Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development – JCLAD 
Vol: 9     Issue:  4   413-428, 2021 

                                                                                                                          ISSN: 2148-1997 

 

413 
 
 

 

The developmental realization of ejectives by typically 
developing Amharic speaking children  

 
                                                                  Fikre Diress Alamirew1 

  Addis Ababa University 

                                                     Abebayehu Messele Mekonnen2 
  Addis Ababa University 

 
Abstract 

The current study describes the developmental realization of ejectives by 

typically developing Amharic-speaking children. It employed cross-sectional 

research method in order to collect the primary children’s speeches data within 

a given period. Children’s speeches data were audio-recorded, using single-
words, sentence repetition, and spontaneous elicitation mode tasks. The audio-

recorded data were transcribed using IPA and ExtIPA and analyzed. The result 

obtained from, aged between 3; 0 a and 5; 0, 32 children indicated the different 

realizations of ejectives. Majority of this age bound children under this study 

produced the ejective/p’/,/t’/,/k’/,/tʃ’/and/s’/properly and had a target 

realization when the consonants appeared as a singleton in a word but if they 
were clustered, they  had a possibly to be dropped. On the other hand, there 

were some children, from different age groups, totally substituted these ejectives 

by their pulmonic counterparts as/p’/ [p],/t’/ [t],/tʃ’/ [tʃ],/k’/ [k] and/s’/  

[s] and these developmental patterns detected in these children’s speeches were 

similar to ones, which have already been reported in other local languages (e.g. 
Sidamuu Affoo). However, the realization of the ejective /t’/ [th] and/tʃ’/ [th] or 

[t’] and /s’/ [tʃ’] were uniquely observed in aged between 3;10 and 5;0 three 

children’s production. Such developmental patterns detected at later age might 

pose surmise whether they are typically language-related or a sign of delay and 

it may require a depth study including more participants and designing 

particular task. Generally, this study confirmed that majority of Amharic 
speaking children were able to master the production of ejective consonants 

before at age four. However, the complete target realization of such consonants 

is likely a time taking process, which goes beyond the age bound in some 

children language development. This implies that some children require 

continues exposure to these consonants in different word positions and need to 
have oral practice at school and at home to foster the acquisition process. 

 

Keywords: Language acquisition, language development, phonology, consonant      

 realization, and ejectives 

 

 
1 Mrs. Fikre Diress is a PHD candidate in the department of Foreign Language and 

Literature at Addis Ababa University and instructor at Ambo University. Her research 

interests include language acquisition, language development and language learning. 

    fikrediress@gmail.com  
2 Dr. Abebayehu Messele Mekonnen is an Assistant Professor of Clinical Linguistics in the 

Department of Linguistics, Addis Ababa University, and Ethiopia. His research interest 
includes phoneticsand phonology, typical and atypical speech/language development, co

mmunication disorders, and language based learning disabilities.  
       abebayehu.messele@aau.edu.et  

Received : 28.10.2021 

Accepted : 01.12.2021 
Published : 29.12.2021 

mailto:fikrediress@gmail.com
mailto:abebayehu.messele@aau.edu.et


  
The Developmental realization of Ejectives                                                           Alamirew and Mekonnen 

414 
 
 

 

1. Introduction  

Language is one of the defining human characteristics; children of all 
cultures acquire a native language, and they do so without a need of formal 
instruction. At first a new born child does not have any knowledge or 

experience when he/she comes to this world even if he/she is biologically 
ready to learn from the environment; his/her brain is just like a white paper 

on which nothing is spotted. However, a child is able to accumulate ideas, 
thoughts, and expertise through experience and the exposure he/she gets 
from the surrounding. It is believed that the interaction between nature and 

nurture plays an invaluable role in children’s all-round development in 
general, and in language development in particular (Pinker, 2004; Sabahat, 
2012). 

Language acquisition is a process by which children attain the capacity to 
perceive and understand sound systems, as well as develop different 

category of lexicons and sentences. Language acquisition is one of a typical 
child behavior, which can be developed through time and enables he/she to 
make use of sophisticated means of communication when they grow old 

(Ferguson, 1964). However, the regularity and speed in which a child 
acquires  target language has prompted researchers to question whether a 

language is learned in any meaningful sense or not.  
This has built long standing theories, which describe the child’s language 
development and acquisition process. On first language acquisition, 

especially, various studies have been conducted for different reasons, in 
various approaches at the very depths to describe how the acquisition of 
language takes place without adults formally teaching them and without 

conscious learning. This debatable issue for many years has also posed 
many questions and still studies have been carried out on various features of 

the target languages to address whether the acquisition process is cross 
linguistically the same or different. In reference to this, Lust (2006) noted 
that the growing studies of language acquisition have focused to answer 

several questions. Some deal with phonological aspects of the language, 
whereas the others focus on lexicon or morphosyntactic features. 
However, from researchers’ different areas of focus on children’s language 

acquisition, child’s phonology has been the one, which has gained a great 
attention for many years. Different studies (Beckmanet al, 2009; Ingram, 

1974; Rose & Sharon, 2011) have described the acquisition of segmental 
aspects of language, such as the order of consonant acquisition, mastery of 
segmental features, like voicing and devoicing. Other researchers have also 

focused on the development of various suprasegmental features such as 
syllable structure (Dodd, 1995; Watson, 1997; Demuth et al, 2014), tone, 

stress/intonation patterns, etc. (Kehoe, 2001) and studies have explained 
the different features of phonological development and designed different 
models and theories. 

As the theories and models have indicated, most of the phonological 
acquisition studies have focused for the last three decades in most European 



Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development – JCLAD 
Vol: 9     Issue:  4   413-428, 2021 

                                                                                                                          ISSN: 2148-1997 

 

415 
 
 

 

and Asian languages (Ferguson 1964; Ingram, 1989; Wyatt, 1994).However, 
the acquisition of phonology in many African languages has not been 

addressed properly despite some studies’ reports. For example, Demuth 
(2010) in the acquisition of Sesseato passives; Takeida (2011) on a 

significant role caregiver-infant interaction in children’s language 
development and Deen, KamilUd (2002) the acquisition of Nairobi Swahili: 
The morphosyntax of inflectional prefixes and subjects. 

The current study was conducted in Ethiopian, which is one of east African 
countries where more than 80 languages are spoken. In spite of this fact, 
studies on the acquisition of Ethiopian languages are very few. Only few 

studies were traced during this study. In Amharic, Abebayehu (2013) has 
dedicated chapters of his Ph.D. dissertation to a description of 

developmental speech sound realizations and Ebenezer (2013) examined the 
nature of child language and baby talk. Similarly, Tariku (2019) has reported 
the acquisition of Afaan Oromo Phonology and Abebayehu and Demeke 

(2017) described aspects of phonological acquisition in children speaking 
Sidamuu Afoo. These local researchers have made significant contributions 

towards the efforts to fill the gap in the area of languages acquisition. 
However, there are number of language acquisition issues, which have not 
been addressed yet. Thus, the present study aims to contribute to filling the 

gap by describing the developmental realization of ejectives by aged between 
3;0 and 5;0 typically developing Amharic-speaking children. The following 
questions were also set to be answered in this ongoing research findings. 

  
1.1. Research questions 

This study was designed to answer the following questions. 
I. How does the developmental realization of ejectives look like in 

Amharic? 

II. What are developmental realization patterns of ejectives observed in 
aged 3;0 and5;0 years children? 

III. What is the theoretical and the practical implications of the finding of 

the current study? 
 

1.2. Summary of phonological theories 
This study aims to describe the developmental realization of ejectives by 
typically developing children acquiring Amharic language. The result from 

the current study is believed to contribute to the knowledge of language 
acquisition in general and the acquisition of Amharic phonology in 

particular. Therefore, this, the theoretical framework in this section is 
written as follows. It was also discussed based on the most common types of 
theoretical frameworks /approaches/ used in language acquisition research. 

In language acquisition research, the major theoretical argument is between 
the two frameworks, rationalists, and impressionists. In this regard, 
literature has tended to focus on the distinguishing between the two 

theoretical approaches that have proposed different accounts for the 



  
The Developmental realization of Ejectives                                                           Alamirew and Mekonnen 

416 
 
 

 

acquisition phonology Bybee,2006). Several researchers show domain-

specific models to explain various features of phonological development like 
how the speech stream is segmented and word learning happens. Along with 

this proposal, different ideas were suggested on how children identify the 
speech sounds and produce a word, what shape and representations of 
lexical entries have, and how they are acquired. Different approaches and 

models are directly connected to phonological acquisition, which takes place 
in children’s language development process (Renner, 2017). 
The general model of phonological development is one of the earliest 

endeavors; Jackson designed it1968) and grounded his model on diary 
studies and took it as a paralinguistic period of babbling and a period of 

phonological acquisition. As his proposal, universal structures guide 
phonological acquisition; it means, most common sounds cross-linguistically 
are acquired first. Jacobson’s effort was very central to the relatively short 

history of child phonology and considered as a good promoter for advanced 
research on child language acquisition and development (Renner, 2017; 

Stoel Gamon, 2010). On the other hand, Rule-based theory, which was 
proposed by Chomsky and Halle (1968), has taken into account the 
generative phonology. The attention of this phonological theory relied on the 

connection between abstract and surface forms: in other words, it was the 
only in its time for its’ rule-based account of phonology, which tried to show 
the rules of how phonemes or classes of phonemes were produced under a 

specified time.  
The theory is taken as one of the basic views, which examine a child’s 

speech production by applying a set of phonological rules. As applied to child 
phonology, generative rules were used to describe children’s simplifications 
of adult phonemes such as[+continuant] segments (fricatives) becoming 

[continent] (stops) in certain word positions (Smith, 1973). Similarly, Stampe 
(1969) tried to show the theory of natural phonology which was established 

by describing a set of universal and native phonological processes. These 
were used to apply to adult and child speech. As the theory stated, to 
acquire adult-like phonology, a child needs to pass different processes to 

learn the sound systems that do not even occur in their language (Stampe, 
1972). In short, these approaches comprise levels of the phrase and prosodic 
words, going down to the grass root and syllable structures. It describes the 

order of phonological features (Bernhardt & Stumberger, 1998).  
On the other hand, Optimality Theory (OT) focuses on the view, which 

organized input to enhance certain output forms rather than being process-
oriented. OT is outcome-oriented theory; it leads child philologists to focus 
on the system of language acquisition and what is obtained in no adult-like 

changes in the output instead of focusing on errors (Prince & Smolensk, 
2004). In this theory, the child’s phonology is described as an actively 
growing system, but it is not seen as a complete realization of adult 

phonology. OT proposal deals with a universal set of contradictory 
constraints of two basic types. The first one is nakedness constraints, which 

do not allow the existence of marked structures in the output a constraint 



Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development – JCLAD 
Vol: 9     Issue:  4   413-428, 2021 

                                                                                                                          ISSN: 2148-1997 

 

417 
 
 

 

on final consonant and faithfulness constraints, which needs the 
requirement of matching between the input and output (Rose, 2009). 

Generally, meaningful language development is a multifaceted process. 
Concerning phonological development, children must acquire the sounds 

system to produce words to match the adult targets; they must also 
understand the phonological forms of words in their native language. This 
complex process has two essential components. The first one is a biologically 

based component associated with the development of the speech motor skills 
required for the production of adult-like words and the second are a 
cognitive linguistic component associated with learning the phonological 

system of the ambient language. The cognitive aspect of phonological 
learning entails the mental representation of sounds, which are stored and 

accessed in the encoding and decoding of words. Some type of mental 
representation is necessary for the perception and production of words. To 
pronounce the word cat, for example, an individual child must have a stored 

version of the sound sequence associated with the word; the individual child 
must be able to differentiate the word cat from related sounding words, such 

as hat, cap, cut, (Clark, 2005; Alqattan, 2015). In the same way, the 
production of the word cat requires knowledge of the target sound sequence 
and the articulatory movements that are necessary to distinguish it from 

other words with similar sound patterns. Therefore, in children’s speeches, 
sound development can be seen from phonetic as well as phonological 
acquisition (Winitz, 1969). 

The development of a phonological system is progressive and needs gradual 
change. However, the sequence of contrast affects smaller sound units due 

to the principle of extreme divergent form of the adult phonological systems. 
Concerning this, Gleason & Nan (2009) tried to show the acquisition of 
sound systems in terms of the distribution of sound among the world’s 

languages. They have indicated that the most widely distributed sound is the 
earlier to be acquired. Nasals, front, and stop consonants would be acquired 
earlier than orals, back consonants, and fricatives, respectively, as children’s 

language differ in certain features from adults. A good example of this is a 
phonological inventory of children’s language in English indicating that 

children use tore or sore for a store or a gig for pig.  
Children are seen they are unable to produce certain sounds or sound 
combinations of the adult language; they simplify the sound system of 

language to make up the limitations. Studies on a child’s phonology provided 
a certain process that can be seen in child language (Ingram, 1978 &,1989; 

Stampe, 1973; Smith,1973; Menn, 1971, 1977). The processes are often 
expressed as common changes that are seen in children's sound systems. 
Children often create a suitable sound system for themselves so that they 

produce an output of their form until they can produce adult-like words. In 
other words, the processes described as the relations between the adult and 
the child form.  



  
The Developmental realization of Ejectives                                                           Alamirew and Mekonnen 

418 
 
 

 

As far as children’s phonological processes are concerned, different scholars 

(Ingram, 1989; Stoel-Gammon, 2006) categorized the process as cluster 
reduction, omission, substitution and assimilation (consonant harmony). 

From different phonological features that children pass through, 
substitution of one consonant by others has divergent developmental 
patterns: stopping, fronting, devoicing, backing, gliding as well as de-

ejectivisation (in some languages like Amharic). In sum, children pass 
through certain phonological processes: especially, substitution, in general, 
de-ejectivisation in particular was the focus of this article. 

 
2. Methodology 

The main aim of this study was to describe the developmental realization of 
ejectives by typically developing Amharic speaking. Such a research in the 
area of language acquisition often follows longitudinal and/or cross-sectional 

research designs (Clark, 2003; O’Grady, 2006; Ingram, 1989). However, in 
this study, cross-sectional research design was used. The researchers 

preferred to use a cross-sectional study designed to collect data at once in a 
given point of time across a sample population or a pre-defined subset 
(Zoltan, 2007). Cross-sectional research design is helpful to include a large 

representative sample and less demanding to recruit respondents. It also 
allows researchers to gather a large size of data within a short period. The 
method helps the researcher to see individual ability of producing various 

phonological elements. According to Zoltan (2007) explanation, cross-
sectional data collection method is less exposed to detrimental factors like 

unexpected external events that are beyond the researcher control (e.g. 
children illness, drop out and other) 
 

1.1. Participants 
As part of the arrangement and for the choice of appropriate participants, it 

was made brief discussions with school chief principals on the objectives of 
the study and why the children get involved. Having the school’s consent, 60 
children, aged between 3;0 and 5;0 were selected from the two schools. 30 

children were from St Michael and 30 from Mentor Academy and the consent 
letter were sent to those children’s parents. The final selection was made by 
checking parent’s agreement and the appropriate information filled. Based 

on the information collected from fifty returned sheets, thirty–two children 
who and their parents did not speak other local languages except Amharic, 

were selected purposively. Sixteen female and sixteen male children were 
grouped in to four places according to their age with five-month intervals as 
presented as follows. 

 
 
 

 
 

 



Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development – JCLAD 
Vol: 9     Issue:  4   413-428, 2021 

                                                                                                                          ISSN: 2148-1997 

 

419 
 
 

 

Table 1  
Participants demographic data 

Code   Name  Sex Age  Code  Name  Sex   Age  

               Group 1  GROUP 3 

C1  TSN F 3;1;16 C17  BTHY F 4;5;0 

C2  HLN F 3;2;5 C18 HNA F 4;5;12 

C3  AMN  F 3; 2;23 C19  IDY F 4;5;19 

C4  MKL F 3;2;14 C20 FKR F 4;5;2    

C5  NTN M 3;3;15 C21  AMN2 M 4;6;7 

C6  ELS M 3;3:6 C22 KAL M 4;5;21 

C7  HRY M 3;2;26 C23 EBN M 4;6;9 

C8  EYB 1 M 3;3: 8 C24 EPR M 4;5;0 

Group 2 Group  4 

C9   BLN F 3;9:23 C25 SOL.M  F 4;11;10 

C10  SOL.G  F 3;10:2 C26 SMR F 4;11:6 

C11  DGM F 3;11:28 C27  RDT 2 F 4;11;20 

C12 RDT.B F 3;10 C28  LDY F 4;11;26 

C13  EYB2 M 3;1017 C29  ELNT M 4;11;23 

C14 AMN 1 M 3;9:24 C30  AMR M 4;11;12 

C15 MKYS M 4;0:5 C31   NOD M 4;11:21 

C16  BRK   M 3;10:7 C32 EYB.G M  4;11;2 

 
1.2. Data collection and processing 

Children’s speech data were collected using single-words, sentence 

repetition, and spontaneous elicitation modes. The speech data were audio-
recorded using a modern audio recorder and transcribed using IPA and Exist 
IPA. The transcription was done in ELAN linguistic annotator tools as it was 

easy for phonological segmentation and was helpful to display different 
words to see the developmental realization of various consonants.  

 
1.3. Data analysis 

The study had descriptive data analysis method. Children’s speeches were 

transcribed, using analytical listening technique, following Ashby et al. 
(1996). First, the analysis was done by classifying the different realizations of 

the target words. Then, an error identification approach was used. Next, the 
developmental realizations of different ejective consonants were identified 
and analyzed. Finally, an attempt was made to forward a possible discussion 

and conclusion. 
 
3. Findings 

In this section, it was attempted to describe and discuss the developmental 
realizations of ejectives attested in children’s speeches production. Due to 

spatial issue, the other substitution patterns were not included. However, 
de-ejectivisation of ejective consonants was taken here to refer to the 
developmental realizations of ejectives by non-ejective consonants. 

 



  
The Developmental realization of Ejectives                                                           Alamirew and Mekonnen 

420 
 
 

 

Table 2   

The realization of the ejective /p’/ [p] 

  Example 
 Process   Pattern   Realization   Target   Gloss  

De-ejectivisation /p’/ [p] etopa/ɨtopa etiopʼia Name of   
Country  

 papas P’ap'a’s  Bishop  

tɘjɘpesa(AMN) 
tɘrɘpesa(ELS,AMN2) 

t’ɘlɘpeza(HLN) 

t’ɘrɘp’eza  Table  

 
As the ejective /p’/ does not frequently appear in Amharic word formation, 

the number of words detected in children’s speeches was also very limited. 
However, it occurred in some words and was realized differently. Most of 

aged between 4;0 and 5;0 years children correctly produced the ejective /p’/ 
but there were still some from different age groups who substituted/p’/ [p] in 
this study. The realization of /p’/ [p] was also reported by Abebayehu and 

Demeke (2017) in typically developing Sidamuu Afoo children. 
 
Table 2  

Developmental realization of /t’/ [t]or[th] 
 

Process Patterns Word 
position 

Children   Age 
respectively  

 De-
ejectivisation   

/t’/ [t]/[th] WI AMN,ELS,ELN,AMN2 3;1,3;3,3;10,4;6 

WM HLN,ELS ELN,AMN2  3;2,3;3,3;10,4;6 

 Examples 

 
 
De-ejectivisation 

 Pattern  Realizatio
n  

 Target  Gloss 

/t’/ [t]/[ 
th] 

tota t’ot’a  Ape  

kɨtəl k’ɨt’əl leaf 

təntʃən t’ɨntʃəl rabbit 

tɨkul t’ɨk’ul Black  

tɨlle t’ɨre Cereals  

ajɨtuan ajɨt’uan  The rat 

 

From thirty-two children,five of them realized/t’/differently. They substituted

/t’/ [t]; For example, the word /t’ot’a/ meaning ‘ape’, which was detected 

in all children’s speeches, was consistently realized as [tota]. Similarly, the 

word/t’ɨntʃəl/ ‘rabbit as [təntʃən]. These children also realized the target word 

Process patterns Word 
position 

Children  Age respectively  

De-ejectivisation   /p’/ [p] WI AMN,ELS,AMN2 3;1, 3;3,4;6 

WM HLN ELS,AMN2  3;2,3;3, 4;6, 



Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development – JCLAD 
Vol: 9     Issue:  4   413-428, 2021 

                                                                                                                          ISSN: 2148-1997 

 

421 
 
 

 

/k’ɨt’əl/‘leaf’ as [kɨtəl]. However, a 3;10 years old, ELN had a different realizati

on of the ejective /t’/ [th]. She totally replaced this ejective by aspirated 

[th] in different words. 

Table 3  

The Realization of /tʃ’/ [tʃ] or[/ th] 
Process patterns Word 

position 
Children   Age 

respectively  

 De-ejectivisation   /tʃ’/
[tʃ]or[th] 

WI AMN,ELS,AMN2 3;1, 3;3,4;6 

WM HLN,ELS,AMN2 
ELT  

3;2,3;3,4;6, 
&4;11 

 
Examples  

process  Pattern  Realization   Target  Gloss 

 
De-ejectivisation 

 
 
/tʃ’/  

[tʃ]or[th] 

totutotʰ tʃ'atʃ'utotʃ  chickens 

kutʃ kutʃ’   sit 

ɨtʃawətaləhu 
ɨtawotaləhu 

ɨtʃ’awətaləhu  I play 

ʈʃamma ʈʃ’amma  shoes 

tʰatutotʰ tʃ’atʃ'utotʃ Chickens  

pɨta bɨtʃ’a Yellow  

afɨtʃa afɨtʃ’a  nose 

tʃɨla tʃ’ɨla  tail 

 
 
Like the ejective /t’/, the affricate-ejective/tʃ’/was also produced by most of 

the children who were under this study; they had matured realization. This 
enabled to say between aged 3;0 and 5;0 children were physiological mature 

enough to be able to produce the ejective/tʃ’/ in different word positions 
although there were still children who had different patterns. These few 
children realized the affricate-ejective/tʃ’/ [tʃ]or[th].For instance, in the 

word/ɨtʃ’awətaləhu/ ‘I play’ as [ɨtʃawətaləhu],/ʈʃ’amma/ ‘shoe’ as [ʈʃamma],the 
ejective /tʃ/ was realized as [tʃ] as indicated in the above  examples whereas, 
the 3;10  year old ELN realized /tʃ’/ [th] in her entire words like in the  

word  /tʃ'atʃ'utotʃ/chickens’ as [thatutotʰ]. This girl had a unique realization 
of different consonants, which were not included in this article. Indeed, 

children who were unable to produce this ejective had a tendency to 
substitute by its pulmonic counterpart [tʃ] or [th] but 4;11 years old boy 
ELT substituted/tʃ’/by the other ejective /t’/ like in the word [afɨt’a] for the 

target /afɨtʃ’a/ mean ‘nose’. 
 

 
 
 



  
The Developmental realization of Ejectives                                                           Alamirew and Mekonnen 

422 
 
 

 

Table 4  

Realization of /k’/ [k] 
process patterns Word 

position 
Children   Age 

respectively  

De-
ejectivisation 

/k’/ [k] WI AMN,HLN,ELS, 
AMN2 

3;1,3;2,3;3,4;6 

WM 
WF 

 
AMN,HLN,ELS,A
MN2  

3;2,3;3, 4;6, 

Examples  

 
 
De-ejectivisation 

 Pattern  Realization   Target  Gloss 

 
 

/k’/ [k] 

kɨtəl k’ɨt’əl leaf 

tɨkul t’ɨk’ul Black  

ɨjələkəmu ɨjələk’əmu Sorting 

tɨlɨk tɨlɨk’  big 

kana k’ana Tv channel  

bəkolo bək’olo  Corn  

kəj k’əj  Red  

kutɨl k’ut’ɨr  number 

 
By the same token, like other ejectives, majority of the children were able to 
produce the ejective/k’/as singleton target consonant but if it was clustered, 

it might have a possibility to be dropped or substituted. From thirty-two 
children, who participated in this study, the realization of /k’/as [k] were 
detected in AMN, HLN, ELS and AMN2. As depicted in the above table, these 

children substituted /k’/ by its pulmonic counterpart [k] in the initial, 
medial and final positions. For instance, the word /k’əj/ ‘red’ as [kəj] and 

/k’ut’ɨr/ ‘number’ as [kutɨl] indicated that substitution of /k’/ as[k] at the 
initial position. Similarly, substitution of /k’/as [k] was also observed at 
medial position in the word /ɨjələk’əmu/ ‘sorting’ as [ɨjələkəmu], /bək’olo/ 

meaning ‘corn’ as [bəkolo] and at final positions it was found in the word 
/tɨlɨk’/ ‘big’ as [tlɨk] 

Table 5  
Realization of /s’/ [s]or[tʃ’] 

Process patterns Word  
position 

Children   Age 
respectively  

De-ejectivisation /s’/
[s]or[tʃ] 

WI AMN,HLN,ELS 3;1,3;2, 3;3,4;6 

WM ,WF AMN,HLN,ELS 3;2,3;3, 4;6, 

Examples 
 
 
De-ejectivisation 

 Pattern  Realization   Target  Gloss 

 
/s’/ [s]or[tʃ’] 

səj (ELS) s’əhaj The sun 

səɡul(HLN) s’əɡul hair 

tʃ'aj (ELN) s’ əhaj  The sun  

tʃ'ɘlot(ELN) s’ɘlot  pray 

 



Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development – JCLAD 
Vol: 9     Issue:  4   413-428, 2021 

                                                                                                                          ISSN: 2148-1997 

 

423 
 
 

 

It may not be surprising if a few children substituted fricative- adjective/s’/ 
by its pulmonic counterpart /s/ as it is age related phenomenon but the 

realization of/s’/ [tʃ’] was unique to AMN’s production. As far as this 
fricative-ejective was concerned, it was difficult to reach to a firm conclusion 

due to its rare occurrence in most children’s speeches. Words with fricative- 
ejective /s’/ were only detected in ten children’s speeches, however, except 
AMN, HLN and ELS, the other children were able to produce it properly. HLN 

and ELS realized /s’/as [s] in two words but AMN had a different realization. 
She repeatedly produced the word /s’əhaj/ means ‘the sun’ and /s’ɘlot/ 
‘pray as [tʃ’'aj] and [tʃ'ɘlot] substituting /s’/by the other affricate-ejective [tʃ']. 

 
4. Discussion  

This study was conducted by employing cross-sectional research method to     
take speech’s data within a short period. The data collected from different 
tasks were audio- recorded, transcribed, and analyzed. As the finding of the 

study in different tables indicated, majority of the children who participated 
in this study produced the target ejectives properly in different word 

positions. However, non-target realizations patterns were also detected 
within the same age group children’s speeches. The substitution of ejectives 
by their pulmonic counterparts like /p’/→[p] ,/t’/→[t] or[th], /k’/→[k], 

/ʧ’/→[ʧ] and /s’/→[s] were apparently common in some children' speeches 
in this study. Abebayehu and Demeke (2017) also reported such 
developmental patterns in Sidamuu Affoo speaking children. Similarly, 

Abebayehu (2008) also described the continuous de-ejectivisation of ejectives 
by their pulmonic counterparts in Amharic speaking children with speech 

delay. 
 
In general, majority of three to five years old children, especially, who joined 

preschool education earlier than others did master the production of ejective
as singleton consonants. On the other hand, there were children who 
substituted these consonants by their pulmonic counterparts. This may be 

taken as age-appropriate in aged 3;0-3;3 children’s speeches as similar 
patterns have already been identified in other local language (e. g. Sidamuu 

Affoo) the sin same age level. However, the realization of /ʧ’/→ [t’] or [th,] and 
/s’/→[ʧ’] were uniquely attested in three children’s (ELN (3;10) AMN2(4;6) 
and ELN (4;11) speeches. These patterns of realization attested in aged three 

to five years children may be taken normal or a sign of delay in their 
acquisition process requires study in depth including more participants. 

 
5. Conclusion  
The development of children’s speech production and the realization of the 

target consonants are likely pre-determined by the amount of input children 
get from the families and school environment. The continuous oral practice 
made by the children in different age levels at the school and home fosters 

the acquisition of different consonants as whole and ejectives in particular. 



  
The Developmental realization of Ejectives                                                           Alamirew and Mekonnen 

424 
 
 

 

As researchers’ observation, children who had joined to preschool education 

earlier than others had better production of different consonants. Especially, 
aged between four and five children pronounced the target ejectives 

properly. However, the non-target realizations were also attested in some 
children’s speeches and are likely taken as a sign that the complete 
acquisition of these consonants requires long period until age five or even 

later than this level. This may have different contributing factors. Hence, in 
order to understand comprehensively, to come to a sound conclusion and 
profile the non-target realization of ejectives, large-scale study which include 

a large number of participants and specific target tasks on Amharic 
consonant phonemes in general, ejectives in particular, is required. In 

addition to this, in order to theorize, the non-target realization of ejectives 
whether they are age appropriate or sign of delay, there is a need to be 
checked by continuous oral therapy. 

 
 

References 
 
Abebayehu, M. (2013). Speech production in Amharic Speaking Children with 

Repaired Cleft Palate (PhD Dissertation). Sheffield, UK: University of Sheffield. 
Abebayehu, M. M., & Demeke, J. (2017). Aspects of phonological acquisition in 

Children Speaking Sidaamu Afoo. Journal of Child Language Acquisition and 
development JCLAD, 5(4), 208-218. 

Abenezer, G. (2013). Child language and baby talk (Unpublished.MA thesis). Addis 
Ababa: Addis Ababa University. 

Alqattan, S. (2015). Early Phonological Acquisition by Kuwaiti Arabic Children: (PhD 
dissertation). School of Education Communication and Language Sciences: 
Newcastle University. UK. 

Amayreh, M., & Dyson, A. (2000). Phonetic inventories of young Arabic speaking 
children.  Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 14(3), 193-215) 

Ashby, M. G., Maidment, J.A., & Abberton, E. R. M. (1996). Analytic listening: a 
new approach to ear training. Speech, Hearing and Language, 9,1-10. 

Beckman, M., & Edwards, J. (2003). Language-Specific and language universal 
aspectsof lingual obstruent productions in Japanese acquiring Children.Journ
al of the Society of Japan, 7(2),18-28]. 

Bernhardt, B., Joseph, P. Stumberger. (1998). Handbook of phonological 
development from the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. 
San Diego: Academy press. 

Bybee, J. (2010). Language, usage, and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University 
Press. 

Bybee, J. (2006). From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. 
 Language, 82(4), 711–733. 

Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin and Use. New York:  
 Praeger press. 

Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The Sound Pattern of English .Harper and Raw, 
 New York. 

Clark, E.V. (2009). First language acquisition. (2nded) New York: Cambridge 
University  Press. 



Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development – JCLAD 
Vol: 9     Issue:  4   413-428, 2021 

                                                                                                                          ISSN: 2148-1997 

 

425 
 
 

 

Clark, E. V. (2003). First language acquisition. New York: Cambridge 
University Press 

Deen, K. U. (2002). The omission of inflectional prefixes in the acquisition of Nairobi 
 Swahili (Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation). UCLA. 

Dodd, Demuth, K., Moloi, F., & Machobane, M. (2010). ‘3-yea-olds’ comprehension,
 production generalization of Sesotho passives. Cognition, 115(2), 238-251. 

Ferguson, A.C. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist New 
 Series. 66, 6: 103–114. 

Gleason, G.B., and R., B. (2009). The Development of Language. Cambridge: 
Cambridge  University (7thed). 

Ingram, D. (1989). First language acquisition: method, description and explanation. 
 Cambridge; Cambridge University Press. 

Ingram, D. (1988). Jacobson revisited: some evidence from the acquisition of Polish. 

Lingua 75, 55-82. 
Ingram, D. (1978). The role of the syllable in phonological development. In Bell & 

 Hooper (1978). 143 156. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press. 
Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. The 

Hague & Paris: Mouton. 
Kehoe, M. (2001). Prosodic patterns in young children's multisyllabic word 

Production.  Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 284-
295. 

Lust, B. (2006). Child language acquisition, and growth. Cambridge: Cambridge 
 University press. Machinery(eds.). Handbook of Child Language. London: 
 Basil  Black well. 

Menn, L. (1980). Phonological theory and child phonology. In Child phonology (pp. 
23-41). Academic Press. 

Menn, L. (1977). An auto segmental approach to child phonology first explorations. 
 In G.N. Clements (ed.), Harvard studies in phonology. Linguistics 
Department:  Harvard University. 315 -334. 

O'Grady, W. (2005). How children learn language. Cambridge: Cambridge University 
 press. 

Pinker, S., & Ullman, M.T. (2002). The past and future of the past tense. Trend in 
 Cognitive Science 6.11, 456–63. 

Prince,A., Smolensky,P.(2004).Optimality theory:constraint interaction in generative
 grammar. Malden, MA. Oxford, UK. Blackwell. Revision of 1993 technical 
 report, Rutgers University. Center of cognitive science available on Rutgers
 Optimality, Archive, ROA-537. 

Renner, L.F. (2017). The magic match–speech production, and perception in language 
 acquisition (A doctoral dissertation in phonetics): Sckhtolm. 

Rose, Y. (2009). Internal and external influences on child language productions. 
Ioana  Chitosan, François Pellegrino & Egidio Marsico (eds.) Approaches to 
Phonological  Complexity, 329-351. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 

Sabahat, A. (2012). Crucial differences between a behaviourist and a nativist views 
of  first language acquisition. https:/ amnasabahat.wordpress.com 
/2012/06/  19 crucial-difference-between-a-behaviorist-and-a-nativist-view-
of-first-  language-acquisition. 

Stampe, D. (1973). A dissertation on natural phonology. (Doctoral dissertation) 
University of Chicago. Statistics Agency of Ethiopia 

         http://www.gov.et/imaged  document/pd files/regional/ 
CountryLevel.pdf. 

http://www.gov.et/


  
The Developmental realization of Ejectives                                                           Alamirew and Mekonnen 

426 
 
 

 

Smith, N. V. (1973). The Acquisition of phonology: A case study. London: Cambridge 
 University Press. 

Smith, R., & Leveillé, M. (1973). The French syntax of a child’s noun phrases. 
Archives de Psychologies 42:207–269. 

Stoel-Gammon, C. (2006). Infancy: phonological development.  Journal of Infancy. 
642-648. Seattle University of Washington. 

Takeida, A. (2011). The children's Role in Parents Language Shift. Sydney: Sydney 
 University press. 

Tariku, N. (2019). Oromo phonology by typically growing children (PhD. 
Dissertation).  Addis Ababa: Addis Ababa University. 

Watson, J. (1997). The theory of human caring: retrospective and prospective. 
Nursing science quarterly,10,49 -52. 

         https://doi.org/10.1177/089431849701000114 
Whyatt, B. (1994). Baby talk the language addressed to language acquiring 

children: A Review of the Problem. Poznan: Adam Mickiewicz University. 
Winitz, H. (1969). Articulatory acquisition and Behavior. New York: Appleton 

Century-crofts.  
Xu Rattanasone, N., & Demuth, K. (2014). Acquisition of linguistic tonal systems. In 

 P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), (pp.352-353). 
Zoltan, D. (2007). Research methods in applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University 
 Press. 
 
 

 
 
 

 
 

 
 
 

 
 

 
 
 

 
 
 

 
 

 
 
 

 
 

 
 



Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development – JCLAD 
Vol: 9     Issue:  4   413-428, 2021 

                                                                                                                          ISSN: 2148-1997 

 

427 
 
 

 

 
Appendices 
 

Appendix 1 

Amharic consonant phonemes 

 

Bilabial Labio-

dental 

Dental  Alveol

ar 

Postalveola

r Palatal vela

r 

Glottal 

Stops  P     b  t d  k k  ɡ ʔ 

Affricative     ʧʤ    

Nasal M   n  ɲ   

Fricative  f     v  s       z ʃʒ   h 

Trill    r     

Approximates W     j   

Lateral     l     

Ejectives  stops                     P’  t’    k’  

Ejecte  ejective affricative         ʧ’       

Ejective  fricative                       s’     

 

 

 
 

Appendix 2 

Amharic vowels phonemes  

 

 

 



  
The Developmental realization of Ejectives                                                           Alamirew and Mekonnen 

428 
 
 

 

 

Appendix 3 

The frequency of the occurrence of different Amharic consonant phonemes