Microsoft Word - 6.Tanyer_266-288.docx


 
1This research is part of the master’s thesis completed at Anadolu University (See Tanyer (2014)).   

   

 

Tanyer, S. (2017). Domain specific beliefs about writing and 
writing performance of preservice English teachers: Is 
there any relationship? International Online Journal of 
Education and Teaching (IOJET), 4(3), 266-288. 
http://iojet.org/index.php/IOJET/article/view/191/172 

 

Received:    28.03.2017 
Received in revised form:   30.04.2017 
Accepted:   03.05.2017 

 
DOMAIN SPECIFIC BELIEFS ABOUT WRITING AND WRITING 
PERFORMANCE OF PRESERVICE ENGLISH TEACHERS: IS THERE ANY 
RELATIONSHIP?1 
 
Seray Tanyer  

Anadolu University 
seraytanyer@anadolu.edu.tr; seraytanyer@gmail.com    

 
Seray Tanyer has been working as a research and teaching assistant since 2011. She received 
her BA (2011) and MA (2014) in ELT from Hacettepe University and Anadolu University 
respectively. Recently, she has been studying for a PhD in ELT at Anadolu University. Her 
research focus areas include foreign language writing, language teacher education and second 
language acquisition. 

 
Copyright by Informascope. Material published and so copyrighted may not be published 
elsewhere without the written permission of IOJET.  



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 

266 

DOMAIN SPECIFIC BELIEFS ABOUT WRITING AND WRITING 
PERFORMANCE OF PRESERVICE ENGLISH TEACHERS: IS THERE ANY 

RELATIONSHIP? 
 

Seray Tanyer 

seraytanyer@anadolu.edu.tr; seraytanyer@gmail.com 
 

Abstract 
Learning as a retrospective phenomenon can make learners transmit their past as an ingredient 
while they are (re)structuring their present and future. Previous and present experiences can 
form a basis for cognitive, behavioral and motivational factors which can create a cognitive 
load for learners and affect their learning process. In this regard, current study aims to 
investigate first-year undergraduates’ beliefs about writing and relation of these beliefs to 
writing performance in essay writing. A total of 147 students studying in ELT department of a 
Turkish university participated in the research. Their domain-specific beliefs about writing 
were determined through the Beliefs about Writing Survey (BAWS). Writing performance 
was measured on an essay writing task by calculating both overall grade and six component 
grades. As a result, multiple regression analysis affirmed that beliefs about writing accounted 
for writing performance independently. Pearson correlation values showed that some beliefs 
about writing were adaptive and associated with higher writing scores (e.g. “Adapt to the 
Audience”). Also, some belief subcategories were associated with each other. The results of 
the present study have been discussed along with the related literature on beliefs about writing 
and writing performance. Implications/suggestions related to the coursework, writing 
practices and future research have been presented.  

Keywords: beliefs about writing, foreign language writing, writing performance, preservice 
English teachers, teacher training.  

 

1.  Introduction 
Writing, the neglected skill of foreign language education, “today is not a frill for the few, 

but an essential skill for the many” (National Commission on Writing, 2003, p. 11). We 
require this skill to flourish as a student, as an employer and as a citizen because foreign 
language writing is one of the critical components of academic foundation that students need, 
an essential prerequisite in the workplace and a critical requirement to be active citizens of a 
globalizing world. Therefore, this skill becomes valuable for different groups including 
preservice teachers of English who are potential teachers of all other groups. This assumption 
has motivated the researcher to explore quite a lot about writing discipline, and its first and 
earlier practices in undergraduate composition classes. Following this perspective, domain-
specific beliefs about writing have been chosen as the target focus in this study. 

Social cognitive theory of Albert Bandura refers to the significant role of beliefs in human 
learning and performance (Bandura, 1989). Self-efficacy beliefs are one of them which are 
mainly related to a person’s belief in oneself to perform tasks and reach particular goals (see 
Bandura (1997) for further reading). More than thirty years, research on participants from 
primary school children to university students have already reinforced the relationship 
between writing self-efficacy, writing apprehension and writing performance (e.g., McCarthy, 
Meier, & Rinderer, 1985; Meier, McCharty, & Schmeck, 1984; Pajares & Johnson, 1994; 
Pajares & Valiante, 1999; Prat-Sala & Redford, 2012; Tanyer, 2015). However, only recently, 



Tanyer 

 

267 

the social cognitive view of writing has been extended via the concept of beliefs about writing 
which has been related to writing performance as well as writing self-efficacy and 
apprehension (i.e., Sanders-Reio, 2010; Sanders-Reio, Alexander, Reio, & Newman, 2014). 
This belief category (terminologically domain specific beliefs about writing) has been built on 
studies from the fields of educational psychology, writing and rhetoric, and concentrates on 
beliefs about what good writing is, what god writers do as well as the elements related to 
writing process itself.  

The exploration of preservice English teachers’ beliefs might be crucial in terms of various 
reasons. Firstly, it is possible for them to transmit their past as an element to learning 
environments of today and future. Therefore, if preservice teachers get promoted to reflect 
their own beliefs, they can construct or reconstruct their belief systems about writing. 
Moreover, beliefs about writing can be related to learners’ writing performance as shown in 
the literature (e.g., Perry, 2011; Sanders-Reio, 2010; Sanders-Reio et al., 2014; White & 
Bruning, 2005). Also, like other foreign language skills such as speaking and listening, 
writing can be regarded as one of the neglected skills until university education. Probably 
because of this fact, this skill has been observed as a challenge for first-year preservice 
English teachers, and it would be worthy to discover what kinds of beliefs about writing are 
held by this specific group. In addition, while organizing a writing course in an EFL teacher-
training program, there may be a need to learn future teachers’ beliefs about a specific domain 
that they would teach in the future which is “writing” in this study. Identifying their personal 
beliefs and the additional details about these beliefs in depth by means of valid and reliable 
instruments might present valuable implications for teaching writing skill in a teacher 
education program.  

1.1. Literature review 
This review presents a conceptual framework for the exploration of research on beliefs 

about writing as well as a summary and discussion of the studies conducted in this area. The 
topics related to the research on writing beliefs have been ranging from innateness of writing 
skill to role of audience, mechanical and substantive writing skills, specific models of writing 
beliefs as well as transmissional, transactional and domain specific beliefs about writing. The 
first topic of empirical studies about writing beliefs concentrates on the role of giftedness in 
writing ability. With 247 undergraduates, Palmquist and Young (1992) examined the 
relationship between the beliefs in the innateness of writing ability and four other variables 
that were writing apprehension, self-assessment of writing skills, the confidence in mastering 
writing skills and genres, and previous experience with writing teachers. According to the 
results, the belief in the innateness of writing skill might “contribute to these students’ 
apprehension about writing” (Palmquist and Young, 1992, p.151). Also, the participants 
believing that writing skill was innate-gift tended to be less confident in their ability to 
become professional writers. The undergraduates carrying this belief also reported their 
experiences with their previous writing teachers less favorably. Therefore, Palmquist & 
Young (1992) concluded that the belief in innateness might provoke unprompted limitations 
about undergraduate courses and future careers which required writing skills.  

From the same point of view, Charney, Newman & Palmquist (1995) added one different 
variable that was writing performance, and investigated the relationship between beliefs about 
the innateness of writing ability, students’ assessments about their own writing, writing 
apprehension and writing performance of 446 undergraduate students. The findings revealed 
that participants believing that writing could be learned were more likely to enjoy writing 
although they did not accordingly score higher on their writing assignments. Also, 
participants enjoying writing more tended to label themselves as good writers, and the female 
participants were prone to regard writing skill as something that could be learned. In addition, 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 

268 

they enjoyed writing more, and scored higher in writing assignments than the male ones. As 
suggested by the two studies above, the belief in giftedness might have prejudicial effects on 
student writers and trigger limitations in writing career (Palmquist & Young, 1992). 
Therefore, it would be valuable to question this belief in that writing lecturers may need to 
become aware of it and combat its negative effects. 

Another perspective in beliefs about writing research deals with the role of audience in 
writing process. The analysis of audience is an important element of planning process because 
it decides the format, the language, the information included, and even the use of figures and 
graphics. In this regard, Nelson (2008) indicated the role and importance of audience and 
addressed three issues that were “writing related to reading, writer related to reader, and text 
related to text” regarding them as central to written discourse (p. 547). Her study revealed that 
the participants considered readers and writers interacting in a bilateral aim instead of isolated 
existences. It was indicated that the products of writers varied according to the different types 
of audience by means of an adaptation process. Nelson also noted the evidence of 
developmental differences between younger and older students. 

The four other researchers, Miller and Chorney (2008) and Beach and Friedrich (2006), 
focused on three main dimensions of writing that are persuasion, audience and argument, and 
discussed how writers adapted the organization, content, tone of their arguments according to 
target audience, their age, their assumed attitudes, and to the specific discourse community. 
Also, emphasizing the significance and pervasiveness of writing in the workplace, Beaufort 
(2008) researched how writers in workplace adapted their voice, tone, level of clarity, and 
stated that the writers had decided on their choice of words in response to the power groups. 
As claimed by Miller and Charney (2008), the notion of influencing and persuading readers 
has been emphasized since Aristotle. However, in school environment, the written products 
that students produce may not address a real aim or a real audience. For this reason, it can be 
valuable to question students’ beliefs about this inauthenticity and the role of audience in a 
foreign language environment.  

Mechanical and substantive skills are two other components of writing skill which address 
different issues: while mechanical skills are interested in grammar, spelling, punctuation and 
style; substantive skills attend to organization, development, clarity, and cohesion. The 
differences between these two skill types can be observable in research. In their study of how 
students’ beliefs about writing affect the product and writing process, Graham, Schwartz & 
MacArthur (1993) made a distinction between mechanical and substantive skills. Via open-
ended interviews, the 4th, 5th,7th and 8th grade students with and without learning disabilities 
were asked about their beliefs and knowledge with respect to what good writing was, what 
good writers did, why some students had trouble in writing, and how they would write a paper 
for a younger child. The participants were also asked to evaluate a text written by a child by 
employing their knowledge and beliefs about writing. The findings revealed that the better 
writers who were also older and normally achieving students tended to highlight substantive 
skills instead of mechanical skills in their definitions of good writing. In their accounts of who 
good writers were and what they did, these participants emphasized writing processes over 
written product. To sum up, Graham, Schwartz & MacArthur (1993) claimed that “The 
knowledge, attitudes, and beliefs that students hold about writing play an important part in 
determining how the composing process is carried out and what the eventual shape of the 
written product will be” (p.246). As writing teachers, if we uncover the students’ beliefs about 
mechanical and substantive issues, these beliefs might be (re)shaped by writing instruction 
that they would receive.  

Some scholars have also started to develop models on beliefs about writing combining 
different variables. The earlier empirical study of beliefs about writing has been published by 



Tanyer 

 

269 

Silva and Nicholls (1993) who designed their scales based on six traditions of discourse 
theory. Based on these traditions, Silva and Nicholls included some goals and beliefs in two 
different scales (i.e., “Writing Goals” and “Beliefs about the Causes of Success in Writing”). 
They also applied three more scales that were “Intrinsic Commitment to Writing”, “Dualism 
Scale” and “Perceived Ability Scale”. The results revealed that students with beliefs referring 
to substantive issues liked writing more than the ones holding beliefs stressing “Surface 
Correctness and Form”. Silva & Nicholls (1993) also stated that beliefs about writing might 
reflect writing teachers’ styles and classroom culture. Therefore, as in the Bandura’s model, 
the effect of environment on the person could be observed in writing classrooms, as well.  

In addition to those above, Ellen Lavelle has published a number of studies about students’ 
approaches to writing (e.g., Lavelle, 1993, 2001, 2003; Lavelle & Guarino, 2003; Lavelle, 
Smith & O’Ryan, 2002; Lavelle & Zuercher, 2001). She started her research by developing a 
questionnaire, The Inventory of Processes in College Composition (IPCC; Lavelle, 1993), a 
factor analysis of which has provided five different writing approaches of college students: 
Elaborationist Approach, Low Self-Efficacy Approach, Reflective-Revisionist Approach, 
Impulsive Approach and Procedural Approach. Those five approaches were collected under 
two broad categories as deep and surface approaches (Lavelle & Guarino, 2003). Writers 
taking a deep approach are more meta-cognitive, more involved in their writing and regard 
themselves as a real operator in meaning making. These writers tell a strong sense of 
audience, carry holistic views of writing tasks, and they are directed more toward meaning of 
the written product than form. For them, revision is an important part of writing process, and 
they tend to revise and reflect thoroughly on their product. Writers taking the surface 
approach are less aware of writing process and audience, and they are less dedicated to their 
written product. Writing is not a learning source for them; they are more rule-bound, focus on 
mechanical errors in writing, and instead of revising, they edit their work at the surface level. 

As well as the models mentioned above, White and Bruning (2005) adapted the earlier 
works of Schraw and Bruning (1996, 1999) to writing which investigated transmissional and 
transactional beliefs about reading. According to the new model, writers with high 
transmissional beliefs see writing principally as a channel of transmitting authoritative 
knowledge to readers with minimum addition of writer’s own perspective while writers with 
high transactional beliefs view writing as a medium to combine what they have learned with 
their own knowledge and perspectives. In order to measure transmissional and transactional 
beliefs about writing, White and Bruning (2005) developed the Writing Beliefs Inventory and 
examined the relations among 170 undergraduates’ beliefs about writing, writing self-
efficacy, writing apprehension, past writing experiences and writing performance. The results 
indicated that beliefs about writing were related to writing performance because the 
participants with transmissional beliefs had significantly lower writing scores while those 
with transactional beliefs had higher writing scores. Students with high transmissional scores 
also had less affective and cognitive engagement with writing and were less likely to write for 
pleasure. On the other hand, students with high transactional scores spent more time for 
writing and were more likely to find writing pleasurable.  

More recently, Bruning, Dempsey, Kauffman & Zumbrunn (2011) extended the work of 
White and Bruning (2005) by revising Writing Beliefs Inventory and surveyed 556 eleventh 
graders from two high schools to investigate the relationship among implicit beliefs about 
writing, affects towards writing, writing self-efficacy, writing grades and statewide writing 
assessment scores as well as English/Language Arts course enrollment. Results demonstrated 
that transactional beliefs were significantly related to liking writing, self-efficacy for writing 
ideation, self-efficacy for writing conventions, self-efficacy for writing self-regulation, self-
reported grades, and the statewide writing assessment score. Also, students in more advanced 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 

270 

classes of English/Language Arts course had higher transactional beliefs and lower 
transmissional beliefs. Following that study, Perry (2011) investigated 153 college students’ 
implicit beliefs about a specific writing task and associations of those beliefs to writing score 
with the measures of “Writing Habits and Beliefs Scale”, “Writing Beliefs Inventory-
Revised” (Bruning et al., 2011), “Liking Writing Scale” and “Beliefs about Intelligence” 
scale. Results revealed that college students held implicit beliefs about the specific writing 
task and those beliefs were related to liking writing and beliefs about intelligence. However, 
transmissional and transactional beliefs did not affect scores of writing task. All in all, this 
study indicated that learners approached “writing with a unique set of beliefs, assumptions, 
and motivations”, and they entered “the classroom with a wide variety of skill sets, 
experiences, and prior knowledge” (Perry, 2011, p.96).  

Referring to a scarcity, Sanders-Reio (2010) indicated that while investigation on writing 
self-efficacy beliefs and its relation to writing apprehension and writing performance had 
started to become accumulated, the research area of beliefs about writing was limited. 
Therefore, she examined the association between domain specific beliefs about writing, 
writing self-efficacy and writing apprehension, and their relations to writing performance on a 
three-phase study. The first two phases were assigned to instrument construction and 
validation while the last phase investigated the relations among the target variables. The 
participants were 207 pre-service teachers studying in the College of Education, and the 
measures were “Beliefs about Writing Survey”, the modified “Writing Self-Efficacy Scale” 
(Zimmerman and Bandura, 1994), the modified “Writing Apprehension Test” (Daly and 
Miller, 1975), and a demographic survey. Writing performance of the preservice teachers was 
assessed via a structured five-page paper written for the educational psychology course.  

The results revealed that four of the beliefs about writing – Expert Orientation, Writing 
Supports Thinking, Address Substantive Issues First and Mechanical Errors Are Shameful - 
appeared to be adaptive in that they positively correlated with all or some of the grades for 
writing performance or with other adaptive beliefs. Expert Orientation also correlated 
positively with writing self-efficacy and enjoyment for writing while Writing Support 
Thinking had the highest correlation to enjoyment of writing. Hierarchical regression analysis 
revealed that beliefs about writing independently explained 12% of the variance in writing 
performance. Apprehension about making grammatical and other mechanical errors had a 
strong negative effect on writing performance. Lastly, after controlling for domain specific 
writing beliefs, writing self-efficacy weakly predicted writing performance.  

A following study of Sanders-Reio et al. (2014) also followed Kellogg’s (2008) cognitive 
model of writing development and investigated the relations among beliefs about writing, 
writing self-efficacy, writing apprehension and writing performance. “The Beliefs About 
Writing Survey”, “the Writing Self-Efficacy Index” and the modified “Writing Apprehension 
Test” were administered to a total of 738 undergraduates, and writing performance was 
evaluated based on a class paper. According to the findings, beliefs about writing accounted 
for writing scores significantly and, while the beliefs, Audience Orientation and Recursive 
Process, were the positive predictors of the scores, Transmission and Transaction were the 
negative predictors. As for the other variables, the writing self-efficacy positively and 
apprehension about grammar negatively predicted writing performance while writing 
apprehension as a block was not a significant predictor.  

More recently, Tanyer & Subaşı (2016) conducted a qualitative study and investigated 26 
preservice English teachers’ beliefs about EFL writing via interviews. In the study, the 
participants’ beliefs about good writing and writers, writing ability and the factors affecting 
their beliefs about good writing were interrogated. As a result, it was found that participants 
approached good writing and writer characteristics and the nature of writing ability with 



Tanyer 

 

271 

particular belief sets. The scholars also interpreted their findings according to the writing 
scores gathered by the participants. Based on these interpretations, they claimed that the 
relationship between writing beliefs and writing performance had been reinforced by their 
study.  

All in all, the findings above support the possibility that writing beliefs can be an 
influential variable while teaching writing skill. Following the research above dealing with 
domain-specific beliefs about writing, the present study has two main purposes. Firstly, it 
aims at investigating first year undergraduates’ domain-specific beliefs about writing. 
Secondly, it questions the role of these beliefs in undergraduates’ writing performance. In line 
with these purposes, it has been assumed that this study would provide a profile of preservice 
English teachers by reporting their beliefs about what good writing is, what good writers do in 
addition to the writing processes, writing tasks, writing skills and the procedures these tasks 
and skills involve. The research questions guiding the study are as follows: 

RQ1. Do preservice English teachers hold any domain specific beliefs about writing? 
RQ2. Is there any relationship between domain specific beliefs about writing and writing 
performance? 
RQ3. Do domain specific beliefs about writing predict writing performance? 

2. Methodology 
2.1. Participants 
The participants included 147 first-year preservice teachers studying in the ELT 

Department of a Turkish university. In this department, students must take two main writing 
courses in their first year (i.e. Written Communication and Academic Writing and Report 
Writing). In this study, the ones from all eight sections of “Academic Writing and Report 
Writing Course” participated. Each section of that course comprised almost 30 students; 
however, the ones that had failed in previous years and retook the course were excluded from 
the analysis. Most of the participants were female with the proportion of 74.8% (N=110), 
while 25.2% of them were male (N=37). Additionally, the average age of them was 19.69 
(SD= 2.12).  

There are several reasons for the selection of this population of interest in this research. 
Firstly, they receive considerable amount of practice and instruction in writing through two 
semesters, so learning more about them and undergraduate writing might facilitate the 
development of writing instruction for this sample. The second reason is that the participation 
of this sample would facilitate comparison with and the extension of much of the existing 
research (e.g., Bruning et. al., 2011; Sanders-Reio, 2010; Sanders-Reio et al., 2014; White & 
Bruning, 2005) about pre-service teachers’ domain specific beliefs about writing. 

2.2. Instruments 
In the present study, two data sources were combined which were a recent survey of 

beliefs about writing and writing scores. The survey administered and measurement of writing 
performance have been described below.  

2.2.1. Survey 
The participants indicated their domain-specific beliefs about foreign language writing on 

a five-point likert scale which was The Beliefs about Writing Scale (BAWS; Sanders-Reio, 
2010). BAWS had specifically been designed with Hispanic first-year preservice teachers 
who were enrolled in College of Education in south Florida, USA. The original BAWS 
comprised 76 items with 14 subscales, although a four subscales-version of it have recently 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 

272 

been developed and used by Sanders-Reio et al. (2014). In the current study, the first version 
with 76 items and 14 subscales were administered.  

Beyond the beliefs about foreign language writing, the survey battery also asked 
participants to provide relevant background information about themselves such as their age, 
gender and year of study. The respondents were also required to reveal some more details 
such as their attitudes toward foreign language writing and their past educational experiences. 
Some items of the instrument theorized to be vague for the first-year preservice teachers were 
disambiguated with their synonyms or with some examples. Moreover, three writing 
instructors all of whom were experts in ELT and lecturing in ELT Department reviewed the 
survey battery and approved the modifications. The comparison between the initial and 
modified version of the items have been listed in Table 1: 

Table 1. The original and modified version of five items of BAWS 

Original Modified 
9. It’s important to develop a distinctive 
writing style. 
19. Writers need to immerse themselves in 
their writing.  
39. Papers with typos are terrible 
embarrassment.  
45. During revision, one should carefully 
check one’s manuscripts for both 
substantive and mechanical problems. 
  
46. Good writers demonstrate their skills at 
crafting complex sentences.  
69. It’s humiliating to give a PowerPoint 
presentation with typos and misspellings.  

9. It’s important to develop a distinctive 
(peculiar, original) writing style. 
19. Writers need to immerse themselves in 
(involve deeply in) their writing. 
39. Papers with typos (misspellings) are 
terrible embarrassment. 
45. During revision, one should carefully 
check one’s manuscripts for both 
substantive (organization, development 
etc.) and mechanical (grammar, spelling 
etc.) problems. 
46. Good writers demonstrate their skills at 
crafting (creating) complex sentences. 
69. It’s humiliating to give an essay with 
typos and misspellings. 

2.2.2. Writing Performance 

Writing performance was assessed via the grades participant received on the papers they 
wrote for the Academic Writing and Report Writing Course. During an in-class exam, the 
students were provided with three optional writing prompts and were free to choose any of 
them for essay writing. Also, based on their topic, they were supposed to decide the genre 
type which they would write in using APA citation techniques appropriately. The papers were 
assessed analytically via ESL Composition Profile of Jacobs, Hartfield, Hughey and Vormuth 
(1981) which includes five main rows: Content, Organization, Vocabulary, Language Use, 
and Mechanics. As Andrade & Boulay (2003) have argued, using such profile for assessment 
can support learning and development of writing skills by laying out clear, concrete 
characteristics of good writing.  

2.3. Data Collection and Analysis Procedures 
2.3.1. The Survey 

At the beginning of spring semester, randomly selected thirty-two (32) preservice teachers 
studying in ELT Department firstly indicated their domain specific beliefs about foreign 
language writing via BAWS. The purpose of applying this instrument to that small group was 
to discover whether the items were clear and definite for the respondents. As no negative 
feedback was received and there had been no additional modification on the survey, the data 



Tanyer 

 

273 

gathered from this quite small sample were combined with the main study. Thus, a total of 
147 participants answered the survey battery in a class hour. 

The ELT Department demands their candidates to be at B2 level according to the standard 
language levels of Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR) while 
accepting them to the department. This level was assumed to be appropriate for preservice 
teachers to comprehend the items of the BAWS by the faculty members. For this reason, the 
original English version of the survey was applied during the data collection process. 
Furthermore, the preservice teachers were demanded to approve their voluntarily participation 
to complete the survey package by signing a consent form. In that consent form, the 
participants were enlightened about the purpose of the research and asked for their 
permission.  

2.3.2. Writing Performance 
As for the writing performance, the participants’ first midterm exam scores were taken into 

consideration. During an in-class exam, the preservice teachers wrote their own essays. In the 
exam paper, the students were provided with three optional writing prompts and were free to 
choose any of them. Also, based on their topic, they were supposed to decide the genre type 
they would write.  

2.3.3. Data Analysis 
A number of statistical calculations have been performed in order to address the research 

questions. For RQ1 (i.e., Do preservice English teachers hold any domain specific beliefs 
about writing?), descriptive statistics such as means and standard deviations were used. For 
RQ2 (i.e. What is the relation between beliefs about writing and writing performance?), the 
Pearson correlations were computed and analyzed between the independent (beliefs about 
writing) and dependent (writing performance) variables. To answer RQ3 (i.e. Do domain 
specific beliefs about writing predict writing performance?), a standard multiple regression 
analysis was employed to determine the unique variance explained by beliefs about writing in 
writing performance.  

3. Results 
In the following headings, reliability of measures, descriptive statistics, and the findings of 

correlational and standard multiple regression analyses have been presented.  
3.1. Reliability of measures 
To assure that the Beliefs about Writing Scale (BAWS) was a reliable measure, internal 

consistency of measures was used by computing the Cronbach’s alphas for the total scale and 
all subscales (see Table 2). The Cronbach’s alpha for the total scale was .855, which was 
close to the entire value of original BAWS (i.e. 0.87; Sanders-Reio, 2010, p. 116). The 
Cronbach’s alphas for the fourteen subscales of BAWS ranged from .524 to .797, which had 
ranged from .61 to .80 in the original scale. As eliminating any items did not provide higher 
reliability, none of the items had been removed from the scale.  
Table 2. Reliability coefficients for BAWS and its subscales 

 N of items Cronbach’s α 
1. Transmissional 6 .569 
2. Writing Supports Thinking 4 .797 
3. Writing Is a Personal and Emotional Experience 6 .653 
4. Writing Is an Innate Gift 5 .762 
5. Basics (Mechanics) First 4 .578 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 

274 

6. Address Substantive Issues First 5 .524 
7. Writing Is an Iterative Process 8 .597 
8. Minimize Revision 7 .604 
9. Write to Impress 4 .609 
10. Use Plain English 6 .638 
11. Adapt to the Audience 8 .664 
12. Clarity Is Essential 3 .532 
13. Development Is Important  5 .589 
14. Mechanical Errors Are Shameful 5 .778 
      Beliefs about Writing Survey (TOTAL) 76 .855 

Writing performance was assessed via the scores participants received on the essays they 
wrote for the first midterm exam on three optional topics. As for the reliability of writing 
scores, two graders each of whom were experienced instructors of writing and had been 
lecturing for at least fifteen years evaluated the participants’ papers following the dimensions 
of ESL Composition Profile (Jacobs et. al., 1981). The mean of the two graders’ scores was 
used in the study. Based on these two score sets, a correlational analysis was operated in order 
to calculate the inter-rater reliability as instructed by Gay (1992). The correlation value 
between the total scores given by the two scorers was .94. 

3.2. Descriptive statistics 
The values below demonstrate descriptive statistics about the participants’ profile, past 

educational and writing experiences in addition to means and standard deviations of the 
subscales.  

   Figure 1. Do you like writing in English? (N=147) 

 
                               

The first question, Do you like writing in English?, inquired participants’ attitudes toward 
foreign language writing. As revealed by Figure 1, most of the students (77.6%) seem to have 
developed a positive attitude toward writing in English, while the rest 22.4% of them reported 
that they did not like writing in English.  

The remaining open-ended questions provided us some more details about the participants’ 
past writing practices in English. For example, the hours of English courses they took in a 
week during high school ranged from 2 to 14 hours with a mean of 10.71. But, when the 
course hours allocated for foreign language writing was searched, it was indicated that 67.3% 
of the participants (N=99) had not done any practices of writing in English at high school (see 
Figure 2 below). The time allocated for writing practice for the rest of the participants was 
also quite limited varying between one (1) and five (5) hours with a decreasing proportion 
from 17.7% to 0.7% respectively.  

 
 

 

77,6%

22,4%

Yes

No



Tanyer 

 

275 

Figure 2. How many hours of English courses in a week were allocated to improve your 
writing skills at high school? (N=147). 

 
In Table 3, the means and standard deviations of the fourteen (14) subscales in BAWS 

were demonstrated. The subscales were ordered in a descending order from the one with the 
highest average to the one with the lowest average.  

Table 3. Means and standard deviations of the subscales in BAWS (N=147) 

 M SD 
1. Development Is Important 4,125 0,424 
2. Clarity Is Essential 4,063 0,571 
3. Writing Supports Thinking 4,056 0,663 
4. Adapt to the Audience 3,996 0,437 
5. Writing Is an Iterative Process 3,938 0,427 
6. Writing Is a Personal and Emotional Experience 3,818 0,524 
7. Address Substantive Issues First 3,693 0,574 
8. Writing Is an Innate Gift 3,449 0,805 
9. Basics (Mechanics) First 3,449 0,635 
10. Use Plain English 3,353 0,554 
11. Write to Impress 3,258 0,727 
12. Transmissional 2,997 0,584 
13. Mechanical Errors Are Shameful 2,851 0,793 
14. Minimize Revision 2,409 0,524 
 

As presented in Table 3, the beliefs that were Development Is Important, Clarity Is 
Essential and Writing Supports Thinking had the highest means while the beliefs, 
Transmissional, Mechanical Errors Are Shameful and Minimize Revision, had the lowest 
mean scores. The other eight belief categories about writing were in between these two outlier 
sets. Descriptive statistics showed that the upper three beliefs were highly agreed by the 
participants; on the contrary, the last three beliefs seemed to be disagreed. 

In Table 4 (below), the results of one-sample t-test that had compared the averages of each 
subscale with the middle value (i.e. 3) were presented. Among the subscales which were 
agreed, the highest average belonged to the belief, Development Is Important (M= 4.125), 
while the lowest average belonged to the belief that writers should Write To Impress the 
audience (M= 3,258). Although the lowest average had a mean of 3,258, this value was 
higher than the middle value (i.e., 3) with a t-value of 4.307 and a probability value of .000. 

67,3%

17,7%
9,5%

2,7% 2,0% 0,7%
0%

10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
90%

100%

Never 1 Hour 2 Hours 3 Hours 4 Hours 5 Hours



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 

276 

Therefore, it was found that the participants agreed all the first eleven (11) beliefs about 
writing listed below. 
Table 4. Summaries of one-sample t-test comparing the averages of variables with the middle 
value of the likert scale (i.e. 3) (N=147) 

Variable Mean SD t df p < 
1. Development Is Important 4,125 0,424 32.120 146 .000 
2. Clarity Is Essential 4,063 0,571 22.575 146 .000 
3. Writing Supports Thinking 4,056 0,663 19.288 146 .000 
4. Adapt to the Audience 3,996 0,437 27.639 146 .000 
5. Writing Is an Iterative Process 3,938 0,427 26.632 146 .000 
6. Writing Is a Personal and Emotional Experience 3,818 0,524 18.910 146 .000 
7. Address Substantive Issues First 3,693 0,574 14.656 146 .000 
8. Writing Is an Innate Gift 3,449 0,805 6.757 146 .000 
9. Basics (Mechanics) First 3,449 0,635 8.564 146 .000 
10. Use Plain English 3,353 0,554 7.731 146 .000 
11. Write to Impress 3,258 0,727 4.307 146 .000 
12. Transmissional 2,997 0,584 -.047 146 .963 
13. Mechanical Errors Are Shameful 2,851 0,793 -2.265 146 .025 
14. Minimize Revision 2,409 0,524 -13.668 146 .000 
 

On the other hand, the last two beliefs were significanly disagreed which were Mechanical 
Errors Are Shameful (M=2.851) and writers should Minimize Revision (M=2.409) whose 
means were lower than the middle value (i.e. 3) with t-values of (-2.265) and (-13.668) 
respectively. Also, they had probability values of (.025) and (.000) which were lower than 
.05. Therefore, it was apparent that respondents disagreed with these beliefs. The last belief, 
Transmissional (M=2.997), was not agreed or disagreed by the subjects. In other words, they 
were unsure about this belief since the mean of it was so close to the middle value of the scale 
(i.e. 3). Additionally, it had a very low t-value (-.047) and the probability value of it (i.e., 
.963) was not lower than .05. In this regard, the pre-service English teachers participating in 
the present study were found to be unsure about the Transmissional belief category.  

3.3. Correlations 
In order to answer Research Question 2 (i.e. Is there any relationship between beliefs about 

writing and writing performance?), Pearson correlations among dependent variables (i.e. the 
subscales of the BAWS), and the Pearson correlations between the dependent variables and 
the independent variable (i.e. writing performance) were computed. 

3.3.1. Correlations Among the Subscales of the BAWS 

Firstly, the correlational values among the dependent variables have been reported, and the 
correlation matrix in Table 5 signified that a number of subscales had been statistically 
significantly correlated among each other and with writing performance. Some previous 
studies have theorized (e.g., White & Bruning, 2005; Sanders-Reio, 2010) that beliefs about 
writing can be adaptive since “they either reflect expert practice, support writing process, 
and/or tend to be as associated with better grades on writing assignment” (p.151). The belief 
categories theorized to be adaptive were: 1) Adapt to the Audience, 2) Clarity Is Essential, 3) 
Development Is Important, 4) Writing Is An Iterative Process, 5) Use Plain English, 6) 
Substantive Issues First, 7) Writing Supports Thinking and 8) Writing Is A Personal and 
Emotional Experience. 



Tanyer 

 

277 

In the current study, Adapt To The Audience significantly and positively correlated with all 
of the theorized adaptive beliefs. This means that preservice teachers believing that writing 
should be adapted to the audience were more likely to believe that Development Is Important 
(r =.56, p < .01), Clarity Is Essential (r =.54, p < .01), Writing Is An Iterative Process (r 
=.47, p < .01),  Writing Supports Thinking (r =.31, p < .01),  foreign language writers should 
Use Plain English (r =.29, p < .01), Writing Is A Personal and Emotional Experience (r =.26, 
p < .01) and that writers should Address Substantive Issues First (r =.24, p < .01). To the 
contrary, first-year preservice teachers subscribing to the belief, Adapt To The Audience, were 
more likely to hold three of the maladaptive beliefs which were Basics (Mechanics) First (r 
=.303, p < .01), Transmissional (r =.24, p < .01), and Write to Impress (r =.20, p < .01). 

Second adaptive belief correlating significantly and positively with all theorized adaptive 
beliefs is Development Is Important. Therefore, those believing that writers should explain 
their thoughts effectively were more likely to believe that Clarity Is Essential (r =.62, p < 
.01), writers should Adapt To The Audience (r =.56, p < .01), Writing Is An Iterative Process 
(r =.50, p < .01), writers should Use Plain English (r =.37, p < .01), Writing Is A Personal 
and Emotional Experience (r =.34, p < .01), Writing Supports Thinking (r =.31, p < .01), and 
writers should Address Substantive Issues First (r =.28, p < .01). In contrast, participants 
subscribing to this belief (i.e., Development Is Important) were more likely to believe that 
writers should master Basics (Mechanics) First (r =.38, p < .01), and should Write to Impress 
(r =.18, p < .01) the reader.  

The third writing belief subcategory theorized to be adaptive is Clarity Is Essential. 
According to the Table 5, the respondents believing that foreign language writers should 
convey information clearly were more inclined to believe that Development Is Important (r 
=.56, p < .01), writers should Adapt To The Audience (r =.54, p < .01), Writing Supports 
Thinking (r =.39, p < .01), Writing Is An Iterative Process (r =.39, p < .01), writers should 
Use Plain English (r =.34, p < .01), Writing Is A Personal and Emotional Experience (r =.31, 
p < .01), and that writers should Address Substantive Issues First (r =.28, p < .01). On the 
contrary, the subjects believing that Clarity Is Essential were more likely to believe that 
writers should master Basics (Mechanics) First (r =.30, p < .01) and should transmit 
authorities’ words into their writing (Transmissional, r =.23, p < .01).  

Five beliefs about writing that are Transmissional, Write to Impress, Minimize Revision, 
Writing Is An Innate Gift and Basics (Mechanics) First are theorized to be maladaptive for 
they contradict with “expert writing practice” and have a tendency to link to lower scores on 
writing tasks (Sanders-Reio, 2010; p. 154). In the current study, three of these maladaptive 
beliefs, Transmissional, Write to Impress and Basics (Mechanics) First, correlated with other 
three maladaptive beliefs. The first one, Transmissional, positively and significantly 
correlated with Write to Impress (r =.30, p < .01), Minimize Revision (r =.23, p < .01) and 
Basics (Mechanics) First (r =.18, p < .05). This means that participants who regarded writing 
as a means of transmitting scholarly information to the audience with almost no contribution 
also believed that writers should Write To Impress and Minimize Revision while writing.  

Minimize Revision supports the notion that writers write it appropriately in the first time 
and need no revision, and stands as a counterpoint to the concept of expert orientation, 
especially to the belief that Writing Is An Iterative Process. This belief correlated positively 
and significantly with the other beliefs theorized to be maladaptive including Transmissional 
(r =.23, p < .01), Write To Impress (r =.20, p < .05) and Writing Is An Innate Gift (r =.19, p 
< .05). As anticipated, it also significantly and negatively associated with Writing Is an 
Iterative Process (r =.19, p < .05). The belief, Write to Impress, which suggested that writers 
should use big words and attract their readers, was also correlated with the three beliefs. 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 

278 

Table 5. Intercorrelations among the subscales of beliefs about writing survey and writing performance 

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 

1. Transmissional --              
2. Writing Supports 
Thinking .090 --             

3. Writing is a Personal 
Emotional Experience .216

** .265** --            

4. Writing is an Innate Gift .107 -.164* .136 --           

5. Basics (Mechanics) First .186* .130 .069 .156 --          
6. Address Substantive 
Issues First -.016 .132 .244

** .047 -.208* --         

7. Writing is an Iterative 
Process .188

* .378** .282** -.057 .300** .137 --        

8. Minimize Revision .232** -.131 .122 .194* .048 .220** -.192* --       

9. Write to Impress .303** .128 .325** .214** .118 .081 .152 .205* --      

10. Use Plain English .234** -.023 .102 .214** .160 .204* .273** .077 -.069 --     
11. Adapt to the Audience .244** .313** .268** .051 .303** .245** .475** -.025 .209* .299** --    

12. Clarity is Essential .233** .394** .314** .055 .309** .284** .391** -.071 .147 .349** .543** --   
13. Development Is 
Important .159 .315

** .349** .100 .389** .282** .501** -.077 .189
* .377** .568** .626** --  

14. Mechanical Errors are 
Shameful .159 -.018 -.011 .174

* .391** -.234** .000 .022 .217
** .106 .099 .032 .068 -- 

15. Overall Grade -.085 .016 .053 -.135 -.022 .153 .170* -.128 .026 .107 .269
** .136 .171* .118 

               ** Correlation is significant at p < .01.  *Correlation is significant at p < .05.  



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 
 

279 

theorized to be maladaptive that were Transmissional (r =.30, p < .01), Writing Is An Innate 
Gift (r =.21, p < .01) and Minimize Revision (r =.20, p < .05). 

The other maladaptive belief, Writing Is An Innate Gift, was also positively and 
significantly correlated with Minimize Revision (r =.19, p < .05) and Write To Impress (r 
=.21, p < .01). In contrast, this belief also negatively associated with the belief of Writing 
Supports Thinking (r =.16, p < .05) which proposes that writing can help writers better 
understand what they think. An interesting result was that Basics (Mechanics) First 
correlated positively with only one maladaptive belief which was Transmissional (r =.18, p 
< .05) while correlating positively with four adaptive beliefs, in which the correlational 
values ranged from .30 to .38 (p < .01).  

The last belief, Mechanical Errors Are Shameful, was not theorized as adaptive or 
maladaptive. In our study this belief correlated positively and significantly with three 
maladaptive beliefs including Basics (Mechanics) First (r =.39, p < .01), Write To Impress (r 
=.21, p < .01) and Writing Is An Innate Gift (r =.17, p < .05). It also negatively and 
significantly associated with Address Substantive Issues First (r =.23, p < .01) which had 
been theorized as an adaptive belief in the literature.  

3.3.2. Correlations Between the Independent Variables and Writing Performance 
One of the characteristics of adaptive beliefs is that they tend to match with higher scores 

on writing tasks (Sanders-Reio, 2010). As seen in Table 5, the belief that writers should 
Adapt To The Audience was significantly and positively correlated with overall writing 
performance (r=.26, p< .01), which proposes that those who put emphasis on audience-
orientation were more probable to receive higher grades on their written work. In addition to 
the audience adaptation, the belief, Development Is Important, positively and significantly 
correlated with the overall grade (r=.17, p< .05). This result suggested that those believing 
that writers should present logical and convincing arguments were more likely to score higher 
just like the ones believing that audience-adaptation was significant. Lastly, the belief, 
Writing Is An Iterative Process, correlated positively with the overall score (r=.17, p < .05). 
These results indicated that students subscribing to the beliefs held by expert writers such as 
audience-orientation, presenting logical and convincing arguments as well as the ones 
viewing writing as a process of reviewing were inclined to have higher scores on their papers. 
On the other hand, although some beliefs such as Minimize Revision (r= -.12, NS), 
Transmissional (r=-.85, NS) and Basics Mechanics First (r= -.02, NS) negatively correlated 
with writing performance, these correlational values were quite low and not significant. 

In sum, the exploration of the correlations between the beliefs about writing and overall 
writing performance justifies the view that some beliefs are prone to be adaptive while others 
are tendentious to be maladaptive although the correlational values were not statistically 
significant in our findings regarding the maladaptive beliefs. 

3.4. Standard Multiple Regression 

Standard multiple regression analysis was conducted to decide the variance in writing 
performance explained by domain specific beliefs about writing totally and individually. The 
prediction model summarized in Table 6 (below) was statistically significant 
F(14,132)=2.192, p < .05 and accounted for approximately 18.9% of variance of writing 
score (R2 = .189, Adjusted R2 = .103). This value is fairly higher in comparison with Sanders-
Reio’s (2010) outcomes in which all beliefs about writing explained 11.8% of the variance 
(p<.001). 

 



Tanyer 
 

    
 

280 

Table 6. Standard multiple regression analysis results 

Model b SE-b Beta 
Pearson r 

(β) sr
2 Structure 

Coefficient 
    Constant 41.167 14.484     
1. Transmissional -3.331 2.016 -.150 -.085 .016 -.195 
2. Writing Supports Thinking -2.761 1.829 -.141 .016 .013 .036 
3. Writing Personal and Emotional .465 2.276 .019 .053 .000 .122 
4. Writing Is an Innate Gift * -2.843 1.403 -.176 -.135 .025 -.311 
5. Basics (Mechanics) First -2.738 2.114 -.134 -.022 .010 -.050 
6. Substantive Issues First 2.630 2.213 .116 .153 .008 .352 
7. Writing Is an Iterative Process 2.596 3.155 .085 .170 .004 .391 
8. Minimize Revision  -2.160 2.294 -.087 -.128 .005 -.294 
9. Write to Impress  .493 1.677 .028 .026 .000 .059 
10. Use Plain English .977 2.263 .042 .107 .001 .246 
11. Mechanical Errors Are 
Shameful* 3.489 1.492 .213 .118 .033 .271 

12. Development Is Important .847 3.674 .028 .171 .000 .394 
13. Clarity Is Essential .336 2.578 .015 .136 .000 .313 
14. Adapt to the Audience* 7.741 3.126 .260 .269 .037 .619 

According to Table 6, writing scores were primarily predicted by the belief category of 
Adapt to The Audience (β=.26, p<.05), and to a lesser extent by two other subcategories, that 
were Mechanical Errors Are Shameful (β=.21, p<.05) and Writing Is An Innate Gift (β=-.17, 
p<.05). Also, the raw and standardized regression coefficients of the predictors together with 
their correlations with the writing performance, their squared semi-partial correlations and 
their structure coefficients were shown in the table. The belief category, Adapt To The 
Audience, received the strongest weight in the model followed by Mechanical Errors Are 
Shameful and Writing Is An Innate Gift. Overall, the model indicated that higher Adapt To 
The Audience and Mechanical Errors Are Shameful scores predicted higher writing grades 
while the higher Writing Is An Innate Gift scores predicted lower writing grades in our 
research sample. 
4. Discussion 

This current research aimed to identify preservice English teachers’ domain specific 
beliefs about writing and examine the relations between various writing beliefs and writing 
performance. The research literature on writing beliefs does not go far away in time. 
Moreover, it is limited and related to different samples and disciplines such as reading 
research (e.g., White & Burning, 2005), writing pedagogy (e.g., Silva & Nichols, 1993), early 
childhood and/or elementary teacher education (e.g., Graham, Schwartz & MacArthur, 1993). 
Only a recent study of Sanders-Reio (2010) focused specifically on writing processes and 
practices of expert writer candidates. Following her, the current study attempted to contribute 
to the literature on domain specific beliefs about writing applying the BAWS to the 
preservice English teachers.  

One characteristic of adaptive beliefs was that they tended to match with higher scores on 
writing tasks (Sanders-Reio, 2010). According to our findings, the belief that writers should 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 
 

281 

Adapt To The Audience was significantly and positively correlated with overall writing 
performance. This proposed that those who put emphasis on audience-orientation were more 
probable to receive higher grades on their written work. In addition to the audience 
adaptation, the belief that Development Is Important positively and significantly correlated 
with the overall writing grade. This result suggested that the students who believed that 
writers should explain their thoughts and feelings effectively were more likely to score higher 
as were those who believed that audience-adaptation was crucial and had a significant role in 
writing process. Lastly, the belief, Writing Is an Iterative Process, correlated positively with 
the overall score. These results indicated that the students subscribing to the beliefs held by 
expert writers such as audience-orientation, effective essay development as well as the ones 
viewing writing as a process of reviewing were inclined to have higher scores on their papers.  

These findings also support Sanders-Reio’s (2010) and Sanders-Reio et al.’s (2014) 
outcomes in which audience-adaptation, attaching importance to development and viewing 
writing as an iterative process were the three of the beliefs held by expert writers and 
contributed positively to the writing performance. On the other hand, although some beliefs 
such as Minimize Revision, Transmissional and Basics (Mechanics) First negatively 
correlated with writing performance in our outcomes, these correlational values were not 
significant and were quite low. Two of these beliefs that were Minimize Revision and 
Transmissional had been negatively and significantly correlated with writing performance 
and found maladaptive in previous studies as well (e.g. Sanders-Reio, 2010; Sanders-Reio et 
al., 2015; White & Bruning, 2005). This means that some beliefs about writing tend to be the 
negative predictors of writing performance even in different research contexts.  

Nevertheless, the findings of the study did not support one of the outcomes of White and 
Bruning (2005). The Transactional belief that had originated from the research on reading 
and claimed that writers were supposed to be interested in their writing both emotionally and 
cognitively were divided into two different subscales in Sanders-Reio’s (2010) newly 
developed BAWS. These divided subscales were Writing Supports Thinking and Writing Is A 
Personal and Emotional Experience. Despite effecting writing performance positively and 
significantly in the research of White and Bruning (2005), both beliefs did not have any 
significant effect on writing performance in the current study. This can mean that our 
students do not regard writing as a mirror on which they can watch and evaluate their own 
ideas and/or they do not need to immerse themselves deeply and/or develop a distinctive 
writing style to become a good writer. One of the underlying reasons for this finding can be 
preservice teachers’ short-term interaction with foreign language writing. However, our 
results confirmed the findings of Burning et. al. (2011) in which both Transmissional and 
Transactional beliefs did not affect the scores on writing, and did not support the findings of 
Sanders-Reio et al.’s (2014) in which Transmission and Transaction was the significant 
negative predictors of writing performance.  

As researchers and teacher trainers, we are pleased to find out that the mean scores of 
preservice teachers in the ELT department were quite high for the three adaptive beliefs. The 
first one, Development Is Important, has the highest mean score among all others which is 
followed by Adapt To The Audience and Writing Is An Iterative Process in the fourth and 
fifth order respectively (See Table 3). So, it can be said that the first-year preservice teachers 
tend to implement the requirements of becoming an expert writer. As it is known that 
majority of the participants (i.e., 67.3%, see Figure 2) have never practiced foreign language 
writing, the writing classes and its components at university will be the first environment in 
which the teacher candidates can shape their beliefs about foreign language writing. 
Therefore, as teacher trainers, we should benefit from these beliefs of students as much as 
possible.   



Tanyer 
 

    
 

282 

According to the standard multiple regression, all beliefs about writing accounted for 
approximately 19% of variance of writing score. This value is higher in comparison with 
Sanders-Reio’s (2010) and Sanders-Reio et al.’s (2014) outcomes in which the all beliefs 
about writing explained 11.8% and 8.4% of the variance in writing scores respectively. 
Writing score was primarily predicted by the belief category that was Adapt To The 
Audience, and to a lesser extent by two other subcategories, that were Mechanical Errors Are 
Shameful and Writing Is An Innate Gift. On one level, the outcome of present study supports 
the findings reported in Sanders-Reio (2010) and Sanders-Reio et al. (2014) since the belief 
category, Adapt To The Audience, was positive predictor of writing performance in those two 
studies. The findings of the study also revealed that the most adaptive variable in this 
research was the belief, Adapt To The Audience, because it was the only belief category that 
both positively correlated with writing grades and explained statistically significant amount 
of variance in the writing scores. It was also positively correlated with other variables related 
to good writing that were Development Is Important and Writing Is an Iterative Process, 
which strengthens the level of adaptiveness of it. The other supporting point for this 
adaptiveness was that this belief also positively and significantly correlated with Mechanical 
Errors Are Shameful, which is one of the adaptive beliefs according to our findings. 

There is one more issue that needs to be discussed related to these quantitative data. As 
stated above, Mechanical Errors Are Shameful is one of the adaptive beliefs since this belief 
explained the variance in the writing scores. As proposed by Sanders-Reio (2010), finding 
this belief as adaptive is quite unexpected since “shame is a negative emotion” (p. 201). It has 
been argued that the preservice teachers’ capacity of using writing mechanics might be an 
effective factor for this issue. For instance, this belief “may motivate students with moderate 
mechanical skills to” overuse these skills “not to be shamed”, or it may cause preservice 
teachers having weak mechanical skills stay away from “writing and facing the shame 
associated with” this writing component (p.201). Besides, this belief might associate with 
higher grades of the participants due to their writing instructors’ evaluation and scoring 
criteria. From a different point of view, regarding mechanical errors as shameful might have 
resulted in dealing with this issue successfully without no excuse for the preservice teachers 
because most of our participants did not strongly believe that Mechanical Errors Are 
Shameful (M=2.851) with a t-value of -2.265 although it significantly and positively 
contributed to the total writing score. Also, some writing mechanics such as punctuation rules 
can be believed to be a requirement for good foreign language writing, which may shed light 
on why the belief that Mechanical Errors Are Shameful explained approximately 2% of the 
variance in writing score. When participants were asked what they did with their draft before 
it was subscribed to their teachers (see Tanyer, 2014; 2017), they stated that they checked 
both their grammar and other writing mechanics such as typos, spelling and punctuation. All 
these findings may be one of the explanations of why the belief Mechanical Errors are 
Shameful had been found as an adaptive belief and contributed positively to writing scores in 
the present study.  

As a result of the analysis of data from the survey, the belief, Adapt To The Audience, was 
found as an adaptive belief. This finding was also supported with the interview results in 
Tanyer & Subaşı (2016). In that study, the nineteen percent (19%) of preservice teachers 
believed that good foreign language writing aimed to address and impress audience. The 
same beliefs were also discovered to be one of good writer characteristics because, according 
to the interviewees, good writers should be aware of their audience (46%). In the same study, 
also, the belief that Development Is Important was also confirmed both as a good property of 
foreign language writing (e.g., Developing good and creative ideas, 58%) and good foreign 
language writer (e.g., Producer of knowledgeable and convincing essays, 27%) with the 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 
 

283 

interview results. Moreover, the other adaptive belief, Writing Is An Iterative Process, also 
associated with the features of good writing and good writers by the participants. Therefore, 
it can be argued that the preservice teachers have started to discover and internalize expert 
writing orientations, which is a desirable and promising finding.  

As stated above, two other beliefs, Mechanical Errors Are Shameful and Writing Is An 
Innate Gift, explained writing performance individually according to our outcomes. The first 
one (i.e., Mechanical Errors are Shameful) was found adaptive, and this result was also 
supported with the interviews conducted in Tanyer & Subaşı (2016). 8% of the participants 
stated that good foreign language writing was required to be “accompanied with punctuations 
rules”, and all those respondents (100%) were high-achieving students. Moreover, 58% of the 
participants expressed that complex and correct sentences must be formed with advance 
grammar in good foreign language writing, and 67% of that group had also received higher 
scores in their writing exams. This means that interview results of Tanyer & Subaşı (2016) 
are in line with the survey results in terms of adaptiveness degree of the belief, Mechanical 
Errors Are Shameful.  

The last belief, Writing Is An Innate Gift, was one of the maladaptive beliefs in the 
literature (e.g. Palmquist & Young, 1992; Charney, Newman & Palmquist, 1995; Sanders-
Reio, 2010). According to our research sample, that belief had the capacity to negatively 
explain writing performance. This means that in line with the previous studies, the students in 
our sample who tended to view writing as an innate gift were likely to score lower in their 
writing exams, as well. This finding has also been supported with the interview results of 
Tanyer & Subaşı (2016). As for writing ability, 42% of teacher candidates viewed writing 
both as an innate talent and a skill that can be improved with appropriate instruction, teacher 
feedback and student effort. However, 39% of participants characterized writing skill only as 
an innate gift, which means that it is almost impossible to become a good writer no matter 
how a novice writer puts effort to success. Lastly, only 19% of teacher candidates viewed 
writing as a skill that could be developed via enough training and student attempt. 
Interestingly, 80% of the respondents who had regarded foreign language writing as an innate 
gift were low-achieving students while 60% of the interviewees who had viewed it as an 
improvable skill were high-achieving students. Moreover, 73% of the participants who 
regarded foreign language writing both as an innate gift and an improvable skill (42%) had 
received higher scores in their exams as well. All these interview findings have confirmed the 
maladaptive tendency of the belief, Writing Is An Innate Gift, for our sample, and they might 
be the explanation of why this belief was found as maladaptive in our current research 
environment.  
5. Conclusion  

Social cognitive theory anticipates that beliefs about writing are associated with success 
and failure. Within the scope of this theory, self-efficacy in writing and its association with 
apprehension and success in writing has been discussed in a number of research studies; 
however, research on domain specific beliefs about writing and its relation to writing 
performance has been limited. Therefore, the main purpose of the current study was to 
examine first-year preservice English teachers’ beliefs about writing and the relation of these 
beliefs to writing performance in essay writing.  

As for the relationship between beliefs about writing and writing performance, the 
findings indicated that the beliefs, Adapt To The Audience, Development Is Important and 
Writing Is An Iterative Process, were significantly and positively correlated with overall 
writing performance. According to this finding, those who put emphasis on audience-
orientation, those who believe that writers should explain their thoughts and feelings 



Tanyer 
 

    
 

284 

effectively and those who view writing as a process of reviewing and revising were more 
probable to receive higher grades on their written work. On the other hand, although three 
beliefs, Minimize Revision, Transmissional and Basics Mechanics First, negatively correlated 
with writing performance, these correlational values were not significant and were quite low. 
Overall, the first three beliefs mentioned above (i.e., Adapt To The Audience, Development Is 
Important and Writing Is An Iterative Process) were prone to be adaptive associating 
positively with writing performance while the others (i.e., Minimize Revision, Transmissional 
and Basics Mechanics First) were tendentious to become maladaptive although statistical 
values regarding the maladaptive ones were not significant in our sample.  

As for the domain-specific beliefs about writing predicting writing performance, the 
findings revealed that all beliefs about writing as a block accounted for approximately 19% 
of variance of writing score (p<.05). Writing performance was primarily predicted by the 
belief category of Adapt to The Audience, and to a lesser extent by two other subcategories 
that were Mechanical Errors Are Shameful and Writing Is An Innate Gift. While two of these 
beliefs (i.e., Adapt to The Audience and Mechanical Errors Are Shameful) accounted for 
approximately 3% and 2% of variance in total writing scores respectively, the belief, Writing 
Is An Innate Gift, explained 2% of variance in lower writing scores. Therefore, the findings 
indicated that higher Adapt to The Audience and Mechanical Errors Are Shameful belief 
scores predicted higher overall writing grades while the higher scores of the belief, Writing Is 
An Innate Gift, predicted the lower writing scores in our research environment.  

5.1. Pedagogical Implications 

The present study highlights the existence of preservice English teachers’ domain specific 
beliefs about writing, and it concludes that domain-specific beliefs about writing are one of 
the influential factors of writing discipline and writing performance. As a training assistant, I 
could observe that “addressing course participants’ beliefs about writing” would provide 
another road to “writing competence and to more positive and productive attitudes” toward 
this discipline (Sanders-Reio, 2010, p. 219). The results of this study indicated that all 
domain specific beliefs about writing explained 19% variance in writing grades. For this 
reason, in addition to personal classroom observations, the scholarly findings attribute value 
to writing beliefs while training preservice English teachers on how to write. That is why the 
probable implications that would be stated based on these findings can be essential for 
teacher educators. 

Firstly, writing instructors can donate their undergraduate writers with particular strategies 
such as how to predict and respond to the questions of target audience and adapt their 
message to them because there are some writing components that need to be adapted 
according to the audience such as the format taken, the information included, and the 
language and graphics used. Furthermore, first-year novice writers may be trained for 
effective peer or individual revision and editing techniques that they can apply before 
submitting their drafts. As stated in the results, the beliefs associated with higher writing 
grades were mostly related to “expert writing guidelines and practices” (Sanders-Reio, 2010, 
p. 212). Moreover, one of the beliefs related to expert writing, Writing Is An Iterative 
Process, which advocates writing as a process of editing and revising, was discovered to be 
associated with high writing grades in our study. Based on this finding, it can be claimed that 
writing instructors should both observe, research and identify expert writers’ practices both in 
academic and authentic contexts, and furnish their learners with these expert writer 
qualifications. It should be remembered that as confirmed by the study of Tanyer & Subaşı 
(2016; i.e., factors and people effecting beliefs about good writing), school environment and 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 
 

285 

its components (e.g., teachers, curriculum, feedback etc.) are the potential factors shaping 
beliefs about good writing.  

All in all, the findings imply that beliefs about writing can be taken as a crucial leverage 
point for teaching writing. For this reason, writing instruction can be modified to “emphasize 
the mindsets and approaches associated with adaptive beliefs and minimize those related to 
maladaptive and ineffective beliefs.” (Sanders-Reio et al., 2014; p. 10). One way to achieve 
this can be giving homework or doing assignments which will inspire learners to focus on 
their readers’ characteristics and interests. Writing instruction can also help learners to 
present logical and convincing arguments because the belief, Development Is Important, has 
been labelled adaptive in nature by the participants. They can be presented with strategies to 
explain their opinions and findings effectively, to produce and convey qualified ideas, and to 
become logical and convincing of their own views.  

5.2. Limitations 

There are some points that need to be characterized as limited in the current study. Firstly, 
the Beliefs about Writing Survey adopted had originally been developed for the first-year 
preservice teachers in a second language environment although it was applied to the 
preservice English teachers in a foreign language context in Turkey. However, beliefs about 
writing can be specific to culture, writing instruction provided and learning context. 
Therefore, students’ beliefs can change based on these factors, or some other beliefs about 
writing can exist. To overcome these limitations, it is possible to replicate the study with 
different populations, or to discover other possible beliefs about writing and causal relations 
of them. Moreover, despite being persistent with some earlier research studies (e.g., Sanders-
Reio, 2010; Sanders-Reio et al., 2014; Shell, Murphy, & Bruning, 1989), the methodology 
used to decide students’ writing performance has not reflected the possible variance in the 
long-term performance because only one grade received on an in-class exam paper was 
included in the study.  

5.3. Suggestions for Further Research 

The findings of this research may call the need for more examination of the variables by 
means of larger and more representative samples in Turkey. Therefore, some beliefs can be 
found as more or less adaptive or maladaptive in different learning environments of Turkish 
universities, or for different writing tasks. Moreover, possible effect of instructional and 
assessment procedures and writing expertise on beliefs about writing can be investigated via 
longitudinal studies. Finally, it has been theorized that “there is also a cognitive link mediated 
by the writer’s choice of strategies or a student’s openness to instruction in specific 
strategies” (Sanders-Reio, 2014; p. 10). For instance, learners may pay attention to 
instruction on how to revise only if they believe that successful writers also revise. Therefore, 
the possible relationship between beliefs about writing and readiness to instruction, and the 
effect of this relationship on writing performance can be investigated as a follow-up research 
study.  
 

 
 

 
 

 



Tanyer 
 

    
 

286 

References 

Andrade, H. G., & Boulay, B. A. (2003). Role of rubric-referenced self-assessment in 
learning to write. The Journal of Educational Research, 97(1), 21-34. 

Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44(9), 
1175-1184.  

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman.  
Beach, R., & Friedrich, T. (2006). Response to writing. In C. A. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. 

Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 222-234). New York: Guilford. 
Bruning, R., Dempsey, M., Kauffman, D., & Zumbrunn, S. (2011). Writing viewed as 

transmission and transaction: An empirical study of implicit writing beliefs. Poster 
session presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research 
Association, New Orleans. 

Charney, D., Newman, J. H., & Palmquist, M. (1995). “I’m just no good at writing”: 
Epistemological style and attitudes toward writing. Written Communication, 12(3), 
298-329. 

Cramer, R. L. (2001). Creative power: The nature and nurture of children’s writing. New 
York: Logman. 

Daly, J. A., & Miller, M. D. (1975). The empirical development of an instrument to measure 
writing apprehension. Research in the Teaching of English, 9(3), 242-249. 

Gay, L. R. (1992). Educational research: Competencies for analysis and application (4th 
ed.). New York: Merrill. 

Graham, S., Schwartz, S. S., & MacArthur, C. A. (1993). Knowledge of writing and the 
composing process, attitude toward writing, and self-efficacy for students with and 
without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(4), 237-249. 

Jacobs, H. L., Hartfield, V. F., Hughey, J. B., & Vormuth, D. R. (1981). Testing ESL 
composition: A practical approach. Rowley, Mass: Newbury House Publishers. 

Kellogg, R. T. (2008). Training writing skills: a cognitive developmental perspective. 
Journal of Writing Research, 1(1), 1-26. 

Lavelle, E. (1993). Development and validation of an inventory to assess to processes in 
college composition. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 63(3), 489-499. 

Lavelle, E. (2001). Writing styles of college students. Journal of College Reading and 
Learning, 32(1), 60-67.  

Lavelle, E. (2003). Writing approaches of college students: A relational perspective. TLAR, 
8(2), 5-17. 

Lavelle, E., & Guarino, A. J. (2003). A multidimensional approach to understanding college 
writing. Educational Psychology, 23(3), 295-305. 

Lavelle, E., Smith, J., & O’Ryan, L. (2002). The writing approaches of secondary students. 
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(3), 399-418. 

Lavelle, E., & Zuercher, N. (2001). The writing approaches of university students. Higher 
Education, 42(3), 373-391. 

McCarthy, P., Meier, S., & Rinderer, R. (1985). Self-efficacy and writing: A different view 
of self-evaluation. College Composition and Communication, 36(4), 465-471. 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2017, 4(3), 266-288. 
 

 
 

287 

Meier, S., McCarthy, P. R., & Schmeck, R. R. (1984). Validity of self-efficacy as a predictor 
of writing performance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8(2), 107-120. 

Miller, C. R., & Charney, D. (2008). Persuasion, audience, and argument. In C. Bazerman 
(Ed.), Handbook of research on writing (pp. 583-598). New York: Erlbaum. 

National Commission on Writing. (2003, April). The neglected “R”: The need for a writing 
revolution. Retrieved November 2, 2013, from 
http://www.writingcommission.org/report.html 

Nelson, N. (2008). The reading-writing nexus in discourse research. In C. Bazerman (Ed.), 
Handbook of research on writing (pp. 435-450). New York: Erlbaum. 

Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (1999). Grade level and gender differences in the writing self-
beliefs of middle school students. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24(4), 390-
405. 

Palmquist, M., & Young, R. (1992). The notion of giftedness and student expectations about 
writing. Written Communication, 9(1), 137-168. 

Perry, K. R. (2011). Implicit beliefs about writing: a task-specific study of implicit beliefs. 
(Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved from http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cehsdiss/119/.  

Prat-Sala, M. & Redford P. (2012). Writing essays: does self-efficacy matter? The 
relationship between self-efficacy in reading and in writing and undergraduate students’ 
performance in essay writing. Educational Psychology, 32(1), 9–20.  

Sanders-Reio, J. (2010). Investigation of the relations between domain-specific beliefs about 
writing, writing self-efficacy, writing apprehension, and writing performance in 
undergraduates. (Master’s Thesis). Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations. 
(AAT 3409651). 

Sanders-Reio, J., Alexander, P., A., Reio, T., G. & Newman, I. (2014). Do students’ beliefs 
about writing relate to their writing self-efficacy, apprehension, and performance? 
Learning & Instruction, 33, 1-11.  

Schraw, G., & Bruning, R. (1996). Readers’ implicit models of reading. Reading Research 
Quarterly, 31(3), 290-305.  

Schraw, G., & Bruning, R. (1999). How implicit models of reading affect motivation to read 
and reading engagement. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3(3), 281-302. 

Shell, D. F., Murphy, C.C., & Bruning, R. H. (1989). Self-efficacy and outcome expectancy 
mechanisms in reading and writing achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 
81(1), 91-100. 

Silva, T., & Nicholls, J. G. (1993). College students as writing theorists: Goals and beliefs 
about the causes of success. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18(3), 281-293. 

Tanyer, S. (2014). The relationship between preservice English teachers' domain specific 
beliefs about writing and writing performance: A sample study. (Master’s Thesis). 
Avaliable from https://tez.yok.gov.tr/UlusalTezMerkezi/tezSorguSonucYeni.jsp. 
(Thesis No: 375298).  

Tanyer, S., (2015). The role of writing and reading self-efficacy in first-year preservice EFL 
teachers’ writing performance. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 199, 38-43. 

Tanyer, S., & Subaşı, G. (2016). Domain specific beliefs about writing: A qualitative inquiry 
on first-year pre-service English teachers. ELT Research Journal, 5(4), 278-303.  



Tanyer 
 

    
 

288 

Tanyer, S. (2017). How do first-year pre-service English teachers revise their written works? 
The reported practices of revision process. Manuscript submitted for publication. 

White, M. J., & Bruning, R. (2005). Implicit writing beliefs and their relation to writing 
quality. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30(2), 166-189. 

Zimmerman, B. J., & Bandura, A. (1994). Impact of self-regulatory influences on writing 
course attainment. American Educational Research Journal, 31(4), 845-862.