Journal of Childhood, Education & Society 
Volume 3, Issue 1, 2022, 48-59                                                                                                                          ISSN: 2717-638X 
DOI: 10.37291/2717638X.202231158 Research Article 

 

 
©2022 Journal of Childhood, Education & Society. This is an open access article under the CC BY- NC- ND license. 

 

 

Children’s views on social distancing and playing on an 
adventure playground 

Pete King1,  Chris Gregory2 

 
 

Abstract: Adventure playgrounds have provided an important play environment for 
children in the United Kingdom (UK) since the 1940s.  Twenty-five children ages from 4 
to 13 were asked how they would play if social distancing was introduced on their 
adventure playground. Using Piagetian classification as a framework, responses from 
children in the pre-operational stage were compliant, whilst in the operational stage, 
children were compliant but explained how they would adapt their play. For the formal 
operational stage, the responses were confrontational. The importance of obtaining 
children’s views challenges the original ‘blanket’ policy guidance within the UK on social 
distancing for all children in outdoor environments including an adventure playground 
and considering how children play when with their peers is more social play. 

 
Article History 
Received: 10 December 2021  
Accepted: 04 January 2022 
 
Keywords 
Social distancing; Adventure 
playgrounds; COVID-19, 
Lockdown, Play 

Introduction 

Adventure playgrounds are play spaces for children that emerged after the second world war 
(Newstead, 2019) The idea of the adventure playground, or as it was originally called junk playground, 
was from architect Carl Theodor Sørensen (1893-1979) where the first junk playground opened in Denmark 
in 1943 at Emdrup Weg near Copenhagen (Bengtsson, 1972).  The idea of junk playgrounds was brou1ht 
from Denmark to the UK by Lady Allen of Hurtwood (Hurtwood, 1968), where the name changed to 
adventure playgrounds.  

In 1940 Lady Allen of Hurtwood, supported by the National Playing Fields Association (NPFA), 
now called Fields in Trust (FiT) supported the development of adventure playgrounds in the UK.  The first 
adventure playground recorded was in Mordon (Evening News, 1947) followed by a pilot project being set 
up in 1948 in Camberwell, London (Kovlosky, 2008; Sutherland, 2014).  From these first adventure 
playgrounds, more permanent adventure playgrounds developed in the 1950s in Crawley, Grimsby, and 
Liverpool (Cranwell, 2003), and this spread to Bristol, Manchester, Birmingham, and other areas in the 
Midlands, Sheffield, Newcastle, Cardiff, and Edinburgh (Chilton, 2018).  However, Chilton’s (2003) account 
of adventure playgrounds in the last 40 years indicates a decrease in the numbers across the UK due to 
factors such as health and safety requirements and adult-related agendas such as the need for increased 
childcare, and educational attainment. 

Play has an important role in children’s health and development where Whitebread et. al. (2012) 
identifies five types of play:  physical play; play with objects; symbolic play; pretence/socio-dramatic play 
and games with rules.  These five types of play reflect the Piaget (1962) classification of play of Practice 
Play (linked to the sensorimotor stage); Symbolic Play (linked to the pre-operational and operational stage) 
and Games with Rules (linked to the concrete operational stage).  The wide age range and developmental 
stages of the children have to be considered in the analysis of the data.  Piagetian (1962) theory outlines the 
different cognitive developmental stages as a stage-like process that still has its critics (Feldman, 2004), 
particularly as initial observations undertaken by Piaget were on their children.   However, there are 

_____________ 

1 Swansea University, Department for Education and Childhood Studies, Swansea, UK, e-mail: p.f.king@swansea.ac.uk, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0273-8191  
2 Independent Researcher, Isle of Man, UK, e-mail: chrisgregoryplay@outlook.com, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3156-3204  

https://doi.org/10.37291/2717638X.202231158
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
mailto:p.f.king@swansea.ac.uk
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0273-8191
mailto:chrisgregoryplay@outlook.com
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3156-3204


Children’s views on social distancing and playing… 

49 

general characteristics of children’s play that do reflect the age and stage of development (Garner & Bergen, 
2006), and this provided a framework for analysis considering the potential level and quality of the 
response between children aged 4 years and over 11 years of age.   

The adventure playground provides a unique space for children across a wide age range between 5 
and 15 years (King, 2021a) to be able to engage in all the five types of play identified by Whitebread and 
colleagues (2012).  This would include climbing and running (physical play), using tools such as hammer 
and nails to make dens (play with objects), acting out roles (pretence/socio-dramatic play), use the 
resources in any way they want (symbolic play) or play sports (games with rules).  Chilton (2018) provides 
an overview on the types of play children engage in on an adventure playground and how this type of 
setting can support children’s development. 

The last estimated numbers of adventure playgrounds in England was around 180 (Play England 
(PE), 2011), although this number has decreased with recent closures of adventure playground.  There are 
two adventure playgrounds in Wales and one in Scotland. Adventure playgrounds provide a play 
environment for children and young people, from as young as 4 and up to 17 years.  Adventure 
playgrounds provide a unique play space for such a wide age range for children and young people to be 
in the same environment.  As well as the UK, there are still adventure playgrounds, for example, in the 
United States (Almon & Keeler, 2018) and in Japan (Kinoshita, & Woolley, 2015) that provide a space for 
children and young people to play often in built-up urban environments.  Shier (1984) provides a revised 
description of a typical adventure playground in the UK as: 

…an area fenced off and set aside for children. Within its boundary’s children can play freely,  in their own way, 
in their own time. But what is special about an Adventure Playground is  that here (and increasingly in 
contemporary urban society, only here) children can build and shape the environment according to their own creative 
vision (p. 3). 

Adventure playgrounds provide a unique environment where children have ownership of the space 
(PE, 2017) where they are free to come and go, within what is termed in the UK as an ‘open access policy’.   
Adventure playgrounds provide a wide range of play opportunities such as den building using tools such 
as hammers, large structures for children to climb and jump from, a fire pit to both keep warm and cook, 
small and large movable objects, now often categorized as loose parts (Nicholson, 1971) which may include 
tyres and ropes, or just the open space to play traditional games in the UK such as hide ‘n’ seek or tag 
(chase).  Some adventure playgrounds may also have access to indoor space where more art and craft-
based opportunities of play may occur. 

The adventure playground thus serves a wide age range of children and young people to engage 
and direct a variety of play opportunities.  Although adventure playgrounds are not set up for educational 
attainment (Chilton, 2003), the diverse nature of this type of play provision will support children’s 
development at different stages.  For example, for children aged 4 and 5 years the use of objects becomes 
more functional (Garner & Bergen, 2006) with an increase in fine and gross motor skills (Johnson, 2006) and 
can be easily observed in den making where wooden structures are built using hammer and nails.  
Construction and outdoor play have benefits for cognitive learning in areas of math and science (Trawick-
Smith et al., 2017).  For older children, the play often becomes more complete concerning physical play and 
social play (Lee Manning, 2006).  Although Whitebread and colleagues (2012) refer to socio-dramatic play 
as one of their five types of play, Hughes’s (2002) taxonomy of 16 play types defines social play where 
“experiences in which the rules and criteria for social engagement and interaction can be revealed, explored 
and amended” (p. 33).  This can be observed with children and young people climbing higher structures 
and leaping and somersaulting onto large crash mats, or where the fire pit is being prepared, lit and food 
being cooked which becomes a very socially-based play opportunity.  Ward (1961) considered the 
adventure playground as: 

…a free-society in miniature, with the same tensions and ever-changing harmonies, the same diversity and 
spontaneity, the same unformed growth of co-operation and release of individual qualities and communal sense, 
which lie dormant in a society devoted to competition and acquisitiveness (p.201). 



Pete KING & Chris GREGORY 

50 

Adventure playgrounds often run after school (from 3 pm onwards in the UK) Monday to Friday, 
during the weekends, and during the day in the school holidays (King, 2021a).  It is not uncommon for 
adventure playgrounds during the evening to have up to 60 children use the provision or during the 
holidays over 200, although not all at the same time concerning the open access policy of children being 
able to come and go as they please.  Open access refers to children and young people free to enter and leave 
the adventure playground of their own volition (Welsh Government (WG), 2014).   In addition to providing 
a space to play, adventure playgrounds have also developed other provisional services by acting as a food 
bank (King, 2021b) and providing a base for children and young people engaged in the alternative 
curriculum (King, 2020).  The alternative curriculum is where school-aged children are not following the 
national educational curriculum and may spend part of their education outside of the school. 

In March 2020, the UK went into lockdown as a response to the increasing spread of the SARS-CoV-
2 (COVID-19) which occurs “through contact (via larger droplets and aerosols), and longer-range 
transmission via aerosols, especially in conditions where ventilation is poor” (Alwan et. al., 2020, p.1).  This 
resulted in all child-related provisions (schools, parks, adventure playgrounds, etc.) being faced with 
instant closure, and all play-related activities ceased and staff, not all, but most were furloughed (King, 
2021a).  Where adventure playgrounds were able to run some kind of provision, this continued to be 
community-based by increasing or developing food bank services or providing a more mobile service 
delivering resources to children’s houses (King, 2020). 

The March 2020 lockdown in the United Kingdom lasted for 3 months, and when adventure 
playgrounds re-opened in July 2020 restrictions about hygiene and social distancing were still in place (UK 
Governemnt, 2020a; 2020b). However, there was no consistent approach throughout the UK of the “2 
meters or 1 meter with risk mitigations (where 2 meters is not viable) are acceptable” (UK Government, 
2020a) being applied to all ages (UK Government, 2020b).  Whilst England kept the 2-meter distance for all 
ages, there was leeway put in place for children under the age of 11 years in Northern Ireland (NIDirct, 
2020), Scotland (Scottish Government (SG), 2020), and Wales (WG, 2020a; 2020b).  Guidance on how 
adventure playgrounds could operate post-lockdown was developed by London Play (2020) where: 

Social distancing measures should be observed by anyone present who does not need to be in closer contact with the 
family group – maintaining a 2m physical distance where possible” and “limit the duration of playground stays to 
an hour at a time (p. 1).  

The guidelines produced by London Play indicated parents and carers stay with the children (which 
would be included in the numbers allowed) where ‘bubbles’ of groups would have 1-hour time slots.  With 
adventure playgrounds re-opening across the UK, the guidance by London Play reflected the UK Guidance 
in England where social distancing of at least 1m in England for all children was encouraged. In Scotland 
and Wales, the respective Governments provided guidance where social distancing for children under the 
age of 12 was relaxed (e.g. WG, 2020).   However, for older children and young people, the social distancing 
of 1-2 m still applied, as with the adventure playground staff.  

Significance and Purpose of the Study 

The aim of this study was to obtain a child’s perspective on social distancing and play.  The 
significance of the study was measures were being introduced on how children should play in relation to 
social distancing, however children’s views were not included, or considered when play-based settings 
were to re-open.  For example, play is a social activity, for example, pretend play is common with 5-year-
olds, chase games with 8-year-olds, and just ‘hanging out’ with 15 years, all involve close contact.  
Adventure playgrounds have a wide age range of 5-15 years using the provision.  This poses the question 
of how to socially distance children 12 years and older and who are playing with those 11 years and 
younger.  How possible is it to implement social distancing in children’s play? 

In the Isle of Man, lockdown finished earlier, and social distancing was abandoned on 15th June (Isle 
of Man Government (IoMG), 2020a; 2020b).  Concerning the adventure playground provision, this meant 
with necessary hygiene precautions in place (concerning the cleaning of equipment, sanitising hands, etc.) 
there was no limit to the number attending, no bookable system put in place and the children and young 



Children’s views on social distancing and playing… 

51 

people who used the provision could play in the way they did as before, not in ‘bubbles’ or designated age 
groups.  

Children and young people were thus able to return to their unique play environment, not having 
to worry about playing in close physical contact with both other children and the staff.  This provided a 
unique context to ask children and young people a hypothetical question on how they would be able to 
play once they were back on their adventure playground if social distancing had to be implemented?  The 
benefit of the hypothetical question is children and young people could respond without these measures 
being imposed on them, thus responses were based on their anticipated experience, rather than direct 
experience.  This study was undertaken after the first lockdown in March 2020 was relaxed, although in 
the UK there has been another two lockdowns and one further lockdown in the Isle of Man. 

Method 

Research with children requires different considerations when compared to adults.  This 
consideration requires what Punch (2002) refers to as using “research-friendly or ‘person-friendly 
techniques” (p. 337).  The views of children on their play and social distancing on their adventure 
playground were collected when the provision reopened on 15th June 2020.  

This study provides a historical perspective in one period of time from the end of the first lockdown 
in March 2020 to re-opening in June 2020 where the study was undertaken.  The research study undertaken 
was granted ethical approval from the ethics committee of the College of Human and Health Science at 
Swansea University. 

Research Design 

The research design used video to interview children who attended their adventure playground 
between the 13th and 23rd July 2020 after the first March 2020 Lockdown had finished in the Isle of Man.   
Interviews were undertaken using a ‘vox pop’ approach commonly used in journalism (Beckers, 2019).  
This involves short interviews that are video recorded (Beckers, 2019).  This approach makes the data 
collection method more playful and less intrusive to children and young people in their play and makes 
the ‘interview’ process very informal.   As children were returning after a period of lockdown, the 
interviews did not want to take up too much of their time playing on the adventure playground.  Interviews 
were undertaken between the 13th and the 23rd of July 2020, one month after the adventure playground 
reopened in June 2020 after the March 2020 lockdown. 

Children and young people were recruited from the adventure playground with no social distancing 
restrictions by an advert put up at the adventure playground explaining the study.  The children and young 
people who were interested in taking part had a signed consent form completed by their parent or carer, 
as well as it being stressed any participation was voluntary and would be anonymous.  Whilst the children 
and young people were playing, one of the adventure playground staff members went around with a video 
camera to undertake the short ‘vox pop’ interviews.  The agreed procedure was set out where the purpose 
of the study would be explained to the child and clarification they were happy to participant.  The 
researcher collecting the data used the same four questions below and all videos recorded were consistent 
with this approach.  It was stressed to all the children any participation was voluntary and would be 
anonymous and they did not have to answer any of the four questions: 

1. How old are you? 
2. What is it like being back on the adventure playground? 
3. What things do you do on the adventure playground? 
4. How would you play if you had to stay 2m apart? 

 The questions were specific to children who attend the adventure playground and were developed 
with a member of staff.  This considers addressing the aspect of credibility (Shenton, 2004).  The questions 
were broad and open and did not have any COVID-19 or lockdown-specific questions as the focus of the 
study was on social distancing and play.  The impact of COVID-19 can vary from individual to individual 



Pete KING & Chris GREGORY 

52 

and to keep with the focus of returning to the adventure playground, children were not asking questions 
that could specifically related to their health and well-being.  However, safeguarding procedures were put 
in place if any responses from the children and young people reflected any concern or distress as a result 
of COVID-19 or lockdown, and relevant parties would be informed.  Although this did not occur, it was 
important to consider the safeguarding of research participants. 

Although issues of ‘bias’ and ‘coercion’ always need to be considered in research, the video data 
were collected by a member of staff could raise issues of bias.  However, it was not possible to interview 
the children and young people by an independent researcher as travel to and from the Isle of Man was 
prohibited.  Other methods of data collection such as recording interviews through platforms like Zoom® 
were considered but would have been too intrusive as this would have required participants to leave their 
chosen play activity and be led to a laptop or computer.  The use of Zoom for interviews also relies on 
consistent connectivity.  The use of recording the videos on a mobile device worked well.  Interviews lasted 
between 35 seconds to 3.03 minutes and were short enough not to disrupt any children’s play more than 
was needed.  Most interviews were done individually, although most had other children and young people 
around playing, or in three instances, this was done individually but in a group of two or three children.  
The use of video recording allowed analysis of non-verbal language, location on the adventure playground, 
and is a naturalistic study, the noise and activities taking place in the background provided a snapshot of 
how the adventure playground was running with no social distancing needed.  

Interviews were all undertaken outside in a range of places including a picnic bench, wheelchair, 
dens, slides, rope swings, and standing in the grass area of the adventure playground.  One interview was 
undertaken in the indoor space where children also play and socialize.  Some participants were holding 
objects such as hammers so although interrupted in their chosen play, the children and young people were 
able to return to it relatively quickly. 

Participants 

The chosen sampling method of inviting children to take part who were already using the adventure 
playground used voluntary non-probability sampling.  However, when children were taking part, this 
resulted in other children being interested reflecting snowballing sampling in addition.   In total twenty-
five children and young people took part in the study aged from 4 years up to 13 years. 

Table 1. Participants’ characteristic  

 2-6 years 7-10 years 11+ years 
Girls 1 (4 years) 

2 (5 year) 
2 (8 years) 6 (11 years) 

2 (12 years) 
Boys 2 (5 years) 

(6 years) 
1 (7 years) 
3 (8 years) 
1 (9 years) 
1 (10 years) 

3 (11 years) 
1 (13 years) 

Analysis 

The Piagetian framework provided enabled a content analysis (Marshall & Rossman, 1995) to be 
undertaken. This enabled responses from the children to be coded (Elo & Kyngas, 2008) within the 
Piagetian framework.  The Piagetian framework had three headings:  pre-operational, concrete operational, 
and formal operational. This was all used to develop a framework to analyse the response and consider the 
age and developmental state of the children.  This is shown in Table 2. 

Table 2.  Analysis framework on Piagetian classification 

Piagetian 
Play (1962) 

Pre-operational 
2 to 6 years 

Concrete Operational 7 to 10 years Formal Operational 
11+ years 

 Children are figurative or perception-
oriented and play is symbolic and 
uses creative imagination 

Children are capable of thinking 
logically, but always with a basis 
in concrete or material things and 
includes construction games 

Children are capable of abstract 
reasoning 



Children’s views on social distancing and playing… 

53 

The framework enabled the coding of data to consider how each participant's play preferences and 
responses reflect their potential age and development.  This included both verbal responses transcribed 
directly to what the children were saying as well as the non-verbal responses.  Transcription involved 
transcribing by hand each interview.  This involved watching and re-watching each video.  With the 
interviews being video recorded, non-verbal responses can be considered as “Video offers an open 
invitation to the researcher to look beyond the spoken word and find meaning from other dimensions of 
participant activity” (Ramsey et. al., 2016, p. 3) explained as “recursive transcription” (p. 3) where non-
verbal responses in conjunction with the spoken (verbal) narrative can include: 

nonverbal, semiotic fields of interest as: gesture and pointing, gaze and attention, body position and movement, 
touch, tone and inflection, facial expression, and engagement with material objects (Ramsey et. al., 2016, p. 3) 

The data collected by the adventure playground staff member was analysed by a second member of 
the research team who did not know the children.  This enabled a separation of data collection and analysis 
to reduce bias and consider credibility (Shenton, 2004) of the process.  When the data was analysed, this 
was sent back to the staff member to check for accuracy and the confirmability of the results (Shenton, 
2004).  The aspects of credibility and confirmability relate to trustworthiness (Lincoln & Guba, 1985).  The 
responses were discussed between the researcher collecting the data and the second researcher analysing 
the interviews. It was agreed the use of Piagetian classification reflected the responses, and the content 
analysis enabled capturing the views of the children. 

Findings 

Table 3 shows the content analysis using the Piagetian framework to group responses from the 
different age groups.  The Piagetian framework used reflects three of the four stages and these are linked 
to two of Piaget’s classification of play of symbolic play and games with rules.  As the questions focused 
on how children use the adventure playground, the responses were more detailed with the older children, 
reflecting more cognitive thinking of their responses.  This is considered when discussing the results below. 

Table 3.  Responses within Piagetian classification 

When returning to the adventure playground, all the children felt positive with the words fun and 
good being used the most.  For the children 11+ years, their responses were accompanied by elaborate 
gestures such as giving a thumbs up.  The content analysis is discussed about each of the three age groups. 

Pre-Operational (2-6) Years 

When asked what it was like to return to the adventure playground, single-word answers of “Good” 

 Pre-operational  
(2-6) 

Concrete Operational 
(7-10 years) 

Formal Operational 
(11+ years) 

Return to Adventure 
Playground 

Good (2) Fun (6) 
Weird (2) 
Good (2) 
Best 
Awesome 
Amazing 
Great 

Fun (8)  
Good (4) 
Okay 
Amazing 
Happy 
enjoying 
Cool 

Types of Play Slides (2) 
Swings 
Teddies 

Build (5) 
Play on slides (3) 
Break stuff (2) 
Explore (2) 
Food 

Building stuff (9) 
Play with other people (5) 
Food (4) 
Slides (3) 
Swings 

Social Distancing Line up and take 
turns (2) 

Bring in Nerf guns to play with 
Only play with people you know 
Build standing apart 
Not come in 
Play carefully 
Wear masks 

Would not be here (3) 
Would not do it (2) 
Difficult to implement (2) 

Non-Verbal Smiles Smiles and gestures Smiles and more elaborate gestures 



Pete KING & Chris GREGORY 

54 

accompanied by a smile were consistent.  When asked how they like to play on the adventure playground, 
a specific object was stated such as swings and slides.  The responses on how they would play if they had 
to stay 2 m apart, it was clear there was an understanding of the distance, and the replies could be described 
as ‘compliant’ where the child demonstrated how turn-taking could be undertaken by queueing as 
illustrated in this response: 

“I would stand here, and they would be over there and then I would go on the slide and then they would go on the 
slide” (Girl aged 4 years) 

Concrete Operational (7-10 years) 

As with the pre-operational responses, when asked what it was like to be back on the adventure 
playground, single-word answers of “Fun” and “Good” with a smile, where one child said:  

“Really fun, I haven’t been here for a whole 3 months and I was sad I could not come” (Boy, aged 7 years) 

The types of play included both object play (slides and swings) but also construction play of building 
dens, forts.  This was evident where the children being interviewed had a hammer in their hands whilst 
answering the questions the responses were more detailed: 

“I like jumping off over there *points to the play storage container*, like having the food here. Have fun with my 
friends, build and digging” (Boy aged 10 years) 

When asked how they would be playing if they had to remain 2m apart, the responses would be 
described as ‘compliant and adaptive’ where again acceptance of 2m was reflected in their responses, 
however, the children would go into more detail on how they would adapt their play to maintain a 
distance: 

“I would build, but *uses stretched out arm to illustrate* one person would be on one side of the building and other 
person would be on the other side of the building” (Girl aged 8 years) 

Formal Operational (11+ years) 

The responses on what it was like to return to the adventure playground were also single words 
such as ‘good’ and ‘fun’ and the use of smiles, however, these words were accompanied with distinct 
gestures such as a ‘thumbs up’ and exaggerated hand and arm movements: 

 “FUN *wide smile and stretching out their arms* (Girl a, aged 11 years) 

The type of play included both objects (swings and slides) and construction (den building) but also 
specified the social aspect of meeting friends.  There was also more emphasis in the answers using head 
movements and pointing to where the various activities take place: 

“I like hammering, cooking food, starting the fire, helping people out and I like playing” (Girl b, aged 11 years) 

When asked how they would play if social distancing had to be adhered to, there was a distinct 
‘confrontation’ in their responses from defiance (not do it) to would not come to the adventure playground.  
The responses included very clear gestures of shaking heads from side to side, shrugging of shoulders, and 
widening of eyes or curling of lips: 

“I wouldn’t be able to do that, it would be really stressful *shakes head from side to side* (Girl, aged 12 years) 

One response around the issues of playing and social distancing was encapsulated in the response 
below: 

“It would probably feel weird, because you’re supposed to help people like, say you’re helping someone hammer, 
you need to hold the nail, but you can’t do that social distancing so it’s going to be pretty tricky” (Boy, aged 11 years) 

What was evident from the video interviews was the snapshot of what happens in the adventure 
playground to support children’s and young people’s play.  Some of the participants were using the play 
resources whilst being interviewed, such as hammers or sitting on a slide or swing.  For some of the 
interviews, particularly the 7-10-year age range, there was a playful interchange as often other children 
would do things like stand behind the interviewee and do ‘bunny ears’ or when pointing to an activity they 
like doing, children are making dens, jumping off structures or sitting around chatting.  This playful 



Children’s views on social distancing and playing… 

55 

interchange also included the interviewer who also had ‘bunny ears’ made behind them, having their hat 
stolen or children playing with their hair.  This was all off-camera but mentioned by the children being 
interviewed. 

When summarizing the results, it was evident all the children were pleased to be back on the 
adventure playground to engage in their chosen play.  The chosen favourite way children played on the 
adventure playground did reflect the Piaget classification and types of play where pre-operational (4-6 
years) were more object play focused.  For the operational stage (7-10 years) this involved more 
construction whilst for the formal operational (11+ years) the responses from the children had more 
emphasis on the social aspect of play. When asked about social distancing (keeping 2m apart when 
playing), pre-operational children were more compliant, operational children were adaptive and formal 
operational were confrontational. 

The results will be discussed concerning the unique environment adventure playground provide in 
meeting the play needs of a wide range of children and consider how applying social distancing, 
particularly to the formal operational age range of 11+ years may need reconsidering in light of their social 
needs and expectations. 

Discussion 

When adventure playgrounds re-opened in July 2020 in the UK, social distancing regulations were 
placed on all children in England (UK Government, 2020b) and for children aged 12 years or over in years 
in Northern Ireland (NIDirct, 2020), Scotland (SG, 2020) and Wales (WG, 2020). The restrictions imposed 
on social distancing and children’s play were asked to children and young people who returned to their 
adventure playground with no social distancing restrictions in place.  This provided a unique study where 
children and young people could hypothesise how they would play if 2m apart, rather than it being 
imposed and having to do so.  The study also provides an important historical context of playwork during 
the Covid-19 pandemic. 

When children were asked about their play and social distancing using a Piaget (1962) 
developmental classification indicated children’s understanding of the world differs between the pre-
operational (4-6 years), operational (7-10 years), and formal operational (11+years) stages where children 
at the pre-operational and operational differ on how they play and respond to questions compared to 
children in the formal operational stage.  Although critics of Piagetian theory exists (Feldman, 2005), the 
use of pre-operational, and formal operational did provide a framework where the responses on how they 
play, and how they would play 2m apart did reflect this Piagetian classification.  Pre-operational children 
were more focused on object play, operational children with physical play and formal operational children 
preferred more social type focused play.  A consideration of social play and social interaction reflects 
Vygotsky’s (1978) view of the importance of play in children’s development and how this will differ in 
respect to the different Piagetian stages.  For example, for the formal operational (11 years and older) the 
social aspect may take more priority than what and how children play.  For the pre-operational, children 
will play socially, but the object being played or the activity undertaken may take more priority.  The 
variety of what happens in any play space indicates why adventure playgrounds are important as they do 
provide varied play spaces for play to meet children’s developmental and social needs across a wide age 
range. 

This varied play environment indicates how children’s understanding and acceptance of social 
distancing would be different as indicated in this study where children in the pre-operational stage were 
more compliant, operational children adaptive and formal operational confrontational about keeping 2m 
apart when playing.  The ‘blanket approach’ of the social distancing policies of 1m to 2m for children in 
England (UK Governement, 2020a) would have been met differently by the children in this study 
dependent on their age.  Where in Scotland (SG, 2020) and Wales (WG, 2020a; 2020b) the 2m social 
distancing only applied to those over 12 years, children within the formal operational stage in this study 
would have either not attended the adventure playground or ignored the 2m rule. For older children, the 



Pete KING & Chris GREGORY 

56 

adventure playground would become a less attractive place to meet friends, and thus not be able to use 
their play environment. Where restrictions were lifted for children under the age of 12 years, this study 
indicated they would be more compliant in maintaining the 2m rule, although if this was put into practice, 
this may not be the case when children are playing. 

The importance of considering children and young people’s views on social distancing and play 
reflects the wider issue of how play is important and the role of adventure playgrounds in providing space 
to play. During the March 2020 lockdown, and when adventure playgrounds re-opened, fears expressed 
in the UK in relation to a reduction to outdoor space have “endangered child health and widened pre-
existing disparities” (Editorial, 2020, p. 1).  Guan et. al. (2020) stated children “obtain their daily physical 
activity” (p. 416) through a range of activities including active play. However, how children and young 
people play the stipulation of social distancing makes interaction with friends in outdoor activities 
problematic especially in spaces such as adventure playgrounds where children engage in many physical 
types of play.  

Children in this study across the age range clearly expressed their pleasure in being back on the 
adventure playground, and with no social distancing, this did not restrict numbers or the type of play that 
engaged in before the March lockdown.  Children returning to their adventure playgrounds in July 2020 
in the UK social distancing was put in place.  However, numbers were reduced and where children did 
attend, social distancing was difficult to maintain, whatever the age of the child (King, 2021b).  As with the 
views of the children in this study, there had to be some compliance, where for some children they were 
placed in ‘bubbles’ and some adaptation of both resources provided and the types of play that was still 
possible for children to engage in (King, 2021b).  However, the enforcement of social distancing became 
harder to enforce particularly for the older-aged child (King, 2021b).  The views of the children from this 
current study where social distancing was hypothesized reflected what happened with children and young 
people in adventure playgrounds with limited numbers and social distancing measures that were put in 
place.  This indicates the importance of consulting with children concerning policies that have an impact 
on their dedicated play space. 

Limitations and Implications for Future Research 

There are limitations to this study that are acknowledged.  Firstly, children were asked questions by 
the adults in the adventure playground which could have influenced or biased their responses.  As children 
were asked during their play, and only lasting between 50 seconds and 3 minutes, this did not take up too 
much of their time and the responses reflected the enjoyment that could be ascertained in the play activities 
in the background and the noise of children playing.  The responses were reliable and not coerced by the 
interviewer.  The sample of 25 children, although fairly small, was a good sample size for the number of 
children registered and using the adventure playground, especially as it had only been running for 2 
months before the March lockdown.   

The third limitation of this study is the children left lockdown earlier in the Isle of Man and with no 
social distancing compared to England for example.  Since the lockdown, when the study was undertaken 
only one case of COVID-19 on the Isle of Man had been reported and so the views of the children may not 
represent those who have returned to their adventure playground with social distancing requirements put 
in place.  This would be a relevant follow-up study with children to see if social distancing had been 
implemented on their adventure playground and what impact did it have on their play.  

Conclusion 

This study provided a unique opportunity to listen to children’s voices on their experience of 
returning to their play environment after a period of lockdown, and not having to worry about social 
distancing only hypothesis about it.  The study shows how important play is to children and that careful 
consideration has to be put in place concerning how play environments, designated specifically for 
children, need to consider their views on social distancing, particularly older children the opportunity to 



Children’s views on social distancing and playing… 

57 

meet friends and socialise may be impeded if restrictions are put in place. 

From this study, children of all ages enjoy and needed the adventure playground post lockdown.  
For the older child, aged 11 years or over, this social distancing would be ‘ignored’.  If this is the case, then 
it has to be considered where will children meet and congregate?  At least on specific play provisions for 
children, such as adventure playgrounds if children are meeting their friends and being able to play 
outside, it would be easier to track and trace if there are any COVID-19 related infections. Government 
policy and guidelines need to consider that children do not always diverge into two groups of primary 
(under 12 years) and secondary (12 years to 18 years), and careful consideration of the risk and benefits of 
adventure playgrounds as a safe space, away from vulnerable adults that may be more important than 
before when considering children’s health, wellbeing, and development. 

Declarations 

Editorial Acknowledgement: The editorial process of this article was carried out by Dr. Mine Gol-Guven. 

Acknowledgements:  Not applicable. 

Authors’ contributions: This was collaboration between PK at Swansea University and CG at the Isle of Play. 

Competing interests: The authors declare that they have no competing interests. 

Funding: No funding. 

Ethics approval and consent to participate: The research study undertaken was granted ethical approval from the ethics committee 
of the College of Human and Health Science at Swansea University. 

References 

 Allen of Hurtwood, M. (1968).  Planning for play.  MIT Press. 

Almon, J., & Keeler, R. (2018).  The rise of adventure play provision in North America.  Children, Youth and Environments, 28(2), 67-77. 
https://doi.org/10.7721/chilyoutenvi.28.2.0067  

Alwan, N. A., Burgess, R. A., Ashworth, S., Beale, R., Bhadelia, N., Bogaert, D., Dowd, J., Eckerle, I.,  Goldman, L., R.,  Greenhalgh, T., 
Gurdasani, D., Hamdy, A., Hanage, W. P., Hodcroft, E. B., Hyde, Z., Kellam, P., Kelly-Irving, M., Krammer, F., Lipsitch, M., 
McNally, A., McKee, M., Nouri, A., Pimenta, D., Priesemann, V., Rutter, H., Silver, J., Sridhar, D., Swanton, C., Walensky, R. 
P., Yamey, G., & Ziauddeen, H. (2020). Scientific consensus on the COVID-19 pandemic: we need to act now. The 
Lancet, 396(10260), e71-e72. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)32153-X  

Beckers, K. (2019). What vox pops say and how that matters: Effects of vox pops in television news on perceived public opinion and 
personal opinion. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 96(4), 980-1003. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699019843852  

Bengtsson, A. (1972).  Adventure playgrounds. Crosby Lockwood Staples. 

Chilton, T. (2003).  Adventure playgrounds in the twenty-first century.  In F. Brown (Ed.), Playwork theory and practice (pp. 101-113).  
Open University Press. 

Chilton, T. (2018).  Adventure playgrounds:  A brief history.  In F. Brown & B. Hughes (Eds.),  Aspects of playwork:  Play & culture 
studies (Vol.14, pp. 157-178).  Rowman & Littlefield Publishing Group. 

Cranwell, K. (2003).  Towards a history of adventure playgrounds 1931–2000.  In N. Norman (Ed.), An architecture of play: A survey of 
London’s adventure playgrounds (pp. 17-26).  Four Corners. 

Editorial Board (2020).  Growing up in the shadow of COVID-19. The Lancet. Child & Adolescent Health, 4(12), 853. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30349-7  

Elo, S., & Kyngäs, H. (2008). The qualitative content analysis process. Journal of advanced nursing. 62(1), 107-115. 
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04569.x  

Evening News. (1947, November 6).  Junk playground keeps these children happy.  Evening News. 

Feldman, D. H. (2004). Piaget's stages: the unfinished symphony of cognitive development. New Ideas in Psychology, 22(3), 175-231. 
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2004.11.005  

Garner, B., P., & Bergen, D. (2006).  Play development from birth to age four. In D. Pronin Fromberg & D. Begren (Eds.), Play from 
birth to twelve:  Contexts, perspectives, and meanings (pp. 3-12). Routledge. 

Guan, H., Okely, A. D., Aguilar-Farias, N., del Pozo Cruz, B., Draper, C. E., El Hamdouchi, A., Florindo, A. A., Jáuregui, A., 
Katzmarzyk, P. T., Kontsevaya, A., Löf. M. Park, Wl Reilly, J. J., Sharma, D., Tremblay, M. S., & Veldman, S. L. (2020). 

https://doi.org/10.7721/chilyoutenvi.28.2.0067
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)32153-X
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699019843852
https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30349-7
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04569.x
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2004.11.005


Pete KING & Chris GREGORY 

58 

Promoting healthy movement behaviours among children during the COVID-19 pandemic. The Lancet Child & Adolescent 
Health, 4(6), 416-418. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30131-0  

Hughes, B. (2002).  A playworker’s taxonomy of play types (2nd ed.).  PlayLink. 

Hurtwood, L. A.. (1968).  Planning for play.  The MIT Press. 

Isle of Man Government. (2020a).  Stay responsible.  https://covid19.gov.im/general-information/stay-responsible/  

Isle of Man Government. (2020b).  Social distancing.  https://covid19.gov.im/social-distancing/  

Johnson, J. E. (2006). Play development from ages four to eight years. In D. Pronin Fromberg & D. Begren (Eds.), Play from birth to 
twelve:  Contexts, perspectives, and meanings (pp. 13-20). Routledge. 

King, P. (2020). Can playwork have a key working role?. International Journal of Playwork Practice, 1(1), Article 1. 
https://doi.org/10.25035/ijpp.01.01.07  

King, P. (2021a). How have adventure playgrounds in the United Kingdom adapted post-March Lockdown in 2020?. International 
Journal of Playwork Practice, 2(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.25035/ijpp.02.01.05  

King, P. (2021b).  The impact of COVID-19 on playwork practice.  Child Care in Practice, 1-17. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/13575279.2020.1860904  

Kinoshita, I., & Woolley, H. (2015).  Children’s play environment after a disaster: The great east Japan earthquake.  Children, 2(1) 39-
62. https://doi.org/10.3390/children2010039  

Kovlosky, K. (2008).  Adventure playgrounds and postwar reconstruction.  In M. Gutman & N. de Coninck-Smith (Eds.), Designing 
modern childhoods: History, space, and the material culture (pp. 171-192).  Rutgers University Press. 

Lee Manning, M. (2006).  Play Development from Ages Eight to Twelve.  In D. Pronin Fromberg & D. Begren (Eds.), Play from birth to 
twelve:  Contexts, perspectives, and meanings (pp. 21-30).  Routledge. 

Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. SAGE. https://doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767(85)90062-8  

London Play. (2020). Adventure playgrounds and the Covid 19 pandemic Guidance on infection risk reduction. 
https://www.londonplay.org.uk/resources/0000/3528/AP_Guidance_Covid_19.pdf.  

Marshall, C., & Rossman, G. B. (1995). Designing qualitative research. SAGE. https://doi.org/10.1130/0016-
7606(1995)107%3C0463:QUASCD%3E2.3.CO;2  

Newstead, S. (2019). Le playwork à la recherche d’une identité perdue. Sciences du Jeu, 12, 1-22. https://doi.org/10.4000/sdj.2337  

Nicholson, S. (1971).  How not to cheat children:  The theory of loose parts.  Landscape Architecture, 62, 30-35. 

NIDirect (2020).  Coronavirus (COVID-19) regulations guidance: What the restrictions mean for you. 
https://www.nidirect.gov.uk/articles/coronavirus-covid-19-regulations-guidance-what-restrictions-mean-you   

Piaget, J. (1962).  Play, dreams and imitation in chilhood.  Norton & Company Inc. 

Play England. (2011).  Adventure playgrounds as community hubs:  Community play briefing 4.  Play England. 

Play England. (2017).  Adventure Playgrounds: the essential elements updated briefing.  Play England.  http://www.playengland.org.uk/wp-
content/uploads/2015/11/Adventure-Playgrounds.pdf?fbclid=IwAR1WDfqzQp4hlpNzXjht5jqIfuToyqEM-n-
1zZYzlTyRKGyLvdnohHGVgOM.  

Punch, S. (2002). research with children:  The same or different from research with adults? Childhood, 9(3), 321-341. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/0907568202009003005  

Scottish Government. (2020).  Publication - advice and guidance Coronavirus (COVID-19): What you can and cannot do.  
https://www.gov.scot/publications/coronavirus-covid-19-what-you-can-and-cannot-do/pages/seeing-friends-and-family/  

Shenton, A. K. (2004). Strategies for ensuring trustworthiness in qualitative research projects.  Education for Information, 22(2), 63–75. 
https://doi.org/10.3233/EFI-2004-22201  

Shier, H. (1984).  Adventure playgrounds:  An introduction.  National Playing Fields Association. 

Sutherland, F. (2014).  What makes an award winning adventure playground?. Journal of Playwork Practice, 1(2), 205–208. 
https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X16664557  

Trawick-Smith, J., Swaminathan, S., Baton, B., Danieluk, C., Marsh, S., & Szarwacki, M. (2017). Block play and mathematics learning 
in preschool: The effects of building complexity, peer and teacher interactions in the block area, and replica play 
materials. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 15(4), 433-448. https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X16664557  

UK Government. (2020a).  Guidance:  COVID-19: Guidance for managing playgrounds and outdoor gyms. 
https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/covid-19-guidance-for-managing-playgrounds-and-outdoor-gyms/covid-19-
guidance-for-managing-playgrounds-and-outdoor-gyms  

https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30131-0
https://covid19.gov.im/general-information/stay-responsible/
https://covid19.gov.im/social-distancing/
https://doi.org/10.25035/ijpp.01.01.07
https://doi.org/10.25035/ijpp.02.01.05
https://doi.org/10.1080/13575279.2020.1860904
https://doi.org/10.3390/children2010039
https://doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767(85)90062-8
https://www.londonplay.org.uk/resources/0000/3528/AP_Guidance_Covid_19.pdf
https://doi.org/10.1130/0016-7606(1995)107%3C0463:QUASCD%3E2.3.CO;2
https://doi.org/10.1130/0016-7606(1995)107%3C0463:QUASCD%3E2.3.CO;2
https://doi.org/10.4000/sdj.2337
https://www.nidirect.gov.uk/articles/coronavirus-covid-19-regulations-guidance-what-restrictions-mean-you
http://www.playengland.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2015/11/Adventure-Playgrounds.pdf?fbclid=IwAR1WDfqzQp4hlpNzXjht5jqIfuToyqEM-n-1zZYzlTyRKGyLvdnohHGVgOM
http://www.playengland.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2015/11/Adventure-Playgrounds.pdf?fbclid=IwAR1WDfqzQp4hlpNzXjht5jqIfuToyqEM-n-1zZYzlTyRKGyLvdnohHGVgOM
http://www.playengland.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2015/11/Adventure-Playgrounds.pdf?fbclid=IwAR1WDfqzQp4hlpNzXjht5jqIfuToyqEM-n-1zZYzlTyRKGyLvdnohHGVgOM
https://doi.org/10.1177/0907568202009003005
https://www.gov.scot/publications/coronavirus-covid-19-what-you-can-and-cannot-do/pages/seeing-friends-and-family/
https://doi.org/10.3233/EFI-2004-22201
https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X16664557
https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X16664557
https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/covid-19-guidance-for-managing-playgrounds-and-outdoor-gyms/covid-19-guidance-for-managing-playgrounds-and-outdoor-gyms
https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/covid-19-guidance-for-managing-playgrounds-and-outdoor-gyms/covid-19-guidance-for-managing-playgrounds-and-outdoor-gyms


Children’s views on social distancing and playing… 

59 

UK Government. (2020b).  Guidance:  Coronavirus (COVID-19): Social distancing.  
https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/coronavirus-covid-19-meeting-with-others-safely-social-
distancing/coronavirus-covid-19-meeting-with-others-safely-social-distancing  

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press. 

Wales Government. (2020a).  Guidance:  Coronavirus (COVID-19) social distancing guidance for everyone in Wales.  
https://gov.wales/coronavirus-social-distancing-guidance   

Wales Government. (2020b).  Guidance:  Protecting staff and children from the coronavirus: Guidance for open access playwork providers.  
https://gov.wales/protecting-staff-and-children-coronavirus-guidance-open-access-playwork-providers    

Ward, C. (1961).  Adventure playground:  A parable of anarchy. Freedom Press. 

Welsh Government (2014).  Wales – a Play Friendly Country Statutory Guidance.  Accessed at 
https://gov.wales/sites/default/files/publications/2019-07/wales-a-play-friendly-country.pdf.  

Welsh Government. (2020).  Guidance for hubs and schools: Provision for children of critical workers and vulnerable children.  
https://gov.wales/sites/default/files/publications/2020-04/guidance-hubs-schools-coronavirus-provision.pdf   

Whitebread, D., Basilio, M., Kuvalja, M., & Verma, M. (2012). The importance of play:  A report on the value of children’s play with a 
series of policy recommendations  Accessed at https://www.csap.cam.ac.uk/media/uploads/files/1/david-whitebread---
importance-of-play-report.pdf  

 

https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/coronavirus-covid-19-meeting-with-others-safely-social-distancing/coronavirus-covid-19-meeting-with-others-safely-social-distancing
https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/coronavirus-covid-19-meeting-with-others-safely-social-distancing/coronavirus-covid-19-meeting-with-others-safely-social-distancing
https://gov.wales/coronavirus-social-distancing-guidance
https://gov.wales/protecting-staff-and-children-coronavirus-guidance-open-access-playwork-providers
https://gov.wales/sites/default/files/publications/2019-07/wales-a-play-friendly-country.pdf
https://gov.wales/sites/default/files/publications/2020-04/guidance-hubs-schools-coronavirus-provision.pdf
https://www.csap.cam.ac.uk/media/uploads/files/1/david-whitebread---importance-of-play-report.pdf
https://www.csap.cam.ac.uk/media/uploads/files/1/david-whitebread---importance-of-play-report.pdf