904 
 

 

 

Akfırat, O.N. & Turan, O. (2021). Adapting the scale 

of cognitive emotion regulation strategies for 

children to Turkish: validity and reliability 

studies. International Online Journal of 

Education and Teaching (IOJET), 8(2). 904-915.  

Received  : 03.07.2021 

Revised version received :------------- 

Accepted  : 15.10.2021 

 

 

ADAPTING THE SCALE OF COGNITIVE EMOTION REGULATION 

STRATEGIES FOR CHILDREN TO TURKISH: VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY 

STUDIES 

 Research article  

 

O. Nejat AKFIRAT  https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6748-9539 

Kocaeli University, Faculty of Education 

nejat.akfirat@kocaeli.edu.tr   

 

Correspondence:  

Olgun TURAN  https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2433-1878 

Republic of Turkey Ministry of National Education 

olgunturanpdr34@gmail.com  

 

Biodatas:  

Dr. O. Nejat Akfirat is a faculty member at Kocaeli University Faculty of Education, 

Department of Guidance and Psychological Counseling. His fields of interest are 

psychosocial development, and gender.  

 

Olgun Turan is working as a psychological counselor at a secondary school in Istanbul. He 

completed his master's degree on the development and evaluation of the cognitive emotion 

regulation strategies for children in 2020. 

 

 

Copyright © 2014 by International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET). ISSN: 2148-225X.  

Material published and so copyrighted may not be published elsewhere without written permission of IOJET.

http://orcid.org/xxxx
mailto:nejat.akfirat@kocaeli.edu.tr
http://orcid.org/xxxx
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2433-1878
mailto:olgunturanpdr34@gmail.com


International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2021, 8(2), 904-915. 

 

905 

ADAPTING THE SCALE OF COGNITIVE EMOTION REGULATION 

STRATEGIES FOR CHILDREN TO TURKISH: VALIDITY AND 

RELIABILITY STUDIES 

 

O. Nejat Akfirat 

nejat.akfirat@kocaeli.edu.tr    

 

Olgun Turan 

olgunturanpdr34@gmail.com  

 

Abstract 

This study aimed to adapt the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children 

Scale (CERQ-k) developed by Garnefski et al. (2007) into Turkish, and to carry out validity 

and reliability studies of the Turkish version for children aged between 9-12. The research 

was carried out with 657 students attending a Middle School in Istanbul's Pendik district in 

2018. As research instruments, Personal Information Form and Cognitive Emotion 

Regulation Strategies for Children Scale were used to collect data. According to the result of 

the confirmatory factor analysis conducted to test the construct validity of the scale, it was 

concluded that the 9-factor structure was confirmed. In the reliability studies regarding the 

scale, it was found that the Cronbach's Alpha coefficients for the sub-dimensions were 

between 0.43 and 0.80 and the total correlation values for the items were between 0.16 and 

0.66. It was found that the test-retest coefficient values of the sub-dimensions of the scale 

ranged from 0.36 to 0.67. Based on the findings obtained, it was concluded that the Turkish 

version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children Scale is valid and 

reliable for children between the ages of 9-12.  

 

Keywords: Cognitive emotion regulation, scale, middle childhood, Turkish adaptation 

 

1. Introduction 

Emotion is defined as a short-term and biologically-based response to physical and 

social factors that result from interaction with the environment (Niedenthal, Barsalou & 

Krauth-Gruber, 2005, p. 22). The view that emotions have a functional role in human life is 

more accepted in today's world than in the past. However, it is also seen that emotions are 

categorized as pleasant or unpleasant, creating positive or negative effects. We acknowledge 
that what the same emotion serves in human life can vary from situation to situation. For 

example, according to an emotion state, it can serve the needs of the person; sometimes 

approaching needs, sometimes avoiding needs, and sometimes both (Power, 2010). Similarly, 

the degree of feeling of emotion plays a functional role in one case, while it may lead to 

undesirable consequences in another. For example, the feeling of low excitement of a football 

player just before the penalty-taking affects his performance positively, while the low 

excitement of the same player during the attack can negatively affect his performance 

                                                        
 This study was written under the supervision of the first author from the master's thesis of the second author. 

mailto:nejat.akfirat@kocaeli.edu.tr
mailto:olgunturanpdr34@gmail.com


Akfirat & Turan 

    

906 

(Oxendine, 1970). This example shows that the severity of the emotion can affect the 

performance of the individual in different ways, even in the same event.  

In addition to the severity level of the emotion felt, the frequency of experiencing may 

affect the individual positively or negatively. For example; the frequency of experiencing a 

sense of joy decreases and the frequency of experiencing repulsion, anxiety, sadness, guilt, 

and anger may lead to an increase in the symptoms of depression (Vatan, 2017). Despite all 

this, emotions have a feature of being able to be regulated consciously or unconsciously. The 

individual can increase or decrease the severity and frequency of their emotions through 

emotion regulation. The duration of feeling the emotion experienced can be shortened or 

extended. However, the feelings about the event can be regulated, or some measures can be 

taken to prevent the emergence of emotions (Gross, 1998).  

The individuals use some preliminary processes and reaction-oriented strategies while 

organizing their feelings. These strategies take place within internal or external processes 

(Gross, 1998). For example, in case selection and change of status, which are the preliminary 

process-oriented strategies; emotions can be regulated by selecting any situation or by 

making changes in the current situation. Therefore, the use of these two strategies is mostly 

related to the selection or regulation of the external environment. Besides, in the diffusion of 

attention, which is also a preliminary process-oriented strategy, the person can regulate 

her/his emotions by defocusing or focusing her attention on the event. In this strategy, 

emotion regulation is carried out mostly by using internal processes. In response-oriented 

strategies, the individual can regulate his emotions by performing some behaviors to target 

his emerging emotions (Gross and Thompson, 2007). To reduce the effect of the emotion that 

emerges as a result of an event, an individual's use of a substance can be given as an example 

of a reaction-oriented strategy. 

Another way to regulate emotions is by attempting to change the meaning of the event 

in mind by creating cognitive change (Gross, 1999). Because, as a coping skill, people can 

evaluate their difficult experiences cognitively (Lazarus & Folkman, 1987). Therefore, 

Garnefski, Kraaij, and Spihoven (2001) discussed nine cognitive emotion regulation 

strategies in the context of a model which individuals use consciously against difficult and 

stressful life events. According to their study, the individual can influence their emotions and 

regulate their emotions by using these strategies. These strategies are; self-blame, acceptance, 

rumination, positive refocusing, refocus on the plan, positive reappraisal, putting in the 

perspective, catastrophizing, and blaming others are cognitive emotion regulation strategies.  

Research results related to cognitive emotion regulation strategies show that some 

strategies affect the functionality of the individual positively (Bormann & Carrico, 2009; 

Wolgast, Lundh & Viborg, 2011; Rood, Roelofs, Bögels & Arntz, 2012), while some others 

affect the functionality of the individual negatively (Çelik & Onat-Kocabıyık, 2014; 

Garnefski, Kraaij & Vlietstra, 2008). Also, it is known that individuals in middle childhood 

use these strategies (Garnefski, Rieffe, Jellesma, Terwogt & Kraaij, 2007; Orgilés, Morales, 

Fernandez-Martinez, Ortigosa-Quiles, & Espada, 2018; Andres, Richaud de Minzi, 

Castaneiras, Canet-Juric & Rodriguez-Carvajal, 2018). For this reason, it is important to 

carry out the validity and reliability studies of the Turkish version by adapting a measurement 

tool into Turkish which aims to determine the level of use of cognitive emotion regulation 

strategies of individuals in middle childhood.  

It is hoped that together with the measurement tool, which has been adapted, the 

practitioners will contribute to identifying the cognitive emotion regulation strategies used by 

the children in this period and to include more studies on these strategies with the 

interventions to be developed. Also, thanks to the scale of validity and reliability studies, it is 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2021, 8(2), 904-915. 

 

907 

thought that the field researchers will be able to research to examine the cognitive emotion 

regulation strategies used by individuals in middle childhood. Therefore, the aim of this study 

is the adaptation of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children Scale 

developed by Garnefski et al. (2007) into Turkish and determining the validity and reliability 

level of the Turkish version for children between the ages 9 and 12.  

2. Method 

2.1. Research Model 

A general survey model was used in the research. Screening studies are studies that 

examine a situation that exists in the past or present, and reveal the characteristics of that 

situation without any intervention. In the general screening model, which is a kind of 

screening research, to generalize the universe, the characteristics of the sample or the sample 

group representing the universe are scanned. In this research model, singular and relational 

scans can be done separately or together (Karasar, 2012). 

2.2. Universe and Sampling 

The universe of the research consisted of children between the ages of 9 and 12. The 

sample of the study consists of 657 (294 girls, 363 boys) students who were continuing their 

education in Istanbul's Pendik district at Muhsin Yazıcıoğlu Middle School in the 2018-2019 

academic year and whose ages were between 9 and 12. The sample of the research had a 

heterogeneous structure in terms of socio-economic and socio-cultural characteristics. 

The test-retest reliability studies of the research were conducted on 52 students who 

continued their education at Muhsin Yazıcıoğlu Middle School in the 2018-2019 academic 

year. 

2.3. Data Collection Tools 

To collect data in the study, Personal Information Form, and Cognitive Emotion 

Regulation Strategies for Children Scale developed by Garnefski et al. (2007) were used. 

Personal Information Form: In the Personal Information Form developed by the 

researcher; the participants' gender, grade level, and year of birth were obtained. 

Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children: The Cognitive Emotion 

Regulation Scale for Children (Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire-k) is a version 

of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Scale (CERS) developed by Garnefski et al. (2001) for 

children aged 9-11. The scale aims to determine the level of cognitive emotion regulation 

strategies used by a person against a particular event or events in general, regardless of 

clinical or non-clinical distinction. The scale, consisting of 36 items and 9 sub-dimensions, 

has a five-point Likert-type rating (1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = 

always). There are 4 items in each sub-dimension and the lowest score from a sub-dimension 

is 4 and the highest score is 20. The score obtained from a sub-dimension represents the level 

of the strategy used by the person regarding that sub-dimension. Sub-dimensions and sample 

items are as follows: 

1. Self-blame: I think it's all because of me. 
2. Acceptance: I think I cannot change what happened. 
3. Rumination: I always want to understand why I feel this way about what 

happened. 

4. Positive refocusing: I think of better things, not what happened. 
5. Re-focus on the plan: I think about how I can change this situation. 
6. Positive reappraisal: I think there are good things about what happened. 



Akfirat & Turan 

    

908 

7. Putting into perspective: I think there are worse things in the world. 
8. Catastrophizing: I always think this is the worst thing that can happen to me. 
9. Blaming others: I think what happened is the fault of others (Garnefski et al., 

2007). 

2.4. Process 

First of all, permission was obtained from Dr. Nadia GARNEFSKI via e-mail, one of 

the researchers who developed the scale, to make the adaptation studies of the Cognitive 

Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children Scale. Later on, the scale was translated 

separately by 5 experts who speak English. After this process, the translations were converted 

into a single table and the items that are thought to express the original items best were 

determined by a field and a language expert and then included in the scale. After obtaining 

the necessary permissions, the scale was applied to 180 students aged between 9-12 for 

piloting. During the piloting process, some changes were made on the form created by taking 

into consideration the feedback received from the students and the recommendations given 

by the assessment and evaluation specialist, and it was applied to 657 students to perform 

validity and reliability studies. Finally, the scale was applied to the group consisting of 52 

students twice in 30 days intervals to perform the test-retest reliability study.  

2.5. Data Analysis 

Validity is related to the level of representation of the feature desired to be measured. 

A valid test must distinguish the property it measures from other properties and accurately 

measure it. Construct validity is one of the validity types. Factor analysis can be conducted to 

determine the construct validity of a scale (Büyüköztürk et al., 2016). Reliability, on the other 

hand, represents the stability between the independent measurements of the properties 

measured by a measuring tool and the extent to which the measurement results are free from 

random errors. The reliability level of a scale can be determined by calculating the reliability 

coefficient (Ergin, 1995: p. 138-139). Cronbach's Alpha coefficient is frequently used to 

determine the level of reliability related to psychological measurements (Kılıç, 2016). Also, 

to have an idea about the scale items, the correlation calculation between the scores obtained 

from the scale items and the overall score of the scale can be used (Büyüköztürk, 2016a: p. 

183). Therefore, to determine the validity level of the scale, confirmatory factor analysis was 

performed and t values, error variances, and fit values were examined. To determine the 

reliability level of the scale, Cronbach's Alpha values for all tests and sub-dimensions and 

item-total correlation values for items were calculated. Finally, Spearman RHO analysis was 

performed to calculate the test-retest reliability of the scale. LISREL 8.7 and SPSS 22 

programs were used in the analyses. 

3. Findings 

In this part of the study, the findings of the validity and reliability studies on the 

Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children Scale are presented.  

3.1. Findings Related to the Validity of the Scale 

In the process of adapting a developed scale to different cultures, it is not necessary to 

perform exploratory factor analysis to determine the validity of the structure, and it is 

sufficient to perform confirmatory factor analysis. In a model representing independent 

structures from each other, the first-degree multi-factor model should be taken as a basis 

(Seçer, 2015). Figure 1 shows the path diagram of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation 

Strategies for Children Scale: 

 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2021, 8(2), 904-915. 

 

909 

Figure 1. Path Diagram of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children Scale 

 

 

When the path diagram is examined, it is seen that the t values related to the scale 

items vary between 5.73-20.54 and are significant at the level of 0.01 (Çokluk, Şekercioğlu & 

Büyüköztürk, 2016). In Table 1, the fit values of the scale are included: 

Table 1. Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children Scale Values 

 χ2 df χ2 /df RMSEA AGFI GFI CFI RMR NNFI 

CERSFC 1146.61 558 2.05 0.040 0.89 0.91 0.93 0.056 0.93 

In Table 1, the ratio of chi-square value to the degree of freedom is less than 3, 

RMSEA value is less than 0.05, NNFI, CFI and GFI values are above 0.90, and RMR value 

is below 0.08 and AGFI value is above 0.85. Above mentioned values indicate an acceptable 

fit of the model (Sümer, 2000; Raykov & Marcoulides, 2000; Schermelleh- Engel & 



Akfirat & Turan 

    

910 

Moosbrugger, 2003; Çokluk et al., 2016). These values show that the 9-dimensional structure 

of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children Scale has been confirmed. 

3.2. Findings Related to the Reliability of the Scale 

Cronbach's Alpha values and corrected item-total correlation values related to 

Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children are shown in Table 2: 

Table 2. Corrected Item Total Correlation and Cronbach Alpha Values for Cognitive 

Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children 

 

Sub-dimension Item 

Number 

Item Total Correlation Cronbach’s α 

 

Self-blame 

i1 

i10 

i19 

i28 

0,43 

0,43 

0,50 

0,57 

 

0,70 

 

Acceptance 

i2 

i1 

i20 

i9 

0,16 

0,24 

0,34 

0,32 

 

0,46 

 

Rumination 

i3 

i12 

i21 

i30 

0,50 

0,51 

0,46 

0,51 

 

0,71 

 

Positive Re-Focusing 

i4 

i13 

i22 

i31 

0,52 

0,66 

0,66 

0,61 

 

0,80 

 

Re-Focusing on the Plan 

i5 

i14 

i23 

i32 

0,33 

0,39 

0,43 

0,49 

 

0,63 

 

Positive Reappraisal 

i6 

i15 

i24 

i33 

0,20 

0,18 

0,32 

0,36 

 

0,43 

 

Putting in the Perspective 

i7 

i16 

i25 

i34 

0,30 

0,40 

0,23 

0,42 

 

0,55 

 

Catastrophizing 

i8 

i17 

i26 

i35 

0,28 

0,40 

0,42 

0,49 

 

0,61 

 

Blaming Others 

i9 

i18 

i27 

i36 

0,49 

0,51 

0,60 

0,51 

 

0,73 

General   0,79 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2021, 8(2), 904-915. 

 

911 

The overall internal consistency coefficient of the scale is 0.79. To say that a scale is a 

reliable measurement tool, the reliability coefficient value must be between 0.40 and 1 

(Özdamar, 1999: p. 522). It is seen that all sub-dimensions of Cognitive Emotion Regulation 

Strategies for Children Scale are within the specified value range (0.43 ≤ α ≤ 0.80). Item total 

correlation values of the scale ranged from 0.16 to 0.66. When the items whose item-total 

correlation value is below 0.30 are removed from the scale, the changes in the Cronbach's 

Alpha values of their sub-dimensions are shown in Table 3: 

Table 3 The Changes in the Reliability Coefficients of the Sub-Dimensions when 

items with Total Correlation Value Below 0.30 are Excluded from the Scale 

 

When Table 3 is examined, when the 6th, 11th, and 15th items are excluded from the 

scale, the reliability coefficient values of the sub-dimensions of these items decrease; When 

items 2, 8 and 25 are excluded from the scale, there is no significant increase in the reliability 

coefficient values of the sub-dimensions of these items. Therefore, the fact that the related 

items remain on the scale does not pose any problem in terms of the reliability of the scale. 

The reliability level of the scale can be determined by applying the scale to the same 

group twice in a certain time interval and calculating the correlation between the results 

(Büyüköztürk, Kılıç Çakmak, Akgün, Karadeniz & Demirel, 2016: p. 113). The Spearman 

RHO Analysis performed to calculate the test-retest reliability of the Cognitive Emotion 

Regulation Strategies for Children is shown in Table 4: 

 

Table 4. Spearman RHO Analysis Results for the Calculation of Test-Retest 

Reliability of the Turkish Version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies Scale for 

Children. 

Sub-Dimensions Test – Re-test Correlation Values 

Self-blame 0,67 

Acceptance 0,43 

Rumination 0,62 

Positive Re-Focusing 0,65 

Re-Focusing on the 

Plan 
0,38 

Positive Reappraisal 0,36 

Putting in the 

Perspective 
0,65 

Catastrophizing 0,53 

Blaming Others 0,63 

General 0,61 

 

Item  

Number 

Item Total 

Correlation 

Cronbach’s α Value of 

Item’s Sub-dimension 

Cronbach’s α Value of Item’s Sub-

Dimension when Item Excluded 

from Scale 

i2 

i6 

i8 

i11 

i15 

i25 

0,16 

0,18 

0,28 

0,20 

0,24 

0,23 

0,46 

0,43 

0,61 

0,46 

0,43 

0,55 

0,48 

0,40 

0,62 

0,41 

0,42 

0,56 



Akfirat & Turan 

    

912 

Test-retest reliability correlation values for the sub-dimensions of the scale ranged 

from 0.36 to 0.67. These values show that there is a moderate relationship between the first 

application and the last application (Büyüköztürk, 2016b). 

 

4. Discussion 

In this study, the researchers aimed to adapt the Cognitive Emotion Regulation 

Strategies for Children Scale (originally named as Cognitive Emotion Regulation 

Questionnaire-k (CERQ-k)) developed by Garnefski et al. (2007) into Turkish and to carry 

out validity and reliability studies of the Turkish version for children aged between 9-12. 

Confirmatory factor analysis was performed in the validity studies of the scale. According to 

the results of the analysis, it was seen that the 9-dimensional structure of the scale was 

confirmed. This results of the study were similar to the original form of the scale and child 

and adult forms in many countries (Garnefski et al., 2007; Onat & Otrar, 2010; Perte & 

Miclea, 2011; Liu, Chen & Blue, 2016; Dominguez & Medrano, 2016; Orgilés et al., 2018). 

It was observed that the Cronbach's Alpha coefficient values related to the sub-dimensions of 

the Turkish version of the scale ranged between 0.43 and 0.80. The Cronbach's Alpha 

coefficients of the sub-dimensions of the original form of the scale were between 0.62-0.79 

(Garnefski et al., 2007), the Cronbach's Alpha coefficients of the Spanish form were between 

0.56-0.75 (Orgilés et al., 2018) and the Cronbach Alpha coefficients of the Chinese form 

were 0.66-0.73 (Liu et al., 2016). The reliability coefficient values for the sub-dimensions of 

the Turkish version of the scale are found to be similar with other versions. In the test-retest 

reliability study, it was found that there was a moderate positive correlation between the sub-

dimensions (Büyüköztürk, 2016b). This finding is compatible with the original version of the 

scale and other versions (Garnefski et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2016; Orgilés et al., 2018).  

Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children Scale was examined in terms of 

item-total correlation values and it was found that the corrected item-total correlation value of 

6 items (I2, I6, I8, I11, I15, I25) was below 0.30. When these items are examined, when the 3 

items (I6, I11, I15) are removed from the scale, the reliability of their sub-dimensions 

decreases. Therefore, it was decided not to exclude the related items from the scale. When the 

other 3 items (I2, I8, I25) are removed from the scale, the reliability of the sub-dimensions 

they are in increases insignificantly. According to Özdamar (1999), it can be categorized at 

the low, medium, and high level provided that the reliability coefficient values are between 

0.40 and 1. Removing related items from the scale does not change the reliability levels of 

the sub-dimensions they belong to. For example; When the 8th item was removed from the 

scale, the Cronbach's Alpha coefficient value of the sub-dimension maintain will maintain its 

medium level reliability. The same is true for items 2 and 25. In the Spanish form of the 

scale, it was decided that the item (I20) would remain on the scale since the item's total 

correlation value below 0.30 but did not affect the reliability level of the sub-dimension 

(Orgilés et al., 2018). In the form of the scale, which was confirmed in 8 sub-dimensions, it 

was found that the total correlation value of 3 items (M7, M25, M34) was below 0.30. 

However, the test-retest reliability level of the sub-dimensions containing the relevant items 

showed the highest level of loading except for the sub-dimension that should be present in 

terms of factor loading in the original form of the scale (7, M2, M8, M11, M21, M24, M30, 

M33). However, it was not excluded from the scale since they were placed in the sub-

dimensions they were theoretically related to (Kurtoğlu-Karataş, 2019). In terms of total 

correlation values of the item, it was decided that items 2, 8 and 25 would remain on the scale 

and as a result, none of the 36 items were from the scale, since the findings included in the 

study were similar between the other forms of the scale and that the exclusion of the related 

items from the scale did not adversely affect the level of reliability. 



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2021, 8(2), 904-915. 

 

913 

As a result of the analysis, it has been concluded that the Turkish version of the 

Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies for Children Scale consisting of 9 sub-dimensions 

and 36 items is a valid and reliable measurement tool for children between the ages of 9-12. 

However, this research has some limitations. One of these limitations is that the scale is based 

on self-evaluation and the participants are assumed to answer the scale sincerely. Besides, 

within the scope of the research, the criterion-based validity of the scale was not examined. In 

other studies, the criterion-based validity of the scale can be tested. 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 



Akfirat & Turan 

    

914 

References 

Andres, M.L., Richaud de Minzi, M. C., Castaneiras, C., Canet-Juric, L. & Rodriguez-

Carvajal, R. (2016). Neuroticism and depression in children: The role of cognitive 

emotion regulation strategies. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 177(2), 55-71. 

Bormann, J. E. & Carrico, A. W. (2009). Increases in positive reappraisal coping during a 

group-based mantram intervention mediate sustained reductions in anger in hiv-

positive persons. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 16(1), 74-80. 

Büyüköztürk, Ş. (2016a). Sosyal bilimler için veri analizi el kitabı (22. baskı). Ankara: 

Pegem Akademi. 

Büyüköztürk, Ş. (2016b). Deneysel desenler: Öntest-sontest kontrol gruplu desen ve spss 

uygulamalı veri analizi (Geliştirilmiş 2. baskı). Ankara: Pegem Akademi. 

Büyüköztürk, Ş., Kılıç Çakmak, E., Akgün, Ö. E., Karadeniz, Ş., & Demirel, F. (2016). 

Bilimsel araştırma yöntemleri (20. baskı). Ankara: Pegem Akademi. 

Çelik, H. & Onat-Kocabıyık, O. (2014). Genç yetişkinlerin saldırganlık ifade biçimlerinin 

cinsiyet ve bilişsel duygu düzenleme tarzları bağlamında incelenmesi. Trakya 

Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 4(1), 139-155. 

Çokluk, Ö., Şekercioğlu, G. & Büyüköztürk, Ş. (2016). Sosyal bilimler için çok değişkenli 

istatistik: Spss ve lisrel uygulamaları (4. baskı). Ankara: Pegem Akademi. 

Dominguez S. & Medrano L. (2016). Propiedades psicométricas del cognitive emotional 

regulation questionnarie (cerq) en estudiantes universitarios de lima. Psychologia: 

Avances Disciplina, 10(1), 53–67. 

Ergin, Y. D. (1995). Ölçeklerde geçerlik ve güvenirlik. M.Ü. Atatürk Eğitim Fakültesi Eğitim 

Bilimleri Dergisi, 7, 125-148. 

Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V. & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive emotion 

regulation and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(8), 

1311-1327. 

Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V. & Vlietstra, A. (2008). Cognitive coping and depressive symptoms 

in definitive infertility: A prospective study. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & 

Gynecology, 29(1), 9-16. 

Garnefski, N., Rieffe, C., Jellesma, F., Terwogt, M.M. & Kraaij, V. (2007). Cognitive 

emotion regulation strategies and emotional problems in 9-11-year-old children: The 

development of an instrument. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 16(1), 1–9. 

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review 

of General Psychology, 2, 271-299. 

Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion regulation: Past, present, future. Cognition & Emotion, 13(5), 

551-573. 

Gross, J. J. & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In: 

Gross, J. J. (Ed.). Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press, 1-24. 

Karasar, N. (2012). Bilimsel araştırma yöntemi: Kavramlar, ilkeler, teknikler (23. 

baskı). Ankara: Nobel Akademik Yayıncılık. 

Kılıç, S. (2016). Cronbach's alpha reliability coefficient. Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 

6(1), 47-48.  



International Online Journal of Education and Teaching (IOJET) 2021, 8(2), 904-915. 

 

915 

Kurtoğlu- Karataş, B. (2019). 9-11 Yaş Grubu Çocukların Bilişsel Duygu Düzenleme 

Stratejileri ve Öz Saygı Düzeyleri Arasındaki İlişki (Yayımlanmamış Yüksek Lisans 

Tezi). Gazi Üniversitesi Eğitim Bilimleri Enstitüsü: Ankara. 

Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1987). Transactional theory and research on emotions and 

coping. European Journal of Personality, 1(3), 141–169. 

Liu, W., Chen, L. & Blue, P. R. (2016). Chinese adaptation and psychometric properties of 

the child version of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Plos One, 11(2), 

1-14.  

Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Ric, F., & Krauth-Gruber, S. (2005). Embodiment in the 

acquisition and use of emotion knowledge. In: L.F. Barret, P. Winkielman & P. M. 

Niedenthal (Eds.). Emotion and consciousness. New York: The Guılford Press, 21-50. 

Onat, O. & Otrar, M. (2010). Bilişsel duygu düzenleme ölçeğinin türkçeye uyarlanması: 

Geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışmaları. Marmara Üniversitesi Atatürk Eğitim Fakültesi 

Eğitim Bilimleri Dergisi, 31(31), 123-143. 

Orgilés, M., Morales, A., Fernandez-Martinez, I., Ortigosa-Quiles, J. M. & Espada, J. P. 

(2018). Spanish adaptation and psychometric properties of the child version of the 

cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Plos One, 13(8), 1-12. 

Oxendine, J. B. (1970). Emotional arousal and motor performance. Quest, 13(1), 23-32. 

Özdamar, K. (1999). Paket programlar ile istatistiksel veri analizi 1. Eskişehir: Kaan 

Kitabevi. 

Perte, A. & Miclea, M. (2011). The standardization of the cognitive emotion regulation 

questionaire (cerq) on romanian population. Cognitie, Creier, Comportament, 15(1), 

111-130. 

Power, M. (2010). Emotion-focused cognitive therapy. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. 

Raykov, T. & Marcoulides, G. A. (2000). A first course in structural equation modeling. 

London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. 

Rood, L., Roelofs, J., Bögels, S. M. & Arntz, A. (2012). The effects of experimentally 

induced rumination, positive reappraisal, acceptance and distancing when thinking 

about a stressful event on affect states in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child 

Psychology, 40(1), 73-84. 

Schermelleh-Engel, K. & Moosbrugger, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation 

models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness of fit measures. Methods of 

Psychological Research Online, 8 (2): 23-74. 

Seçer, İ. (2015). Psikolojik test geliştirme ve uyarlama süreci spss ve lisrel uygulamaları. 

Ankara: Anı Yayıncılık. 

Sümer, N. (2000). Yapısal eşitlik modelleri. Türk Psikoloji Yazıları, 3(6), 49-74. 

Vatan, S. (2017). Duygular ve psikolojik belirtiler arasındaki ilişkiler. Psikiyatride Güncel 

Yaklasimlar, 9(1), 45-62. 

Wolgast, M., Lundh, L. G. & Viborg, G. (2011). Cognitive reappraisal and acceptance: An 

experimental comparison of two emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research 

and Therapy, 49(12), 858-866.