Journal of Childhood, Education & Society 
Volume 2, Issue 3, 2021, 267-286                                                              ISSN: 2717-638X 
DOI: 10.37291/2717638X.202123118 Research Article 

 

 
©2021 Journal of Childhood, Education & Society. This is an open access article under the CC BY- NC- ND license. 

 
 

Science starts early: A literature review examining the 
influence of early childhood teachers’ perceptions of gender 
on teaching practices 

Erin E. Hamel1 

 
 

Abstract: Women are underrepresented in science fields as compared to men and 
although much research has been dedicated to understanding this disparity, most has 
been conducted on older aged children. However, this excludes the youngest and 
arguably most impressionable group of students: preschoolers. This study reviewed the 
literature to investigate how early childhood teachers’ perceptions of gender influence 
their teaching practices. Qualitative analysis and coding of 31 articles resulted in five main 
categories: Teacher Perception, Curriculum, Teacher Interactions, Gender Identity, and Social 
Standing. Results are discussed in the context of early childhood science teaching practices 
to better understand the role of the teacher and gender bias in young children’s preschool 
science experiences and how it may impact their future science interests.  

 
Article History 
Received: 30 July 2021    
Accepted: 09 November 2021 
 
Keywords 
Early childhood; Science; 
Preschool; Gender  

Introduction 

It is widely known that girls and women are underrepresented in science fields. One explanation for 
the noted discrepancy are gender socialization processes and societal attitudes that encourage traditional 
gender roles (Eccles et al., 1993; Eccles, 2007; Haworth et al., 2009; Leibham et al., 2013). Gender roles are 
believed to be socially constructed through values and beliefs present in relationships, society, and 
institutions (Davies, 2003). Gender roles are acquired early in life and have the potential to influence both 
males and females (Bigler & Liben, 2006).  

Developmental Intergroup Theory (Bigler & Liben, 2006) aims to explain children’s acquisition of 
stereotype and prejudice by proposing that “biases may be largely under environmental control and thus 
might be shaped via educational, social, and legal policies” (p.162). This idea is supported by a study of 
interactions in the home environment, finding that mothers’ perceptions of their child’s math abilities 
predicted child beliefs about their math ability (Gunderson et al., 2012). The family context has been a focus 
of research in developing gender roles. A study analyzing the conversations of parents and children during 
science-related tasks indicated that parents perceived science activities as more difficult and less interesting 
for their daughters than their sons (Tenenbaum & Leaper, 2003). As a result, interactions with daughters 
and sons differed, indicating that differential treatment in regards to science occurs in the home 
environment. Further, research indicates that opportunities for science learning also varies, with parents of 
young boys ages 4 to 7 years old reporting more science-related opportunities for their child than parents 
of young girls of the same age (Alexander et al., 2012). Yet these differences are not confined to the home 
environment. In a study of interactions between parents and their children at a museum exhibit, researchers 
found that boys were three times more likely to receive science explanations from their parents than girls 
despite equal amounts of conversation (Crowley et al., 2001).  

Likewise, it is conceivable that early childhood teachers, knowingly or unknowingly, exhibit similar 
gender bias in their interactions which may impact the children in their care. Teachers are largely in control 
of the quality of the classroom. This is particularly noteworthy because for young children, preschool is the 

_____________ 
1 University of Georgia, Mary Frances Early College of Education, Department of Communication Sciences and Special Education, Athens, GA, USA, e-mail: erin.hamel@uga.edu, 
ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9890-8484  

https://doi.org/10.37291/2717638X.202123118
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
mailto:erin.hamel@uga.edu
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9890-8484


Erin E. HAMEL 

268 

first experience in a formal educational setting and sets the stage for development, future interests, and 
learning. Therefore, it is important to understand early childhood teacher’s attitudes and perceptions of 
gender and how it may influence teacher practices. Teachers may explicitly or implicitly demonstrate 
gender biased views or stereotypes that influence their interactions, management, and pedagogical 
decisions in the classroom. Exposing young children to early gender stereotypes has been shown to 
influence children’s long-term interests and ideas about intelligence (Bian et al., 2017). The present study 
systematically examines the literature using the research question, How do early childhood teachers perceive 
gender and how does this influence their teaching practices? 

Developmental Intergroup Theory  

This research is guided by Bigler & Liben’s (2006) Developmental Intergroup Theory (DIT), an 
approach to understanding and explaining how children develop stereotyping and prejudice. Other 
theories have attempted to explain children’s acquisition of biases, DIT furthers those understandings by 
describing why some traits and not others become the focus of bias (Bigler & Liben, 2006, 2007). Many 
variations of human dimensions exist, such as handedness, skin color, gender, and eye color; yet not all of 
these features are prominent factors in stereotyping and prejudice. DIT addresses this difference by 
combining concepts from social identity intergroup theory and cognitive theories of constructivism offered 
by Piaget to propose that stereotypes and prejudice are attained largely through explicit and implicit biases 
displayed in the environment coupled with children’s developing ability for categorizing salient attributes 
(Bigler & Liben, 2007). DIT suggests that three main processes occur for individuals when forming social 
stereotyping (Bigler & Liben, 2007). First, the child establishes psychological salience of attributes that differ 
across people. Next, children categorize individuals by salient traits which is part of a cognitive-
developmental process, constructivism, in which the child attaches meanings (positive or negative, true or 
untrue) to the categories they have created. Finally, this results in the third process of developing 
stereotypes and prejudice related to salient features. Given that the purpose of this paper is to explore 
children’s preschool experiences related to adult perceptions of gender, the first process of DIT is 
particularly relevant. Understanding how children are establishing psychological salience of personal 
attributes, which is largely based on their experiences, may provide insights on how to prevent the 
formation of social stereotypes and prejudice. Bigler & Liben (2007) suggest children’s formation of 
psychological salience of attributes occurs in four ways: proportional group size, perceptual 
discriminability, explicit labeling and use, and implicit use. Proportional group size is relevant when 
working with populations that form a minority. In the case of this study, it is less important and was not a 
focus of my analysis as males and females make up roughly equal amounts of the population. For this 
study, the factors of perceptual discrimination, explicit use, and implicit use are most relevant.  

Perceptual Discrimination   

It is important to consider how children might be receiving messages from their environment and 
interactions and then subsequently shaping these messages into thoughts about their own or others gender. 
Perceptual discrimination refers to noticeable features, which young children tend to focus on, such as 
noticing hair color instead of less-noticeable handedness. Early childhood educators who intentionally 
draw attention to gender defining features, clothing, or traits would make gender more salient for children. 
While categorization is an important skill for children to learn and an almost infinite amount of ways exist 
for categorizing, some are more prevalent than others, such as gender. A continuous emphasis on gender 
categorization by a teacher may signal to children that this is an important bases for classifying people. 
This is significant in relation to science because children are forming ideas about their own gender and 
others gender which may unintentionally serve as a foundation for science-related stereotypes regarding 
who does and does not do science and who belongs in scientific fields. Beyond science, this raises ethical 
concerns for gender nonconforming children who would benefit from gender neutral language and deserve 
a supportive environment that respects their most authentic self. Gender nonconformity is beyond the 
scope of this paper, but warrants further research and consideration.  

 



Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood… 

269 

Explicit Labeling and Use  

Early childhood educators may explicitly categorize gender through their daily routines and 
practices (playing a song during music and movement that requires girls to move in one way and boys to 
move in another). Educators can explicitly enforce gender roles by managing the classroom with a focus 
on traditional gender behavior, such as intentionally leading girls to more feminine centers in the classroom 
to practice gender roles such as “playing house” or “cooking.” This type of explict labeling and use could 
draw children’s attention to and reinforce stereotypes that deter children from playing outside of their 
perceived gender role, potentially limiting girls’ play and exploration of science-focused centers.  

Implicit Labeling and Use  

Implicitly shaping gender stereotypes in the early childhood classrooms occurs when educators 
make gender unnecessarily salient (instructing a child to “Ask the man if the mail has arrived”). Another 
aspect would be grouping children by gender without explicitly labeling the groups but then segregating 
the groups. This approach shows that when groups are segregated by a feature, although the feature is not 
labeled, higher bias occurs (Bigler & Liben, 2007). Teachers implicit labeling has the potential to introduce 
or reinforce bias making it vital that teachers are aware of their own prejudice and biases related to 
children’s gender and science.  

Overall, DIT helps explain factors that may be contributing to the formation of social stereotypes and 
prejudice in young children. Early childhood teachers’ perceptions of gender may influence their classroom 
teaching practices which could impact children’s science learning opportunities and subsequently 
children’s science-related interests and experiences. Thus, it is important to understand early childhood 
teachers’ perceptions and attitudes about gender and how it influences their practices. 

Literature Review  

 A generous amount of effort has been dedicated to studying gender inequality in science, 
technology, engineering, and mathematical (STEM) fields, most conducted at the middle-school, high-
school, and college levels (Riegle-Crumb et al., 2012; Robinson-Cimpian et al., 2014; Wang, 2013). Even 
extensive reviews of the literature on girls and science are comprised largely of studies on older students, 
with only a handful of articles on elementary schools identified (Brotman & Moore, 2008). Unfortunately, 
this overlooks the youngest and arguably most impressionable population of students: preschoolers.  

Findings on science achievement are at odds. In elementary school, girls show less interest in science 
than boys with girls’ interest continuing to decline with age so that by ages 10-14 a marked difference in 
science attitudes by gender appears (Catsambis, 1995). Unfortunately, not only does interest wane for girls, 
but they perceive science as uninteresting and boring (Jones et al., 2000). Although some research indicates 
girls have a less positive attitude and interest in science, there is evidence that they perform equally as well 
or better than their male counterparts in science class (Catsambis, 1995; Greenfield, 1996; Jones et al., 2000). 
However, more recent literature reveals a gender gap in science achievement beginning as early as first 
grade (Curran & Kellogg, 2016) and persisting over time, resulting in a call for intervention at an earlier 
stage of development, such as preschool (Morgan et al., 2016). One area of consensus between early 
childhood and the science field is on the suitability of teaching science in the early years. Most agree that 
children have a natural curiosity of the world around them which motivates them to explore scientific 
concepts and topics in authentic ways. In addition, research supports science education beginning early on 
in life as a way to develop positive attitudes towards science, expose children to scientific concepts through 
language and teachable moments, and develop scientific thinking (Eshach & Fried, 2005; Morgan et al., 
2016). There is increasing evidence that children’s early interests (including those that are science-related) 
can persist over time and influence the course of learning (Alexander et al., 2012) underscoring the 
importance of providing early opportunities for exploring science.  

The early childhood environment may be ripe for children’s science exploration but barriers exist to 
teaching science. Teachers report lack of confidence and content knowledge (Gerde et al., 2018; Kallery & 
Psillos, 2001; Park et al., 2017), time (Greenfield et al., 2009; Park et al., 2017), and materials (Tu, 2006) as 



Erin E. HAMEL 

270 

factors impeding science instruction in the classroom. Notably, these reasons impact both structural and 
process quality indicators. Structural indicators refer to a characteristic of the environment and process 
indicators refer to interaction between individuals (Cassidy et al., 2005). Structural and process indicators 
are concepts by which the quality of early childhood environments are often assessed. Barriers in both of 
these areas are likely to affect the amount and quality of science teaching and subsequently, children’s 
science interest and learning. If the quality and quantity of science teaching in preschool is low for all 
children, it is especially detrimental for young girls who benefit when teachers foster their sustained science 
interest (Leibham et al., 2013). For preschool-aged girls, an intense science interest predicts a significantly 
higher science self-concept at 8 years old (Leibham et al., 2013). Science self-concept is defined as “an 
understanding of their attributes, abilities, and values” (Leibham et al., 2013) and can be constructed 
through daily interactions such as play (Chafel, 2003).  For preschool-aged boys, early interests are not 
predictive of science achievement as they are for girls (Leibham et al., 2013). This underlines the importance 
of fostering girls’ early interests in science during the preschool years.  

An important component of early childhood education is the foundational relationships upon which 
subsequent experiences and knowledge are built. Research implicates teacher attitude as a contributing 
influence on developing gender stereotypes in children (Beilock et al., 2010; Robinson-Cimpian et al., 2014). 
Whether implicit or explicit, teachers’ actions and words can convey gender bias and influence the types 
of activities and interests’ young children develop, even shaping later career choices (Bian et al., 2017). 
Gender bias has the potential to negatively and inaccurately influence young children’s image of 
themselves and their capabilities. 

In summary, gender differences in preschool science experiences and science achievement are less 
understood than those that occur at other levels of education. Reported differences in science achievement 
could mean two things: girls are either as capable as boys in science but do not select science careers, or 
that girls’ lack of interest and/or achievement is identifiable early on and persists. Either way, this warrants 
further investigation of classroom processes, such as teacher and child interactions in the preschool years, 
which have been identified as an ideal time for teaching and exploring science concepts. Science teaching 
is lacking, in part, because early childhood teachers feel unprepared to teach science activities (Greenfield 
et al., 2009). While children are naturally curious about the world around them, developing scientific 
reasoning skills requires both engagement and interaction around science content and materials (Gelman 
& Brenneman, 2012; Morris et al., 2012). Science materials, a structural component of the early childhood 
environment, are selected and displayed for use by classroom teachers who lack confidence in their science 
abilities (Greenfield et al., 2009). Teachers identify several reasons that science instruction is lacking in early 
childhood such as their lack of confidence and instructional time but research to date has not included 
teacher perceptions of gender that may also impact their interactions with children in the classroom 
(Greenfield et al., 2009). These perceptions may implicitly or explicitly influence both structural and process 
quality in the early childhood environment. A clearer understanding of how teachers’ gender perceptions 
may influence their teaching practices and subsequently impact children’s learning opportunities and 
experiences in the preschool classroom is needed. The purpose of this paper is to address this gap in the 
literature.  

Method 

The goal of this paper is to examine research on early childhood teachers’ perceptions of gender in 
the classroom. I used several search strategies to identify studies. First, a discovery catalog and database 
ProQuest Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC), were searched using two combinations of 
terms: 1) preschool teacher and gender; and 2) teacher attitude, gender, and preschool. This resulted in 37 articles 
and 31 articles, respectively.   These terms were selected with a goal of including early childhood articles 
related to teacher perception and gender in any domain of learning. To identify articles related specifically 
to gender topics in education and science content in preschools, the search was narrowed to a selected set 
of journals (Brotman & Moore, 2008). Seven specific journals were searched that focus on science in 
education: Journal of Research in Science Teaching, International Journal of Science Education, Journal of Science 



Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood… 

271 

Teacher Education, Science Education, and Research in Science Education. A review of two other journals were 
also included because they are not limited to the field of science but might provide insights into the research 
topic: Gender and Education and American Education Research Journal (Brotman & Moore, 2008). These efforts 
resulted in an additional 4 articles specific to gender at the early childhood level. Lastly, a search was 
conducted of PsychINFO using a combination of the terms early childhood education, teacher attitudes, teacher 
perceptions and gender. This resulted in an additional 32 articles. In total, 104 articles were originally 
identified.   

After reviewing the collected articles, it was apparent that several of the articles were beyond the 
scope of this paper and did not meet the purpose of the study. In order to address the proposed research 
question in a systematic way, three main inclusion criteria were established for the review. First, the article 
must be published in a peer reviewed journal; this eliminated doctoral dissertations, master theses, and 
other articles printed in news-type magazines. Second, the article must be relevant to the topic of early 
childhood teachers’ perceptions of gender and children 6 years old and younger. Third, the article must be 
in English. This led to research contributions from the following countries: Australia, Canada, Denmark, 
Finland, Indonesia, Israel, Japan, Norway, Poland, Qatar, Spain, Sweden, Turkey, and the United States. 
Specific interest included the teachers’ practice influencing children, teacher perceptions of gender, and 
teacher values about gender for children kindergarten age and younger.  

Articles that were excluded from the review were those that did not meet the purpose of the research. 
This included studies on efforts to increase the amount of male preschool teachers employed in the early 
childhood field, transgender and non-binary research and trainings for early childhood teachers, the 
development of sexuality in preschool, parent gender perceptions and influence, and articles reporting 
gender differences among various academic domains or interventions. After categorizing articles using 
established inclusion and exclusion criteria, 27 articles were identified for review. As recommended in 
methodological literature, a secondary search was conducted using references lists from recent literature 
reviews (Fraenkel et al., 2016). This resulted in an additional four articles that met inclusion criteria. A 
complete summary of the final 31 articles analyzed for this review is available in Table 2.  

Data Analysis 

A thorough review was conducted using content analysis (Saldaña, 2015) to systemmatically 
examine articles for purpose, participants and setting, methodology, and major findings. Major findings 
were manually coded by the reported overall effect on young children. Articles that were primarily focused 
on teacher attitudes towards gender but did not include child outcomes made up their own category. Three 
articles included a child outcome based on parent perception but not teacher perception. These were 
removed from the final analysis. Fourteen initial codes were then combined and reduced to five overall 
categories that signify the findings in the literature (Saldaña, 2015). Articles were organized into established 
categories demonstrating the range of results in each category. Another aspect of the articles analyzed 
included the date of publication of the studies to identify potential patterns or trends in how this topic has 
been explored both recently and historically. In the sections that follow I discuss the major findings and 
implications related to science learning in the early childhood classroom. It is noteworthy that the articles 
originate from multiple countries, adding complexity to the synthesis of findings, but strengthening the 
emphasis on the importance of the need to understand the impact of gender on children in early childhood 
classrooms world-wide.  

Results 

 The five categories that emerged from data analysis were: Teacher Perception, Curriculum, Teacher 
Interactions, Gender Identity, and Social Standing. Each of these categories was created by combining the 
initial codes (See Table 1).  

 

 



Erin E. HAMEL 

272 

Table 1. Categories and underlying codes  

Teacher Perception Curriculum Teacher Interactions Social Standing Gender Identity 

Teacher perception 
Teacher attitude 
Awareness 

Play 
Opportunities for play 
Selection of activities 
Materials for play  

Behavior 
Adult relationships 
Engagement with 
teacher 
Discipline 

Interactions with peers 
Social standing 

 

Understanding of 
identity 

 

Teachers’ perceptions of gender were the focus of most of the research studies (see Figure 1) and 
established the category Teacher Perception. Teachers’ perceptions of how they view young children by 
gender provides insight into teacher thoughts and opinions but it is not possible to infer how this translates 
into practice and pedagogical decisions without further evidence. Studies that reported child outcomes 
suggest that an early childhood teacher’s perception of gender influences four areas, which generated the 
other remaining categories: Curriculum, Teacher Interactions, Social Standing, and Gender Identity (see Figure 
1). 

 
Figure 1. Proportion of Articles by Category 

The oldest publication in the review was categorized in the Curriculum category; however, this 
category also contained recent publications as well. The Curriculum category includes articles related to 
children’s learning, play, along with the materials and environment that create the curriculum. Young 
children’s work is often considered their play and learning through play is a foundational element of early 
childhood education which has been studied for many years, so it is not surprising that this category 
contained the oldest publication. Results show that chronologically the category with the most recent 
publications was Teacher Interactions. This could in part be due to an emphasis in the field on process quality 
indicators and improved measures of teacher interactions. 

Teacher Perceptions of Gender 

Early childhood teachers’ perceptions of gender were investigated through surveys, questionnaires, 
and semi-structured interviews (See Table 2). One of the articles in this category was a review of articles 
related to sexism in the context of early childhood. In that review conducted by Duke and McCarthy (2009), 
nine articles (39%) identified that some teachers were uncomfortable with non-stereotypical gender 
behavior by children and eight (26%) of the articles described ways that early childhood education 

Curriculum 29%

Teacher Perception 32%

Teacher Interactions 
29%

Gender 
Identity

3% Social 
Standing

7%



Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood… 

273 

programs reinforce traditional gender roles. In one study, teachers thought that gender stereotypes were 
reinforced through the use of children’s literature and even television programming. Other articles 
included in this category shed light on teachers experiences, perceptions and classroom practices.  For 
example, early childhood teachers recall playing in gender stereotypical ways in their own childhood 
(Sandberg & Pramling-Samuelsson, 2005). Interestingly, Erden (2004) found that although teachers 
reported an egalitarian view on gender, when asked whether they agreed or not with gender traditional 
statements such as, “girls are more emotional than boys,” up to 40% of teachers agreed. This indicates that 
teachers perceive the genders as having traditional differences while also reporting a belief in gender 
equality. In the classroom, teachers who held egalitarian attitudes had also adopted egalitarian discipline 
styles indicating that at least in one way teacher perception is related to teacher practice (Erden, 2004). How 
teachers perceive appropriateness of children’s behavior, especially aggression, by child gender showed 
differences as well. For example, female teachers differed from male teachers in their identification and 
ratings of children’s aggression (Pellegrini et al., 2011). Male teachers identified more aggression in the 
classroom than their female counterparts (Pellegrini et al, 2011). In addition, one study showed that male 
teachers thought that a child displaying aggressive behavior would perform better and be less likely to be 
excluded in play (Bosacki et al., 2015). 

In summary, early childhood teacher’s perceptions of children’s gender is influenced by the teacher’s 
own previous experiences and their own gender. Perceptions of gender can influence the meaning that 
teacher’s assign to children’s behavior and how they respond to behavior through discipline style. While 
this is informative, the extent to how these perceptions influence children in the classroom is less clear.  

Curriculum   

In the literature addressing curriculum, a variety of qualitative and quantitative methods measuring 
learning and play in the classroom were used (See Table 2). Researchers examined both structural and 
process quality to address children’s classroom experiences. Structure quality was investigated by 
measuring aspects of the environment such as the types of books and toys available to children as well as 
where in the classroom the teacher most often played. Processes of play were measured through examining 
the types of activities and the interactions children had with teachers in the classroom, which were 
investigated using observations, interviews, and focus groups. One study in this category explored the 
environment and culture of the classroom. Borve and Borve (2017) studied the classroom physical 
environment and conducted interviews with teachers and assistant teachers (15 female and 3 male) at a 
Kindergarten in Norway. They found that the environment was primarily designed by the female teachers, 
leading to an environment described by the teachers as “feminine”. Areas were designed based on how 
teachers felt children would use the space which led to teachers creating and designing the classroom in a 
traditionally gendered way. For example, teachers identified boys as playing in ways that were rougher 
and louder and consequently, required more space. Girls, on the other hand, were thought to play in more 
quiet and gentle ways. This resulted in areas designed for boys taking up more space than those designed 
for girls. And although children occasionally crossed into all spaces, teachers thought that children used 
the spaces differently based on child gender. Authors suggest that spaces were not designed and set up for 
gender neutral play and that the intention for play impacts how children used the space. This resulted in 
play practices that mimicked adults’ ideas of gender (Borve & Borve, 2017).  Similarly, interviews with 
teachers during focus groups in Spain revealed gender division in areas of the classroom consistent with 
their views on how boys and girls play (Rodriguez et al., 2006). In these focus groups, teachers noted that 
they had expectations for girls’ play that was mothering and nurturing and when boys played in this same 
caring way, some of the teachers even responded with surprise. It is important to note that while teachers 
voiced an expectation for gendered behavior, they did not negatively reinforce nonconforming play. In 
fact, they used moments of unexpected nonconforming play to talk about equity (Rodriguez et al., 2006).   

Another structural component of the environment and important part of the curriculum is literature, 
including the books available within the classroom and those selected and read by the teacher. In a study 
of 618 book readings, 433 unique books were presented to children across six preschools in Sweden. Results 



Erin E. HAMEL 

274 

showed that significantly fewer girls were main characters than boys (Lynch, 2016). Both teachers’ and 
children’s choices for books to read were significantly more likely to have a male main character. Gender 
equity in literature is deficient in preschool classrooms (Lynch, 2016). This is problematic because children 
may perceive girls as less important because they hold a lesser role in literature than boys. When children 
view less minorities, including females, in literature, they may internalize that they have less value in 
society (Lynch, 2016). It is important to note that teachers’ selections of children’s books are limited to books 
in publication and the lack of children’s books with female main characters is largely out of their control.  

Toy selection is another important aspect of play. One study conducted by Trawick-Smith and 
colleagues (2015) showed that teachers and parents select toys that engage boys more than girls.  
Researchers asked teachers and parents to nominate nine toys that they felt would benefit child 
development. Observations of the toys being used in the classroom indicated that child gender was an 
influencing factor in children’s selection of toys and the quality with which it was played (Trawick-Smith 
et al., 2015). Results showed that when boys played with the toys, the quality play score was higher 
(Trawick-Smith et al., 2015). Quality was measured in seven areas: thinking and learning, problem solving, 
curiosity and inquiry behaviors, sustained interest, creative expression, symbolic representations, and 
interactions, communications, and collaborations with peers. This finding could be problematic because it 
indicates that teachers and parents may not select toys that can engage both genders in equally beneficial 
ways. Children selected items they enjoyed but their play with these toys didn’t automatically result in 
high-quality play. Thus teachers may need to carefully consider how to enhance or scaffold play with a 
preferred toy. Teachers’ careful observations of play should go beyond child preference and include a focus 
on quality of engagement. 

Rodriguez and colleagues (2006) found that teachers gave children the freedom to select and engage 
in play without adult intervention and that it was not necessary to address gender stereotypes in the 
classroom because children are choosing their play preference. As mentioned earlier, teachers expected 
children to play in traditional gendered ways and therefore create an environment conducive to gendered 
play (Borve & Borve, 2017). It is also plausible that children have been socialized to play in gendered ways 
via media, society, and other influences and that early childhood environments perpetuate the stereotype 
by not disputing them. Another study found that a teacher’s presence in areas of the classroom, including 
the science area, drew children to the area to play (Tomes, 1995).  

Early childhood teachers are also responsible for planning activities. Planning is influenced by how 
teachers view gender roles, as shown by interviews and observations in a study of teachers and children 
in Australia (Chapman, 2016). Using observations of the classroom and interviews with four teachers, 
researchers found that teachers who held more egalitarian views on child gender and play appeared to 
acknowledge gender issues in the classroom and implement strategies to counter gender roles or 
stereotypes (Chapman, 2016). The idea that teachers perceive play differently based on child gender is not 
new and was supported in a study by Logue and Harvey (2010) where teachers shared their belief that 
boys participate in more superhero play and girls participate in more nurturing activities, such as “playing 
house” or “family”. Teachers then self-reported stopping dramatic play almost half the time (48%) for boys 
and only 29% of the time for girls, and intervening in social conflict more frequently for boys than for girls. 
The same teachers thought that their own plans for dramatic play were more productive than children’s 
own imaginative play (Logue & Harvey, 2010).  Taken together, these findings suggest that teachers’ 
perceptions of gender play can influence how teachers plan for play and may result in differences in how 
children engage in and are supported in their play.  

Outside of planned activities, teachers have the opportunity to use everyday experiences as 
teachable moments. Granger and colleagues (2017) observed children’s free play and found that teachers 
facilitated gender-typed activities based on the gender make-up of the group. For example, 27 female 
teachers in Head Start classrooms working with girls facilitated masculine and gender-neutral activities 
significantly more often than feminine activities. This same study found that gender-neutral activities were 
implemented more frequently with groups of girls than with groups composed of boys (Granger et al., 
2017).   



Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood… 

275 

Teacher Interactions with Children 

Early childhood teachers have countless interactions with children each day. Most research in this 
category indicates that a child’s gender influences the interactions he or she has with the teacher (see Table 
2). Interactions include classroom conversations, responses, directives, and engagement between the 
teacher and the child.  For example, in-service and pre-service preschool teachers in Sweden thought 
“headstrong” and “disobedient” behavior from girls was less allowable than similar behaviors from boys 
(Hedlin & Aberg, 2018). This led to the teacher interacting in a dismissive way towards the student who 
was then labeled as “very troublesome” (Hedlin & Aberg, 2018). Another study showed a comparable 
finding when staff members at 80 kindergartens in Demark responded to gender-related dilemmas. 
Teachers’ responses to the gender dilemmas differed by child gender, but not by teacher gender, indicating 
that regardless of teacher gender, teachers consistently treated boys and girls differently (Olsen & 
Smeplass, 2016).  A similar outcome was found in a study in Indonesia, where informal and formal 
interviews with teachers, conversations with children, and field notes of daily classroom activities were 
analyzed to explore care from staff. Results indicated that teachers cared for children in stereotypical ways 
that likely preserved gendered behavior in children (Adriany & Warin, 2014). For example, in the annual 
school musical celebrating diversity and uniqueness, the female children were assigned feminine roles of 
flowers, fairies, and a princess; while the male children took on the role of fish. The message of respecting 
differences and celebrating each individual was lost in the expectation for children to perform traditional 
gender roles. Overall, teachers reported that cultural diversity was celebrated at the Indonesian school; 
however, gender was not openly addressed and traditional gender behavior was encouraged (Adriany & 
Warin, 2014). In a study conducted in the United States, differential treatment in four teaching practices 
(physical interactions between children, verbal and physical directions of body, voice control, and behavior 
management) contributed to gender differences in 5-year-old children in 5 preschool classrooms (Martin, 
1998). Differences included how the teacher interacted with a child through restricting a child’s voice, 
dressing-up, limiting physicality, or instructing children how to physically be with one another (Martin, 
1998). Still, some hope for gender equitable interactions in preschool classrooms exists. In a large study of 
342 children aged 34 – 63 months from 100 classrooms in the United States, children’s positive engagement 
with their teachers was analyzed for associations with individual child factors (age and gender). 
Researchers found that neither age nor gender were significantly associated with children’s positive 
engagement with their teachers (Vitiello et al., 2012). Notable differences from this study as compared to 
the older Martin (1998) study include the number of participants, the extended child age range, and the 
measure of engagement. In the latter study, a standardized observational assessment, the InCLASS, was 
used to measure interactions; whereas the former used a semi-structured observation with field notes. In 
summary, support exists to suggest early childhood teachers’ interactions with young children are 
influenced in some ways by child gender.  

Social Standing   

The Social Standing category was created by combining studies indicating that a child’s perceived 
popularity, relationships, or social competence was influenced by adult gender perceptions. Only two 
articles belong to this category (See Table 2). The purpose of the first study was to explore the impact of 
gender stereotyping on young children. Researchers found that teachers rated girls and boys who 
conformed to gender stereotypes as more likable than their non-conforming peers (Sullivan et al., 2018). In 
fact, teachers rated girls more likable than boys and boys were likely to experience criticism for violating 
gender stereotypes (Sullivan et al., 2018). One aim of the second study was to assess and compare gender 
differences in prosocial behavior among preschoolers in a Middle Eastern country. While gender 
differences in prosocial behavior are well documented, this research was unique in that it was the first in 
Doha, Qatar (Al-Thani & Semmar, 2017). Teachers were asked to complete a questionnaire regarding their 
interactions with students and observations. Findings indicated that teachers perceived boys as displaying 
less prosocial behaviors than girls. Significant gender differences were found in subscales of teacher 
preferred behavior, peer preferred behavior, and school adjustment indicating that teachers of preschool 
children in Doha, Qatar, rated boys as underperforming girls in social competence (Al-Thani & Semmar, 



Erin E. HAMEL 

276 

2017). 

Child Gender Identity 

The category of Gender Identity contained only one study, which was conducted in Japan (See Table 
2). Gender identity is viewed as an individual’s perception of who they are and how they characterize 
themselves in terms of culturally defined male and female roles (Wood & Eagly, 2015). For example, a 
study of private day care teachers in Tokyo was conducted over the course of one year, in which researchers 
observed one to two times per month for how children and teachers used the word kawaii (cute, lovable). 
Teachers used kawaii as an indication of gender and described girls using this term which led to girls using 
kawaii to describe or refer to themselves (Burdelski & Mitsuhashi, 2010). This research found that female,  
but not male, children adopted kawaii as part of their identity. 

 

 



Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood… 

277 

Table 2. Articles by category 
 

Author(s) (Year) Purpose of Study Participants and Setting Method Major Findings 

Curriculum 

Borve, H.E. & Borve, E. (2017) 

Explore teacher perception of the 
impact of the physical 
environment on the culture of play 
in the classroom 

Private kindergarten in 
Norway consisting of 73 
children and 18 lead and 
assistant teachers   

Case study using recorded 
staff interviews 

Teachers arranged the environment with expectations 
and intentions of learning in mind that often led to 
gender influenced design.   

Chapman, R. (2016) 
Explore how gender roles might 
be displayed or supported without 
the teacher being aware 

2 preschool teachers, 2 
assistant teachers, 39 
children ages 4-5 years old 
from Australia 

Interviews with teachers 
and observations of 
children  

Teachers’ planning of activities is influenced by how 
they viewed play and gender roles.   

Rodriguez, MdC., Pena, J.V., 
Fernandez, C.M., & Vinuela, M.P. 
(2006)  

Investigate gender discourse used 
by nursery school teachers 

35 teachers of children 
aged 3-6 years old in Spain 

Semi-structured 
interviews of 7 focus 
groups 

Teachers reported a gender division in areas of the 
classroom where children play that is consistent with 
their expectations that children play in traditional 
gender defined ways. 

Granger, K., Hanish, L., 
Kornienko, O. & Bradley, R.H. 
(2017) 

Explore the frequency that 
teachers facilitated gender 
conforming and gender-neutral 
activities during free play 

37 female teachers in Head 
Start classrooms  
 

Observation of teacher-
student interactions and 
group composition during 
free play 

Teachers facilitated gender-typed activities based on the 
gender make-up of the group.  Teachers facilitated 
gender-neutral activities with all girls groups more 
frequently than with all boys groups.   

Logue, M.E., & Harvey, H. (2010) 
Understand preschool teacher’s 
views and practices on pretend 
play  

98 teachers of 4-year-old 
children 

Mailed questionnaires 
Teachers reported significant differences in play for boys 
and girls.  Teachers intervened in social conflict among 
boys more often than girls. 

Lynch, M. (2015) 
Explore how teachers discuss 
gender in social media  

7 Online message boards  
7 Kindergarten teachers 

Netnography of data 
collected from social 
media and semi-
structured interviews.  

The most prevalent theme, “Dramatic play is for girls.” 
Results of interviews indicated that teachers’ views of 
gendered play are projected onto their students.  

Lynch, L. (2016) 
Analyze the content of teacher 
selected children’s literature in 
preschools in Sweden  

618 book readings of 433 
unique books across 6 
preschools in Sweden to 
children aged 3-6 years 

Teachers recorded the 
books read during group 
story times over a period 
of six months 

Significantly less girls were main characters than boys.  
Teachers’ and children’s choice of books to read was 
significantly more likely to include a male main 
character than a female.  

Sniegulska, M., & Pisula, W. (2013) 
Analyze children’s free play with a 
new toy 

189 preschool children 
aged 3-7 years old from 
private and public 
kindergartens in Poland  

Video recorded, 15-minute 
observation of child with a 
new toy 

No gender differences were found in exploration of toys. 

Tomes, R. (1995) 
Explore teacher influence on 
children’s selection of activities 
and areas 

58 children aged 3-5 years 
old and their teachers 

Observations during free 
play once a week for 6 
weeks. 

Students were drawn to areas of art, block, library and 
science if the teacher was present.  Boys preferred large 
blocks and girls preferred to play in the art area.  

Teacher Perceptions 



Erin E. HAMEL 

278 

Bosacki, S., Woods, H., & Coplan, 
R. (2015)  

Explore early childhood teacher’s 
perceptions of rough and tumble 
play based on the gender of the 
child  

22 teachers of young 
children in Canada 

Online survey collecting 
demographics and beliefs 
and attitudes related to 
hypothetical play 
scenarios 

Physical play is perceived differently by male and 
female teachers. Male teachers reported that boys who 
were more physically aggressive in their play would 
perform better academically and would be less likely to 
be excluded.  Female teachers held the opposite belief.   

Dewar, B., Servos, J., Bosacki, S., & 
Coplan, R. (2013) 

Explore early childhood teacher 
perceptions of how gender 
impacts the classroom. 

41 teachers throughout 
Canada 

Semi-structured telephone 
interviews 

Themes of professional development, critical self-
awareness, and critical thinking emerged.  Teachers saw 
reflection as a way to become aware of their own biases 
and promote more inclusive gender roles.  

Duke, T.S. & McCarthy, K.W. 
(2009) 

Lıterature review of sexuality and 
sexism in the context of early 
childhood education 

31 articles published 
between 1975-2007 

Coding system based on 
publication type, research 
design, and emergent 
themes 

Eight articles described ways that programs in early 
childhood and elementary school reinforce gender roles 
and the oppression of women noting children’s 
literature and television programming as a source for 
stereotypes.   

Erden, F. (2004)  
Explore early childhood teachers’ 
attitudes toward gender roles and 
discipline 

130 female public school 
teachers of kindergarten 
and  first grade 

Attitudes Toward Gender 
Roles Scale (AGRS) and 
Attitudes Toward 
Discipline Scale (ADS)  

Up to 40% of teachers agreed with gender traditional 
statements and researchers found a statistically 
significant relationship between teachers’ attitudes 
towards discipline and their attitudes towards gender 
roles.   

Hyland, N. (2010) 
Review of research on developing 
practices that address equity in the 
classroom  

Review of two 
approaches: culturally 
relevant teaching and 
critical pedagogy to develop 
equity  

Describe research showing 
how two pedagogies have 
been use in early 
childhood classrooms to 
address race, gender, and 
sexual orientation 

Research identifies ways to reconstruct gender 
stereotyped messages, which can be detrimental to both 
boys’ and girls’ development. Teachers role is to address 
power imbalances across race and gender through 
empowering underrepresented children.  

Hyvonen, P. (2008) 
Explore teachers’ perceptions of 
mixed-gender play activities 

14 preschool and primary 
teachers in Finland 

Teacher interviews  

Teachers noted a goal of education should be to 
overcome gender boundaries. Teachers intentionally do 
not separate gender in their classrooms and they 
question gendered behavior and ask children to reflect 
on it.  

Lundeberg, M.A. (1997) 

Investigate how preservice 
teachers’ perceptions of gender 
compare with teacher and student 
interactions 

48 preservice teachers  

Analyze data on 
participates in a 
discussion of gender bias 
in classrooms 

Majority (71%) of preservice teachers thought that 
classroom discussion was equal.  More males 
contributed comments in class discussion.  Preservice 
teachers reflected on the importance of creating equal 
experiences for both genders.   

Pellegrini et al. (2011) 
Explore the differences in 
observations and ratings of 
preschool children’s aggression   

89 preschool children  
5 five teachers 

Daily observations of 
children 
Teacher checklist to 
measure child aggression   

Trained female researchers rated children’s aggression 
the same as female teachers who had not been trained.  
Trained male researchers recorded more aggression 
compared to females.   

Sandberg, A., & Pramling-
Samuelsson, I. (2005) 

Investigate the different ways 
male and female preschool 

20 preschool teachers in 
private and public 

In-person, semi-structured 
interviews 

Preschool teachers reflected their play as children was 
gender stereotyped. Female teachers had no play 



Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood… 

279 

teachers think of play preschools in Sweden willingness and avoided playing with children as they 
thought they would disturb the play. Male teachers were 
more playful. 

Trawick-Smith, T., Wolff, J., 
Koschel, M., & Vallarelli, J. (2015) 

Examine the effects of toys on 
play, including factors that 
enhance learning and 
development 

Four classrooms of 60 
children aged 3-4 years 
old.  Parents and teachers 
nominated 9 toys for the 
research project. 
 

Observations of toy use 
recorded using hidden 
cameras and microphones.  
Toy quality was assessed 
using a play quality with 
toys (PQT) measure   

Most of the toys received the highest quality scores 
when boys were engaged with them, suggesting that 
parents and teachers need to identify toys that can 
engage boys and girls in equally beneficial ways.   

Teacher Interactions 

Adriany, V. & Warin, J. (2014) 
Explore the relationship between 
care and gender in an early 
childhood environment 

4 teachers, 1 principal, the 
school owner, and 28 
children between 2 and 6 
years old in Indonesia 

Ethnographic research 
using field notes, 
interviews with adults, 
child conversation, and 
artifacts 

Teachers care for children in stereotypical ways that 
likely perpetuate gendered behavior in children.  While 
most differences were celebrated at the school, gender 
was not outwardly addressed.   

Benozio, A. & Diesendruck, G. 
(2017) 

Assess how children are 
influenced by an authority figure 
in behaving toward others 

111 children aged 3-6 
years from 5 kindergartens 
in Israel 

Teachers randomly 
assigned to demonstrate a 
preference and child 
behavior assessed on their 
response related to 
teacher’s preference 

Children age three to four years old distribute items 
based on being fair or equal, regardless of teacher’s 
preference.  Children aged five to six, distributed items 
based on teacher preference and matched gender of 
recipients. Teacher interactions may impact how 
children respond. 

Bigler, R. S. (1995) 
Explore how the use of gender 
categorization in the classroom 
attributes to gender stereotyping 

66 children ages 6-11 in 
three classrooms 

Classroom teachers 
randomly assigned to 
exhibit three 
categorization styes: 
gender, color group, and 
control group. 

Children in a classroom where the teacher used gender 
as a categorization method increased their gender 
stereotyping.  Children in these classrooms were more 
likely to categorize occupations as appropriate for male 
or females along traditional gender stereotypes.   

Hedlin, M. & Aberg, M. (2018) 

Researchers investigate the 
conditions of stereotypes in 
teacher training and their 
influence on children in the 
classroom 

10 female and 10 male 
preschool teachers in 
Sweden 
7 teachers working with 
preservice teachers  

Semi-structured 
interviews lasting 30-60 
minutes.  
Ethnographic observations 
and interviews with 
preservice teachers.   
 

Researchers identify that teachers and staff found girls 
demonstrating headstrong and disobedient behavior as 
less permissible than boys demonstrating similar 
behaviors.  Girls acting this way were dismissed and 
perceived as “very troublesome.”  

Martin, K. (1998) 
Investigate the development of the 
gendered child in preschool 

5 preschool classrooms 
serving 5-year-old 
children   

Qualitative semi-
structured observations 
and field notes of 
classrooms 

Four teaching practices (behavior management, voice 
control, verbal and physical directions of body, and 
physical interactions between children) and one 
parenting practice (clothing selection) created gendered 
differences.   

Olsen, B. & Smeplass, E. (2016) Researchers report and reflect on 671 teachers and staff Questionnaire of 13 Teachers’ response to dilemmas were nearly identical, 



Erin E. HAMEL 

280 

gendered attitudes of kindergarten 
staff members 

members at 80 
Kindergartens from 2 
Danish municipalities  

pedagogical dilemmas, 2 
specific to gender   
 

regardless of the teacher’s own gender.  Staff, regardless 
of gender, treated boys and girls differently.    

Owens, S., Smothers, B.C., & Love, 
F.E. (2003) 

Examine gender bias in schools 
related to access of education and 
gender inequalities 

Systematic review of 
literature 

Three areas of focus: 
history of women and 
education, schools 
encouragement of gender 
inequality, and solutions 
for equity in schools 

Gender bias or sexism in the classroom is subtle and 
often unconscious.  Differences in how boys and girls are 
treated results in a learned pattern of how to behave 
early on in life.   

Şahin-Sak, İ. T., Sak, R., & Tezel-
Şahin, F. (2018) 

Investigate the perceptions of 
preschool teachers related to 
behavior management 

310 preschool teachers in 
Turkey  

Questionnaire  
Female preschool teachers were more likely to use 
techniques for behavior management involving 
listening, relationship building, and comforting children. 

Vitiello, V.E., Booren, L.M., 
Downer, J.T., & Williford, A.P. 
(2012) 

Investigate the source of 
variability in child engagement 
between child factors and 
classroom settings 

342 preschool aged 
children (34 – 63 months) 
from 100 classrooms in 40 
urban schools  
84 female teachers   

Classroom observation of 
activity settings and 
inCLASS observational 
system 

Child gender was not significantly associated with 
children’s negative or positive engagement with their 
teachers.  

Social Standing 

Al-Thani, T., & Semmar, Y. (2017) 
Assess prosocial behaviors of 
preschool children and differences 
in teacher perceptions of gender 

472 children and 22 teachers 
from 10 schools in Doha, 
Qatar  

 

Teachers completed the 
Prosocial Behaviors of 
Children-Teachers’ 
Perceptions instrument  

Teachers perceived that boys displayed less prosocial 
behaviors than their female peers.  Significant 
differences in gender occurred in teacher preferred 
behavior, peer preferred behavior, and school 
adjustment.  

Sullivan, J., Moss-Racusin, C., 
Lopez, M., & Williams, K. (2018) 

Explore the impact of gender 
stereotyping on young children 

Study 1: 635 adults  
Study 2: 697 adults  
All recruited from Amazon 
Mechanical Turk. 

Study 1: Online task to 
rate typical or desirable 
characteristics for 3 year 
old by gender   
Study 2:  Online task of 
gender conforming and 
non-conforming vignettes 
of preschool applications   

Study 1: Researchers developed a list of traits for each 
gender.   
Study 2: Stereotype conforming boys and girls were 
more likeable than their non-conforming peers.  Girls 
overall were more likeable than boys.    

Gender Identity 

Burdelski, M. & Mitsuhashi, K. 
(2010) 

Explore how teachers and children 
use the word kawaii (cute, 
loveable) and how it impacts 
social interactions in the classroom 

Private day care classroom 
with 14 children and their 
female teachers in Japan 

Recorded observations of 
the classroom taken 1-2 
times a month over one 
year 

Children learn the meaning of kawaii through social 
interactions.  Teachers use kawaii as an indication of 
gender.  Teachers assessed girls using the term.  Female 
children used kawaii more when referring to themselves 
and female peers.   



Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood… 

281 

Conclusion and Discussion 

Several notable ideas emerged from this review of early childhood literature related to gender. First, 
teachers’ perceptions of gender are influenced by their own gender and prior experiences (Borve & Borve, 
2017; Bosacki et al., 2015; Pellegrini et al., 2011; Sandberg & Pramling-Samuelsson, 2005). Second, to some 
degree interactions in the classroom contain bias and stereotypes and implicit gender bias may be present 
in how activities are designed and what materials are selected for use in the classroom (Borve & Borve, 
2017; Rodriguez et al., 2006; Trawick-Smith et al., 2015). Third, children receive gendered messages from 
early childhood teachers which may impact their own view of themselves (Adriany & Warin, 2014; Burelski 
& Mitsuhashi, 2010; Granger et al., 2017; Olsen & Smeplass, 2016). Next, these ideas are interpreted while 
considering implications for science teaching and learning in the early years. 

When applying a lens of science learning to findings in the Curriculum category, it is important to 
remember that an intense early interest in science for girls is related to a higher self-concept in science later 
in childhood (Leibham et al., 2013). Young girls may self-select play that is more nurturing in nature but 
the teacher’s planning could also be contributing to traditional gender play in the classroom. Early 
childhood teachers are influential in fostering science interest through their presence and planning of 
activities that increase opportunities for science play for children (Leibham et al., 2013; Tomes, 1995). 
Research shows that early childhood teachers do not feel confident teaching science and that they would 
prefer to play with children in other areas of the classroom instead (Gerde et al., 2018; Kallary & Psillos, 
2001).  As a consequence, the science area of the classroom may not be selected for play as often and science 
content may be addressed less frequently than other content areas which could be especially detrimental 
to igniting and fostering young girls’ early interests in science. Further, not all early childhood programs 
are equally focused on science. Nature-based preschool programs have a special focus on environmental 
education and spend large amounts of time in the outdoors. Such programs may provide more frequent 
opportunities for science learning yet little is known about the role of gender in such experiences. It is 
possible that nature provides an optimal backdrop for equal science learning for both genders. Children 
enrolled at nature preschools may accrue less gender bias and gendered beliefs about themselves and their 
science abilities when immersed in a natural outdoor learning environment as compared to a traditional 
early childhood setting that contains gendered play areas and gendered toys. More research on 
environment and curriculum is needed to answer these questions.  

In early childhood education, research shows that interactions between children and teachers have 
an important role in predicting child outcomes (Early et al., 2007). The results from this review indicate 
that teachers interact differently with children based on their gender (Granger et al., 2017; Olsen & 
Smeplass, 2016). The impact of differential treatment on science learning is still largely unknown and 
represents a gap in the literature for future researchers to investigate. Interactions during science activities 
and exploration in the early childhood classroom could be a key element to understanding later differences 
in science achievement and interest between boys and girls. And although the focus of the review is 
constrained to perceptions of early childhood teachers, it is worth noting how interactions with other 
authority figures, such as parents, could be contributing to the divide. In a study of interactions between 
parents and their children at a science museum, parents used more explanatory conversations with their 
sons than with their daughters (Crowley et al., 2001). In fact, conversations with sons were three times 
more likely to include explanations and this held true at all ages (1-8 years) even though children who 
heard explanations had rarely asked questions (Crowley et al., 2001). It is not a far leap to suggest that 
similar differences in interactions could be happening in early childhood classrooms given the research 
presented in the Teacher Interactions category showing that child gender impacts teacher’s responses and 
teaching practices (Adriany & Warin, 2014; Martin, 1998; Olsen & Smeplass, 2016). Fortunately, a recent 
study found that of 755 questions asked during preschool science lessons, no significant differences were 
found regarding gender of the child recipient (Hamel et al., 2021). Further, Granger and colleagues (2017) 
found that gender-neutral activities were implemented more frequently with groups of girls than boys. 
While it is promising that teachers are engaging with young girls in gender-neutral activities, it stops short 



Erin E. HAMEL 

282 

of crossing into more stereotypically masculine activities, which are often associated with science. 
Providing stereotypically masculine activities to all genders is an opportunity to counter stereotypical 
gender activities by providing a variety of experiences to all children regardless of gender. 

Findings from the social standing category are relevant to the topic of gender and science because 
girls (and boys, for that matter) may be more inclined to conform to gender stereotypes to achieve approval 
or be liked. Masculinity is often linked to traits of objectivity, lack of emotion, and rationality, which 
frequently are associated with the subject of science (Brotman & Moore, 2008). Exhibiting feminine 
nurturing behaviors such as building relationships, creativity, and showing emotion can be viewed as 
incompatible to science (Brotman & Moore, 2008). Unfortunately, these associations may lead children to 
conform for the sake of acceptance or contradict gender stereotypes with the risk of being rejected. Of 
course, it is important to note that real differences may exist in social behaviors of boys and girls. Some 
researchers have found that relational aggression is more common in girls in early childhood and boys 
show more physical aggression than girls, although not significantly (Ostrov et al., 2004). While difference 
between social behaviors may exist, teachers should encourage all students to engage in science activities 
and content. Attempts have been made to contradict associations of masculinity and science including a 
program coined “Creative Expression in Science” (Meyer, 1998) aimed at elementary science teachers. Early 
childhood teachers could also benefit from professional development and training that emphasizes 
acceptance and encouragement of all students to engage in science topics.  

Lastly, language is powerful and has the ability to influence how children view themselves. The use 
of the Japanese word kawaii is an example of explicit labeling that impacts how children identify themselves 
by gender (Burdelski & Mitsuhashi, 2010). Explicit labeling is an important facet of DIT which provides an 
explanation of core processes responsible for contributing to young children’s understandings of bias and 
stereotype, including gender. DIT posits that four factors may influence the formation of gender bias or 
stereotypes in young children, one of which is explicit labeling and use (Bigler & Liben, 2006). If children, 
especially young girls, are forming (mis)understandings of themselves and their science abilities during 
preschool, it is important to understand not only where and from whom these messages originate (parents, 
teachers, and society), but how children receive and internalize them as part of their identity. 

These findings also have implications for early childhood teacher preparation programs who can 
apply and share the results within required coursework on gender equity and science pedagogy. Early 
childhood teachers’ awareness and understandings of their own biases and perceptions can help them to 
reflect on their pedagogical choices and the subsequent impact on children’s learning opportunities in the 
classroom. Policymakers and administrators should also consider offering professional development for 
early childhood teachers on promoting gender equity by eliminating gender bias and stereotypes as a way 
to enhance teacher practices. It should be emphasized here that this review included research conducted 
in international settings which requires special consideration of socio-cultural influences of gender 
development. Therefore, the cultural norms and context of each unique early care program should be 
thoughtfully considered.  

Limitations 

This review explored how teacher perceptions related to gender influenced teachers’ practices. 
Limitations of the review include the small number of articles collected. Although efforts were made to 
encompass all relevant articles in the early childhood literature, it is possible that some studies were missed. 
A wider search to include other areas of male-dominated domains and careers, such as math, might also 
provide additional insights but was beyond the scope of this review. In addition, the body of literature 
analyzed relied heavily on self-report measures such as questionnaires, surveys, and semi-structured 
interviews of teachers to identify their perceptions and gender bias. For these types of measures, social 
desirability may play a role in how teachers reported their perceptions of gender and lead to answers that 
were not true depictions of actual perceptions. One form of bias termed implicit bias, is based on the notion 
that unconscious beliefs or processes can impact our actions. For teachers, this is particularly important 
because implicit biases may affect teachers’ “understanding, actions, and decisions in an unconscious 



Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood… 

283 

manner” (Staats, 2016). A well-intentioned teacher may unconsciously interpret a situation or behavior in 
a way that is biased therefore observations of children and teachers during science activities may provide 
a different perspective.  

Future Directions 

The results of this review highlight future avenues for research. First, it would be useful to use 
measures that can reveal implicit gender bias. The desire to respond in a more socially acceptable manner 
may play a role in how teachers report their perceptions and feelings about gender. A second direction for 
future research would be an increase in observational data collection to observe teacher and child behavior 
in natural classroom settings. Only one-third (34%) of the articles in the review included classroom 
observations, with others focusing instead on surveys and interviews. Additionally, preschool aged 
children are capable of answering questions related to their perceptions, interests, and experiences and 
could provide insight into preschool science activities.  

Looking forward, it is also important to recognize that separately, the fields of science and early 
childhood education have made valuable contributions to understanding this topic. Gender gaps in science 
fields and occupations are widely acknowledged and continue to be investigated. At the same time, early 
childhood researchers are dedicated to understanding the teaching and learning of science in early 
childhood (Silby & Watts, 2017). However, exploring science teaching and learning in early childhood 
specifically for gender bias or differential treatment by gender is lacking. Expanding the field to include 
more investigations at the early childhood level could provide a more complete understanding of the issue 
of gender inequality in science fields prior to formal schooling. It also has the potential to provide valuable 
information about the teacher’s role in counteracting or contributing to gender differences in science with 
an overarching goal of increasing the participation of women in the science field. Further investigation into 
this topic may prove mutually beneficial for both the fields of science and early childhood education. An 
area of importance for further research is examining how gender bias in the classroom impacts all children, 
including gender-nonconforming children. Early childhood classrooms should provide equitable science 
learning opportunities for all children.  

Declarations 

Competing interests: The authors declare that they have no competing interests 

Funding: No funding was used for this study. 

References 

Adriany, V., & Warin, J. (2014). Preschool teachers’ approaches to care and gender differences within a child-centered pedagogy: 
Findings from an Indonesian kindergarten. International Journal of Early Years Education, 22(3), 315-328. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760.2014.951601  

Alexander, J. M., Johnson, K. E., & Kelley, K. (2012). Longitudinal analysis of the relations between opportunities to learn about science 
and the development of interests related to science. Science Education, 96(5), 763–786. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21018 

Al-Thani, T., & Semmar, Y. (2017). An exploratory study of teachers’ perceptions of prosocial behaviors in preschool children. 
International Journal of Educational Development, 53, 145-150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijedudev.2017.01.005  

Beilock, S.L. Gunderson, E.A., Ramirez, G., & Levine, S.C. (2010). Female teachers’ math anxiety affects girls’ math achievement. 
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(5), 1860. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0910967107  

Benozio, A., & Diesendruck, G. (2017). Parochial compliance: Young children’s biased consideration of authorities’ preferences 
regarding intergroup interactions. Child Development, 88(5), 1527-1535. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12654  

Bian L., Leslie, S., & Cimpian, A. (2017). Gender stereotypes about intellectual ability emerge early and influence children’s interests. 
Science, 335(6323), 389-391. https://doi.org/10.1126/scienceaah6524  

Bigler, R.S. (1995). The role of classification skill in moderating environmental influences on children’s gender stereotyping: A study 
of the functional use of gender in the classroom. Child Development, 66(4) 1072-1087. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
8624.1995.tb00923.x 

Bigler, R.S. & Liben, L.S. (2006). A developmental intergroup theory of social stereotypes and prejudice. Advances in Child Development 

https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760.2014.951601
https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijedudev.2017.01.005
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0910967107
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12654
https://doi.org/10.1126/scienceaah6524
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00923.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00923.x


Erin E. HAMEL 

284 

and Behavior, 34, 38-89. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2407(06)80004-2  

Bigler, R.S. & Liben, L.S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children's social stereotyping and 
prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162-166. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x 

Borve, H.E., & Borve, E. (2017). Rooms with gender: Physical environment and play culture in kindergarten. Early Child Development 
and Care, 187(5-6), 1069-1081. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1223072  

Bosacki, S., Woods, H., & Coplan, R. (2015). Canadian female and male early childhood educators’ perceptions of child aggression 
and rough-and-tumble play. Early Child Development and Care, 185(7), 1334-1147. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2014.980408  

Brotman, J.S., & Moore, F.M. (2008). Girls and science: A review of four themes in the science education literature. Journal of Research 
in Science Teaching, 45(9), 971-1002. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea20241  

Burdelski, M., & Mitsuhashi, K. (2010). “She thinks you’re kawaii”: Socializing affect, gender, and relationships in a Japanese 
preschool. Language in Society, 39(1), 65-93. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404509990650  

Cassidy, D.J., Hestenes, L.L., Hansen, J.K., Hegde, A., Shim, J., & Hestenes, S. (2005). Revisiting the two faces of child care quality: 
Structure and process. Early Education & Development, 16(4), 505-520. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1604_10  

Catsambis, S. (1995). Gender, race, ethnicity, and science education in the middle grades. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 32(3), 
243-257. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.3660320305  

Chafel, J. (2003). Socially constructing concepts of self and other through play. International Journal of Early Years Education, 11(3), 213-
222. https://doi.org/10.1080/0966976032000147334  

Chapman, R. (2016). A case study of gendered play in preschools: How early childhood educators’ perceptions of gender influence 
children’s play. Early Child Development and Care, 186(8), 1271-1284. https://doi.org/10.1080/0300430.2015.1089435  

Crowley, K., Callanan, M., Tenenbaum, H.R. & Allen, E. (2001). Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared 
scientific thinking. Psychological Science, 12(3), 258-261. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00347  

Curran, F. C. & Kellogg, A. T. (2016). Understanding science achievement gaps by race/ethnicity and gender in kindergarten and first 
grade. Educational Researcher, 45(5), 273-282. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X16656611  

Davies, B. (2003). Frogs and snails and feminist tales: Preschool children and gender. Hampton Press. 

Dewar, B.A., Servos, J.E., Bosacki, S.L., & Coplan, R. (2013). Early childhood educators’ reflections on teaching practices: The role of 
gender and culture. Reflective Practice, 14(3), 381-391. https://doi.org/10.1080/14623943.2013.767234  

Duke, T.S. & McCarthy, K.W. (2009). Homophobia, sexism, and early childhood education: A review of the literature. Journal of Early 
Childhood Teacher Education, 30(4), 385-403. https://doi.org/10.1080/10901020903320320  

Early, D.M., Maxwell, K.L., Burchinal, M., Alva, S., Bender, R.H., Bryant, D., Cai, K., Clifford, R.M., Ebanks, C., Griffin, J.A., Henry, 
G.T., Howes, C., Iriondo-Perez, J., Jeon, H.J., Mashburn, A.J., Peisner-Feinberg, E., Pianta, R.C., Vandergrift, N., & Zill, N. 
(2007). Teachers’ education, classroom quality, and young children’s academic skills: Results from seven studies of preschool 
programs. Child Development, 78(2), 558-580. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01014.x  

Eccles, J.S. (2007). Where are all the women? Gender differences in participation in physical science and engineering. In S.J. Ceci & 
W.M.Williams (Eds.), Why aren’t more women in science?: Top researchers debate the evidence (pp. 199-210). American 
Psychological Association.  

Eccles, J.S., Arberton, A., Buchanan, C.M., Janis, J., Flanagan, C., Harold, R., … Reuman, D. (1993). School and family effects on the 
ontogeny of children’s interests, self-perceptions, and activity choices. In J. E. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: 
Developmental perspectives on motivation (145-208). University of Nebraska Press.   

Erden, F. (2004). Early childhood teachers’ attitudes toward gender roles and towards discipline. Hacettepe University Journal of 
Education, 27, 83-90.  

Eshach, H., & Fried, M.N. (2005). Should science be taught in early childhood? Journal of Science Education and Technology, 14(3), 315-
336. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-005-7198-9  

Frankel, J.R., Wallen, N.E., & Hyum, H.H. (2016). How to design and evaluate research in education. McGraw-Hill.  

Gelman, R., & Brenneman, K. (2012). Moving young “scientists-in-waiting” onto science learning pathways: Focus on observation. In 
J. Shrager & S. Carver (Eds.), The journey from child to scientist: Integrating cognitive development and the education sciences (pp. 
155–169). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13617-008  

Gerde, H. K., Pierce, S. J., Kyungsook, L., & Van Egeren, L. A. (2018). Early childhood educators’ self-efficacy in science, math, and 
literacy instruction and science practice in the classroom. Early Education and Development, 29(1), 70-90. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2017.1360127  

Granger, K., Hanish, L., Kornienko, O., & Bradley, R. (2017). Preschool teachers’ facilitation of gender-typed and gender-neutral 
activities during free play. Sex Roles, 76(7-8), 498-510. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0675-1  

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2407(06)80004-2
https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-8721.2007.00496.x
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1223072
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2014.980408
https://doi.org/10.1002/tea20241
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404509990650
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1604_10
https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.3660320305
https://doi.org/10.1080/0966976032000147334
https://doi.org/10.1080/0300430.2015.1089435
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00347
https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X16656611
https://doi.org/10.1080/14623943.2013.767234
https://doi.org/10.1080/10901020903320320
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01014.x
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-005-7198-9
https://doi.org/10.1037/13617-008
https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2017.1360127
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0675-1


Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood… 

285 

Greenfield, T.A. (1996) Gender, ethnicity, science achievement, and attitudes. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 33(8), 901-933.  

Greenfield, D.B., Jirout, J., Dominguez, X., Greenberg, A., Maier, M., & Fuccillo, J. (2009). Science in the preschool classroom: A 
classroom programmatic research agenda to improve science readiness. Early Education and Development, 20(2), 238-264. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/10409280802595441  

Gunderson, E., Ramirez, G., Levine, S., & Beilock, S. (2012). The role of parents and teachers in the development of gender-related 
math attitudes. Sex Roles, 66(3), 153-166. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2  

Hamel, E., Joo, Y., Hong, S. Y., & Burton, A. (2021). Teacher questioning practices in early childhood science activities. Early Childhood 
Education Journal, 49(3), 375-384.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-020-01075-z  

Haworth, C.M.A., Dale, P.S., & Plomin, R. (2009). Sex differences and science: the etiology of science excellence. Journal of Child 
Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 50(9), 1113-1120. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02087.x  

Hedlin, M., & Aberg, M. (2018). Fussy girls and chattering women – the construct and subordination of femininity in preschool teacher 
training. Early Child Development and Care, 188(2), 220-230. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1211120  

Hyland, N.E. (2010). Social justice in early childhood classrooms: What the research tells us. Young Children, 65(1), 82-87.  

Hyvonen, P. (2008) Teachers’ perceptions of boys’ and girls’ shared activities in the school context: Towards a theory of collaborative 
play. Teachers & Teaching, 14(5/6), 391-409. https://doi.org/10.1080/13540600802571312  

Jones, M.G., Howe, A., & Rua, M. J. (2000). Gender differences in students’ experiences, interests, and attitudes toward science and 
scientists. Science Education, 84(2), 180-192. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-237X(200003)84:2<180::AID-SCE3>3.0.CO;2-X  

Kallery, M., & Psillos, D. (2001). Pre-school teachers’ content knowledge in science: Their understanding of elementary science 
concepts and of issues raised by children’s. International Journal of Early Years Education, 9(3), 165-179. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760120086929  

Leibham, M.B., Alexander, J.M. & Johnson, K.E. (2013). Science interests in preschool boys and girls: Relations to later self-concept 
and science achievement. Science Education, 97(4), 574-593. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21066  

Logue, M.E., & Harvey, H. (2010). Preschool teachers’ views of active play. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 24(1), 32-49. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/02568540903439375  

Lundeberg, M.A. (1997). You guys are overreacting: Teaching prospective teachers about subtle gender bias. Journal of Teacher 
Education, 48(1), 55-61.  

Lynch, L. (2015). Guys and dolls: A qualitative study of teachers’ view of gendered play in kindergarten. Early Child Development and 
Care, 185(5), 679-693. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2014.950260  

Lynch, L. (2016). Where are all the pippis?: The under-representation of female main and title characters in children’s literature in the 
Swedish preschool. Sex Roles, 75(9-10), 422-433. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0637-7  

Martin, K.A. (1998). Becoming a gendered body: Practices of preschools. American Sociological Review, 63(4), 494-511. 
https://doi.org/10.2307/2657264  

Meyer, K. (1998). Reflections on being female in school science. Toward a praxis of teaching science. Journal of Research in Science 
Teaching, 35(4), 463-471. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2736(199804)35:4<463::AID-TEA16>3.0.CO;2-A  

Morgan, P.L., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M.M., & Maczuga, S. (2016). Science achievement gaps begin very early, persist, and are largely 
explained by modifiable factors. Educational Researcher, 45(1), 18-35. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X16633182  

Morris, B. J., Croker, S., Masnick, A., & Zimmerman, C. (2012). The emergence of scientific reasoning. In H. Kloos, B. Morris, & J. 
Amaral (Eds.), Current Topics in Children’s Learning and Cognition (pp. 61–82). Intech Open. https://doi.org/10.5772/53885  

Olsen, B., & Smeplass, E. (2016). Gender and distribution of educational values among the staff in kindergartens. Early Child 
Development & Care, 188(8), 1202-1218. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1261125  

Ostrov, J.M., Woods, K.E., Jansen, E.A., Casas, J.F., & Crick, N.R. (2004). An observational study of delivered and received aggression, 
gender, and social-psychological adjustment in preschool: “This white crayon doesn’t work…”. Early Childhood Research 
Quarterly, 19(2), 335-371.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.04.009  

Owens, S.L., Smothers, B.C., & Love, F.E. (2003). Are girls victims of gender bias in our nation’s schools?. Journal of Instructional 
Psychology, 30(2), 131-137.  

Park, M.-H., Dimitrov, D. M., Patterson, L. G., & Park, D.-Y. (2017). Early childhood teachers’ beliefs about readiness for teaching 
science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 15(3), 275-291. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X15614040  

Pellegrini, A.D., Bohn-Gettler, C.M., Dupuis, D., Hickey, M., Roseth, C., & Solberg, D. (2011). An empirical examination of sex 
differences in scoring preschool children’s aggression. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(2), 232-238. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.003  

https://doi.org/10.1080/10409280802595441
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-020-01075-z
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02087.x
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1211120
https://doi.org/10.1080/13540600802571312
https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-237X(200003)84:2%3c180::AID-SCE3%3e3.0.CO;2-X
https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760120086929
https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21066
https://doi.org/10.1080/02568540903439375
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2014.950260
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0637-7
https://doi.org/10.2307/2657264
https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2736(199804)35:4%3c463::AID-TEA16%3e3.0.CO;2-A
https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X16633182
https://doi.org/10.5772/53885
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1261125
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.04.009
https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X15614040
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.003


Erin E. HAMEL 

286 

Riegle-Crumb, C., King, B., Grodsky, E., & Muller, C. (2012). The more things change, the more they stay the same? Prior achievement 
fails to explain gender inequality in entry into STEM college majors overtime. American Educational Research Journal, 49(6), 
1048-1073. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831211435229  

Robinson-Cimpian, J., Lubienski, S.T., Ganley, C.M., & Copur-Gencturk, Y. (2014). Teachers’ percpetions of students’ mathematics 
proficiency may exacerbate early gender gaps in achievement. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1262-1281. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035073  

Rodriguez, M.d.C., Pena, J.V., Fernandez, C.M., & Vinuela, M.P. (2006). Gender discourse about an ethic of care: Nursery 
schoolteachers’ perspectives. Gender & Education, 18(2), 183-197. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540250500380547  

Şahin-Sak, İ. T., Sak, R., & Tezel-Şahin, F. (2018). Preschool teachers’ views about classroom management models. Early Years, 38(1), 
35-52. https://doi.org/10.1080/09575146.2016.1242118  

Saldaña, J. (2015). Thinking qualitatively: Methods of mind. SAGE. 

Sandberg, A., & Pramling-Samuelsson, I. (2005). An interview study of gender differences in preschool teachers’ attitudes toward 
children’s play. Early Childhood Education Journal, 32(5), 297-305. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-005-440-x  

Silby, A., & Watts, M. (2017). Early years science education: A contemporary look. [Editorial]. Early Child Development and Care, 187(2), 
179-180. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1237567  

Sniegulska, M., & Pisula, W. (2013). Object exploration in 3- to 7- year old children. Psychological Reports, 113(2), 528-539. 
https://doi.org/10.2466/10.11.PR0.113x19z1  

Staats, C. (2016). Understanding implicit bias: What educators should know. American Educator, 39(4), 29-33. 
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1086492.pdf  

Sullivan, J., Moss-Racusin, C., Lopez, M., & Williams, K. (2018). Backlash against gender stereotype-violating preschool children. 
PLOSONE, 13(4), 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0195503  

Tenenbaum, H.R., & Leaper, C. (2003). Parent-child conversations about science. Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 34-47. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.34  

Tomes, R.E. (1995). Teacher presence and child gender influences on children’s activity preferences in preschool. Child Study Journal, 
25(2), 123-141.  

Trawick-Smith, J., Wolff, J., Koschel, M., & Vallarelli, J. (2015). Effects of toys on the play quality of preschool children: Influence of 
gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. Early Childhood Education Journal, 43(4), 249-256. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-
014-0644-7  

Tu, T. (2006). Preschool science environment: What is available in a preschool classroom? Early Childhood Education Journal, 33(4), 245-
251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-005-0049-8  

Vitiello, V.E., Booren, L.M., Downer, J.T., & Williford, A.P. (2012). Variation in children’s classroom engagement throughout a day in 
preschool: Relations to classroom and child factors. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(2), 210-220. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.005  

Wang, X. (2013). Why students choose STEM majors: Motivation, high school learning, and postsecondary context of support. 
American Educational Research Journal, 50(5), 1081-1121.  https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831213488622  

Wood, W., & Eagley, A.H. (2015). Two traditions of research on gender identity. Sex Roles, 73, 461-473. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-
015-0480-2 

https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831211435229
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035073
https://doi.org/10.1080/09540250500380547
https://doi.org/10.1080/09575146.2016.1242118
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-005-440-x
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1237567
https://doi.org/10.2466/10.11.PR0.113x19z1
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1086492.pdf
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0195503
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.34
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-014-0644-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-014-0644-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-005-0049-8
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.005
https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831213488622
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-015-0480-2
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-015-0480-2

	Science starts early: A literature review examining the influence of early childhood teachers’ perceptions of gender on teaching practices
	Erin E. Hamel