, 161-182. 

Keyes, C. L. M. (2003). Complete mental health: An agenda for the 21st century. In C. L. M. Keyes & J. 

Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 293–290). Washington, DC: 

American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10594-013 

King, L. A., Heintzelman, S. J., & Ward, S. J. (2016). Beyond the search for meaning: A contemporary 

science of the experience of meaning in life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 211-216. 

https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416656354 

Kormi-Nouri, R., Farahani, M. N., & Trost, K. (2013). The role of positive and negative affect on well -

being amongst Swedish and Iranian university students. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8, 435-443. 

https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2013.823511 

Lies, J., Mellor, D., & Hong, R. Y. (2014). Gratitude and personal functioning among earthquake 

survivors in Indonesia. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 9, 295-305. 

https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.902492 

Luthar, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for 

future work. Child Development, 71, 543-562. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00164 

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic 

Reviews and Meta-analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med, 6, 7, e1000097. 

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed1000097    

Oaksford, K., Frude, N., & Cuddihy, R. (2005). Positive coping and stress-related psychological growth 

following lower limb amputation. Rehabilitation Psychology, 50, 266-277. https://doi.org/10.1037/0090-

5550.50.3.266 

Pecoraro, A., Mimiaga, M. J., O'Cleirigh, C., Safren, S. A., Blokhina, E., Verbitskaya, E., . . . & Woody, G. 

(2014). Lost-to-care and engaged-in-care HIV patients in Leningrad Oblast, Russian Federation: 

Barriers and facilitators to medical visit retention. AIDS Care, 26, 1249-1257. 

https://doi.org/10.1080/09540121.2014.897910 

https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.943801
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439761003790930
https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2006.20208680
https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.103
https://doi.org/10.1002/job.587
https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.758
https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.2012.75.1.60
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014360
https://doi.org/10.1037/10594-013
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416656354
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2013.823511
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.902492
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00164
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed1000097
https://doi.org/10.1037/0090-5550.50.3.266
https://doi.org/10.1037/0090-5550.50.3.266
https://doi.org/10.1080/09540121.2014.897910


International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 69 

Rao, M. A., & Donaldson, S. I. (2015). Expanding opportunities for diversity in positive psychology: An 

examination of gender, race, and ethnicity. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 56, 271-282. 

https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000036 

Rao, M. A., Donaldson, S. I., & Doiron, K. M. (2015). Positive psychology research in the Middle East and 

North Africa. Middle East Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 60-76. 

Reitmaier, T., Bidwell, N. J., & Marsden, G. (2011). Situating digital storytelling within African 

communities. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 69, 658-668. 

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2010.12.008 

Richardson, G. E. (2002). The metatheory of resilience and resiliency. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 

307-321. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.10020 

Rusk, R. D., & Waters, L. E. (2013). Tracing the size, reach, impact, and breadth of positive psychology. 

The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8, 207-221. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2013.777766 

Seligman, P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 

55, 5-14. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5 

Selin, H., & Davey, G. (2012). Happiness across cultures: Views of happiness and quality of life in non-Western 

cultures. New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-2700-7 

Shapira, L. B., & Mongrain, M. (2010). The benefits of self-compassion and optimism exercises for 

individuals vulnerable to depression. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 377-389. 

https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2010.516763 

Shek, D. L., Sun, R. F., & Merrick, J. (Eds.). (2013). Positive youth development: Theory, research and 

application. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science. 

Sheldon, K. M., Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2011). Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and 

moving forward. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 

https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195373585.001.0001 

Shiota, M. N., Yee, C. I., O’Neil, M. J., & Danvers A. F. (2017). Positive emotions. In M. A. Warren & S. I. 

Donaldson (Eds.), Scientific advances in positive psychology (pp. 37-71). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.  

Solano, A. C. (2014). Positive psychology in Latin America. New York, NY: Springer Science + Business 

Media. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9035-2 

Strümpfer, D. W. (2005). Standing on the shoulders of giants: Notes on early positive psychology 

(psychofortology). South African Journal of Psychology, 35, 21-45. 

https://doi.org/10.1177/008124630503500102 

Tedeschi, R. G., Blevins, C. L., & Riffle, O. M. (2017). Posttraumatic growth: A brief history and 

evaluation. In M. A. Warren & S. I. Donaldson (Eds.), Scientific advances in positive psychology (pp. 131-

163). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.  

Vallerand, R. J., Blanchard, C., Mageau, G. A., Koestner, R., Ratelle, C., Léonard, M., ... & Marsolais, J. 

(2003). Les passions de l'ame: On obsessive and harmonious passion. Journal of Personality and Social 

Psychology, 85, 756-767. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.756 

Vallerand, R. J., Ntoumanis, N., Philippe, F. L., Lavigne, G. L., Carbonneau, N., Bonneville, A., . . . & 

Maliha, G. (2008). On passion and sports fans: A look at football. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26, 1279-

1293. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640410802123185 

Vázquez, C., & Hervás, G. (2010). Perceived benefits after terrorist attacks: The role of positive and 

negative emotions. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 154-163. 

https://doi.org/10.1080/17439761003630060 

Vázquez, J. J. (2013). Happiness among the garbage: Differences in overall happiness among trash 

pickers in León (Nicaragua). The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8, 1-11. 

https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2012.743574 

Warren, M. A., & Donaldson, S. I. (Eds.) (2018). Positive psychology of relationships: New directions in theory 

and research. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.  

Warren, M. A., Donaldson, S. I., & Lee, J. Y. (2018). Applying positive psychology to advance relationship 

science. In M. A. Warren & S. I. Donaldson (Eds.), Positive psychology of relationships: New directions in 

theory and research (pp. 9-34). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.  

https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000036
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2010.12.008
https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.10020
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2013.777766
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-2700-7
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2010.516763
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195373585.001.0001
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9035-2
https://doi.org/10.1177/008124630503500102
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.756
https://doi.org/10.1080/02640410802123185
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439761003630060
https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2012.743574


International landscape of positive psychology  

Kim, Doiron, Warren, & Donaldson 

 

www.internationaljournalofwellbeing.org 70 

Warren, M. A., Donaldson, S. I., & Luthans, F. (2017). Taking positive psychology to the workplace: 

Positive organizational psychology, positive organizational behavior, and positive organizational 

scholarship. In M. A. Warren & S. I. Donaldson (Eds.), Scientific advances in positive psychology (pp. 

195-227). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger. 

Watkins, P. C., & Bell, J. (2017). Current theories and research in the psychology of gratitude. In M. A. 

Warren & S. I. Donaldson (Eds.), Scientific advances in positive psychology (pp. 103-129). Santa Barbara, 

CA: Praeger.  

Wood, L., Theron, L., & Mayaba, N. (2012). “Read me to resilience:” Exploring the use of cultural stories 

to boost the positive adjustment of children orphaned by AIDS. African Journal of AIDS Research, 11, 

225-239. https://doi.org/10.2989/16085906.2012.734982 

Yehuda, R. (2002). Post-traumatic stress disorder. New England Journal of Medicine, 346, 108-114. 

https://doi.org/10.1056/nejmra012941 

 

 

https://doi.org/10.2989/16085906.2012.734982
https://doi.org/10.1056/nejmra012941