JULY 2021 34 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

What is a Child? Exploring Conceptualization of Pakistani Adolescents About 
Children

Azher Hameed Qamar

Azher Hameed Qamar has a PhD in interdisciplinary child research from Norwegian University of Sciences and Technology, Nor-
way. His research focuses on childcare beliefs and practices and the social value of the child. He is an assistant professor in the 
Department of Behavioral Sciences, National University of Sciences and Technology Pakistan and a postdoctoral researcher in the 
School of Social Work, Lund University, Sweden. Email: azher_hameed.qamar@soch.lu.se

“Concepts” as tools for analysis (Nilsen, 1990) help 
to dig deeply into the social construction process 
in specific sociocultural contexts. Conceptualizing 
children in childhood studies is significant to un-
derstand childhood in diverse cultures. Every child 
has a childhood that varies depending on social 
context and differences regarding the conceptual-
ization of children and their characteristics. In the 
sociology of childhood, research about children 
in their social context requires an investigation of 
the conceptualization of children (Christensen & 
Prout, 2005; James et al., 1998; Jenks, 1996; Punch, 
2002; Sommer et al., 2010). The new sociology of 
childhood emphasizes studying children as social 
actors in their social contexts who are conscious, 
thinking individuals with the capacity to shape 
their world in a variety of ways by reflecting on 
their situation and the choices available to them at 
any given time (James, 2007). Being a child is not 
a universal experience; it is socially constructed 
and shaped by a range of social, cultural, and his-
torical dynamics (Franklin, 1995; James & Prout, 
1990; Punch et al., 2012). In most societies there is 
a clear distinction between childhood and adult-
hood, and different norms, values, and expecta-

tions are attached to them (Franklin, 1995; Montgomery, 2003). Hence, the question “What is a child?” brings 
to light people’s understanding of the “child” as a concept that may be useful to investigate parenting practices, 
children’s lives, and childhood in their social context. 

The United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child (UNCRC, 1989) is considered a successful UN conven-
tion about children recognized by the whole world (except Somalia and the USA). The UNCRC defines a child as 
a human being below the age of 18 (UNCRC, Article 1). This numerical definition does not include a minimum 
age; hence, it may extend to considering a fetus as a child (as is seen in several cultures). Social research, in general, 
refers to a group of children (and young people) who are defined as children under UNCRC (Punch et al., 2012). 
However, Mayall (2012) emphasizes studying children as a social group like other major groups in the society, “not 
defined by age, but as one defined by social, economic and political class” (p. 348). This emphasis spotlights the 
contested issue of the universality of numerical age (James & James, 2012) and the tendency to see childhood as 
a social and cultural construct. The concept of chronological age seems universal when observed by international 

This study aimed to investigate the responses of university 
students (late adolescents) about their conceptualization 
of a child, exploring the characteristics they associate 
with being a child. The study was conducted in two 
phases. In phase 1, responses to one open-ended 
question, what is a child? (N=75), were analyzed using 
qualitative content analysis. In phase 2, students (N=90) 
filled in an online closed-ended survey that was derived 
from the subthemes that emerged from the qualitative 
data collected in phase 1. Findings revealed multiple 
interconnected aspects of the conceptualization of the 
child, making it a complex whole. This study is helpful 
for understanding the concept of the child grounded in 
various theoretical and mythological categories that 
portray the complexities of existing dichotomies that 
often come up as interconnected in traditional societies.

Key words: child; evil child; innocent child; agentic child; 
human-being; human-becoming



JULY 2021 35 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

laws; however, in practice, contrary to traditional societies where age is perceived in its biological perspectives, the 
modern Western construct of age links the chronological and social age of the child (Halcrow & Tayles, 2008). In 
traditional (particularly agrarian) societies there is a close link between children’s biological age and their social 
participation. Several children’s activities require physical competence to participate in domestic tasks seen as a 
process of learning cultural and vocational skills to enhance the continuity of the socialization process (Abebe, 
2007; Chant & Jones, 2003; Priblisky, 2001; Punch, 2001, 2002; Kesby et al., 2006). Similarly, children’s biological 
growth places them in categories of boys and girls, explicitly segregating them into two different social groups. 
Hence, the biological changes in the body are seen as a standard by which to both measure the child’s physical 
strength to perform different activities and to assign gender roles and structured boundaries. Though these roles 
and boundaries are context specific, they make a difference in the perceptions about children and childhood, em-
phasizing the connection between biological and social age.

Even though the numerical definition of the child is used in legislation and is a part of policy documents about 
children, I am looking into the conceptualization of a child beyond this general definition. In the following passag-
es, I present important theories about children and childhood that will provide a top-down conceptual stance of 
the study. I begin with mythological images of children that perceive innocence and evil as “natural” constructs de-
fining the “child” and constituting parenting practices accordingly. Later, I present the understanding of children 
and childhood as theorized in the new sociology of childhood that contests the socialization and developmental 
theories in sociology and psychology.

Innocence versus evil: Mythological images
The innate nature of the child (as perceived by the society) affects the conceptualization of the child and different 
issues related to parenting and child-rearing practices. For example, considering the child as “naturally good” em-
phasizes the society’s responsibility to protect the child, particularly during early childhood, from corruption by 
developing the child according to his or her “natural” needs (Cunningham, 1995; Fass, 2004). Conversely, a child 
who is “naturally evil” needs strict discipline to get rid of the evil disposition and to become a good adult (Jenks, 
1996; Sorin, 2005). The views about children’s innate nature range from seeing them as sinful, polluted creatures to 
seeing them as innocent beings born naturally good (Hartas, 2008). Jenks (1996) described these two (at different 
times) popular traditions of conceptualizing the child as mythological images and named them Dionysian child 
and Apollonian child. He linked these images with the exercise of social control in Europe and a shift from an “old 
European order to the new order of modern industrial society” (p. 74). The two images are contrasted to each oth-
er with their descriptions of a child born innately evil (Dionysian child) or innately innocent (Apollonian child). 
Note that these are concepts leading to subsequent constructions of childhood; they do not mean that a child is 
necessarily evil or innocent. Jenks made it clear that these are powerful images that empower “normal” discourses 
about children, but they do not contribute to intrinsically different children.

Dionysian child: Evil

The Dionysian child is associated with the doctrine of Adamic original sin, a dominant discourse in Christianity 
(Jenks, 1996; Sorin, 2005). According to the discourse of original sin, “the new-born baby is full of the stains and 
pollution of sin, which it inherits from our first parents through our loins” (DeMause, 1974, p. 10). Jenks explains 
that the concept of the evil child existed in the 16th century when children were vulnerable to corruption and par-
enting consisted of unfriendly and harsh moral supervision and strict discipline. Nevertheless, Sorin (2005) points 
out that current concepts of discipline and punishment in schooling connect with the notion of the evil child. She 
describes the reinforcement of the “evil child” image in teachers’ disciplinary actions aimed at teaching behaviour 
as expected by the adults. The disciplinary power that parents and teachers exercise is a mode of socializing the 
child into a “good” adult. In the school setting, the explicitly decisive adult control and passiveness of the child 
construct childhood as a stage for learning to be an adult. Training through punishment and discipline is a prac-
tice that can be found in empirical findings. During my research on primary schooling in rural Punjab, Pakistan 
(Qamar, 2010), I visited a public primary school and noticed a statement written on the school gate: “No beating, 
only love.” This was a slogan emphasized by the government to stop corporal punishment in the schools. During an 



JULY 2021 36 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

informal discussion with a group of teachers, two children who were fighting came to them with complaints about 
each other. One of the teachers slapped both the children. When I asked him about it, he told me that these two 
kids were naughty and always destroyed discipline in the classroom. The teacher’s description discloses an action 
he engaged in to maintain discipline. However, it shows a conceptual stance that children are naughty and that 
punishment is a mode of training and teaching discipline. I met several teachers and parents who believed in pun-
ishment to correct children, even though they were not clear about “how much” punishment. A general thought 
was, “it depends on how naughty or disobedient the child is.” “Discipline” and “obedience” are two perceived traits 
of a “good” child that help children grow into successful adult members of Punjabi society. Hence, punishment (or 
at least the fear of punishment) is a “useful” parenting and teaching strategy in this context.

Apollonian child: Innocence

The Apollonian child is a Western way of viewing infants as naturally angelic and innocent. An Apollonian child 
does not inherit the original sin but is a descendant of “a humankind before either Eve or the apple” (Jenks, 1996, p. 
73). Children, in this image, are born good with natural virtues and disposition; hence, they should be encouraged 
and facilitated. Jenks views the formalization of the Apollonian child in line with the manifesto Emile, authored by 
Rousseau. In this perspective, children, due to their innate innocence, deserve special care and attention. Child-
hood (for an innocent child) is a pure time of life separate from adulthood when children need protection and 
care by adults (Sorin, 2005). This image reinforces the right to life and the healthy development of a child before 
birth (Jenks, 1996). There are several serious social concerns (such as sexual harassment, molestation, violence) 
that conceptualize children as being at risk. The concepts of a “sacred” child and a child at risk strengthen the dis-
course of innocence and the perception that children need adult protection. There are several protective measures 
that adults adapt to shelter children from the harmful influences of the world. Children without adult protection 
are exposed to violence, molestation, fear, and anxiety. However, adult protection is provided at the cost of limit-
ing children’s participation and agency through an adult view of children as fragile, incompetent, and dependent 
(Sorin, 2005). Nevertheless, the notion of weakness and dependence is central to the children portrayed as sick, 
poor, and hungry in the Global South. Hence, “what a child should be” resists the idealized notion of the child as 
an “independent” human being who can resist their “childish” status (Burman, 1994).

The discourses of evil and innocence persist in different social issues regarding children in society. A child as a 
victim is seen as innocent, and a child committing a crime may be seen as an evil child. Innocence is linked with 
the vulnerability that leads to the child’s weak social status and gives space for adults’ demand for obedience 
(Meyer, 2007). Viewing children as evil or innocent shares a conceptual similarity, and children in both images are 
dependent on adults (Sorin, 2005). In the case of children’s evil nature, adults shape them to learn obedience and 
submission, and children are required to listen to whatever adults command, advise, and expect. This is seen as a 
key to becoming a good adult. On the other hand, an innocent child needs adult protection. Children, with their 
innocence, are ignorant and fragile. In both cases, children’s voices are muted in these two mythological images.

Conceptualization of children in sociology of childhood
To study children and their childhood with a sociological lens, a researcher identifies the commonalities of child-
hood in different societies. A universal understanding of these commonalities that constitutes children as a social 
group includes the difference of children from adults and their submission to and dependency on adults (May-
all, 2012). In the global model of childhood, this distinction between children and adults is seen as natural and 
universal. Children’s smaller, weaker status is defined by the things they “cannot do.” In this model, the notion of 
“competency” (or lack thereof ) is defined by comparing what children “cannot” do with what adults can do. This 
adult-centric comparison is tied up with a future orientation of children (Jenks, 1996; Qvortrup, 2009). Hence, 
the traditional perspective in the sociology of childhood constitutes the “child” as a human becoming who is 
dependent on adults due to its vulnerability, incompetence, and powerlessness. Several developmental and so-
cialization theories support this conceptualization. A taken-for-granted biological immaturity and psychological 
incompetency imply a stage-like progression of a human becoming (the child) gradually developing into a human 
being (the adult). Moreover, the developmental paradigm informs the early educational system, placing children 



JULY 2021 37 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

as underdeveloped cognitive biological bodies and incorporating predetermined pedagogies to shape children as 
perceived and required by the system (Vintimilla & Pacini-Ketchabaw, 2020). This “colonization” of childhood un-
dermines children’s experiences as individuals in their own right and universalizes the psychological construction 
of development placing children as human becomings (Schraube & Osterkamp, 2013). The developmental dis-
course and adult-child dichotomy “naturalized” the notion of the “incomplete child” and established a formal ed-
ucation system where the child’s perceived incompetence determines the stages of progression towards a complete, 
finished, and civilized human being who is not a child any more (Cunningham, 1995; Vucic, 2017). Hence, until 
recently the defining parameters for being a child in almost all fields of enquiry (such as child psychology, child 
labour, child abuse, child welfare, early education) were restricted to the developmental markers conceptualizing 
children as physically, psychologically, and socially vulnerable and developing. It is a recent development (at the 
end of the 20th century) that systematic approaches engaged constructionists’ epistemological position to reexam-
ine the so-called natural boundaries of childhood in connection with the reciprocal relationship between children 
and social forces that shape children and childhood in sociocultural contexts (see Boyden, 1990; Corsaro, 2000).

Before the emergence of the new sociology of childhood, academic experts in different disciplines viewed children 
as learners to be socialized into adults (Montgomery, 2003). In a social world, constructed by adults, children are 
identified as non-adults, and adults structure their lives as objects of socialization (Mayall, 2001). Early socializa-
tion, in this respect, is a process to shape the child to become a functioning member of society. On the other hand, 
the universalization of modern childhood has increased the sensitivity about children’s vulnerability, and the line 
drawn to differentiate between children and adults has become thick and clear. Children are seen as a restricted 
group dependent on adults to protect them from abuse and violence. Parents and teachers are provided with skills 
and knowledge to reinforce children’s appropriate behaviour, and on the other hand, children’s access to knowl-
edge that can corrupt their behaviour is restricted (Buckingham, 2009; Cunningham, 1995).

However, during the last three decades, the social construction of childhood that is the basis of sociology found its 
place in scholarly debates that were dominated by medical and psychological discourses on the concept of a uni-
versal childhood with a stage-like progression toward adulthood. According to Nieuwenhuys (2013), these postco-
lonial theories challenged the Eurocentric constructs of “modern” and “universal” childhoods by contextualizing 
children’s lives in diverse sociocultural contexts. Consequently, it exposed the parallels of developmentalism and 
colonialism, while at the same time it gave voice to children’s perspectives and experiences by reconceptualizing 
childhood in its diversity and uncertainty (Nieuwenhuys, 2013). 

Discourse on the socially constructed child considers the interconnected social realities that may be used to con-
ceptualize children and interpret childhoods by contextualizing the lives of children and how those lives are sit-
uated in specific contexts (James et al., 1998; Kjørholt, 2004). The interconnected social realities include, but are 
not limited to, parenting, families, gender roles, and childcare practices. It is noted that the statuses of socially 
constructed children, who are in no way universal children, should be considered in empirical analyses that use 
the contexts of children’s lives and examine how children are conceptualized in those contexts (Kjørholt, 2004). 
These discourses encouraged the emergence of the new sociology of childhood and the social construction of 
childhood as contesting responses to the global model of childhood. The new sociology of childhood challenges 
the notion of the child as dependent on adults because of its vulnerability and powerlessness. In contrast, it con-
siders children as social actors and social agents who coconstruct their childhood with adults (James et al., 1998; 
Corsaro, 2010). Childhood is a variable of social analysis that is not entirely separate from other social variables, 
such as gender, ethnicity, and class. Conceptualizing children as social actors and childhood as a social construct 
encourages parents and educators to focus on children’s agency. The new sociology of childhood insists on under-
standing children as active beings who contribute to the social construction of their lives and the lives of others in 
society. Children can negotiate their roles, make social relationships, and present themselves as active human-be-
ings (James, 2007; James & Prout, 1990). 

The neglect of children and their voices in traditional views of socialization and the failure of developmental psy-
chology to consider children in the complexities of social structures gave rise to the new sociology of childhood 



JULY 2021 38 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

(Corsaro, 2010). This paradigm emerged in the 1990s. However, it has its roots in several theoretical resourc-
es, such as 1960s/70s interactionist sociology that problematized the notion of the passive child in socialization; 
1980/90s structural sociology (primarily in Europe) that saw childhood as a social structure; and social construc-
tionism (in both Europe and the USA) that problematized taken-for-granted concepts of childhood (Prout, 2005). 
Mayall (2012) identifies other contributing conceptual developments, including identity politics, inclusion and 
exclusion, and power relations. Nevertheless, the new paradigm offers an explicitly distinctive conceptualization 
of children and childhood contrary to the view of a child as “becoming” in socialization theory and developmen-
tal psychology (see Jenks, 1996). Consequently, the methodological frameworks investigating children’s matters 
were also emphasized to include child-friendly research methods to give voice to children as human beings. Clark 
and Moss (2011), in “Listening to Young Children: The Mosaics Approach,” conceptualized children as “skillful 
communicators, experts in their own lives, right holders and meaning makers” (p. 6). Hence, the new sociology of 
childhood offers a shift from a conditioned, controlled, and developing child (child as an object) to an authentic 
and political child (a child-centered perspective to view children as subjects) who authors himself/herself with an 
inherent sense of self-growth (Barter & Renold, 2000; Christensen & Prout, 2005; James et al., 1998; Jenks, 1996; 
Lee, 2001). Table 1 compares the new sociology of childhood’s conceptualization of the child as a human being 
with the traditional “child as becoming” approach. 

Table 1. Child as “Human Being” and “Human Becoming”

New Sociology of Childhood Traditional Perspective
The child is an individual and childhood is socially and 
culturally constructed. 

A child is a minor who goes through a natural process 
of childhood to become an adult.

The child is a contributing member of society and 
childhood is not a separate entity.

Childhood is the child’s world before the child becomes 
a contributing member of the society.

Children are competent beings. They are active social 
actors and contribute as capable members of the society.

Children are incompetent, weak, incapable, and 
passive. They cannot contribute actively until they 
become capable to act.

There is no comparison of children and childhood 
with adults and adulthood. Children should be seen 
independent of adults’ perspectives, and their rights, 
relationships, and interactions should be equally 
valued.

Childhood is a phase in the journey to adulthood 
(i.e., becoming an adult), and children are dependent 
on adults, thus childhood is viewed from an adult’s 
perspective. 

A child as an individual is unique and every child has 
his or her childhood (depending on the social and 
cultural context) that cannot be universalized.

The child as a “human becoming” is a universal 
approach, and childhood is a universal category.

Children do not passively absorb the adult world; they 
are active meaning makers who contribute with their 
creative and interpretative capabilities. 

Children learn values as passive learners and contribute 
later.

Children’s perspective is very important to understand 
childhood and their views are valued as much adults’ 
views.

Children’s perspective is not respected. Children need 
to learn from adults.

 

Conclusively, the new sociology of childhood conceptualizes children as competent social actors and agentic chil-
dren and values children’s voices (Sorin, 2005). Considering a child as a “whole” and “complete” human being 
does not mean a child is indistinguishable from an adult; however, children should have the same value as adults 
(Nilsen, 1990). Moreover, competency cannot be seen in terms of age, hence, cannot be differentiated on the basis 
of numeric age definitions. The social, psychological, and physical competency that a child is born with is affected 
by different sociocultural factors. The idea of an active child who has their own perspective is neither dependent 



JULY 2021 39 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

on chronological age nor is it universally attained (Burr & Montgomery, 2003; Punch, 2002).

Method
I employed a mixed-method approach in this study. Primarily, in phase 1, I did a qualitative content analysis of the 
responses collected through a questionnaire with one open-ended question. Respondents (who were undergrad-
uate university students) wrote 4 to 10 sentences to describe a child; of these, 95% wrote more than 6 sentences. 
All 75 responses included 588 sentences that were coded according to emergent characteristics of the child and 
clustered in subthemes and themes. In the second phase, I used the subthemes to prepare a closed-ended online 
survey containing statements describing the child. Ninety students (different from the 75 respondents in phase 1) 
filled in this online survey. The results of the survey were analyzed using descriptive quantitative analysis to extend 
the validation of phase 1. 

Sampling

Using a convenience sampling technique, I approached undergraduate university students within the age range of 
19 to 23 years (M=20.5) studying social sciences in three universities and requested them to fill in the online ques-
tionnaires used in the two phases of data collection. I was interested to know how the students, who themselves 
were considered to be in late adolescence according to the numerical definition of a child, conceptualized the child. 

In phase 1, I visited students in their classrooms (after seeking permission from the class teacher and head of the 
department), and invited them to fill in an online questionnaire. Besides basic demographic information (age and 
gender), the respondents were asked to answer one open-ended question (What is a child?) in an online survey. I 
briefed them about the topic of the study and the question they were requested to answer. I made sure that respon-
dents understood the question, that is, they were supposed to describe their own understanding of the child. They 
could write short sentences or a paragraph to describe the child. Receiving responses through an online qualitative 
survey was useful to gain a diversity of young adults’ perspectives. Using only one specific question was helpful 
to narrow down the responses and encourage respondents’ focus on a single idea. I received 75 responses (N=75) 
including 28 responses from female students (47 male, 28 female). The medium of instruction in Pakistani univer-
sities is English. Therefore, all the participants responded in the English language. Only 12 participants included 
one to four sentences in Urdu while responding to the question. However, their answers were similar to those who 
reported the same words describing a child in the English language. 

In phase 2, a closed-ended online survey (Table 2) was designed using the subthemes that emerged from the 
qualitative data during phase 1. Thirty-one (31) statements were derived from the subthemes with three options: 
(i) agree, (ii) disagree, and (iii) not sure. During phase 1, none of the respondents mentioned numerical age to 
describe a child. However, during phase 2 one statement was added to find the respondents’ opinions regarding 
the numerical definition of the child, with four options: (i) below 18 years, (ii) below 15 years, (iii) below 10 years, 
(iv) numerical age does not define the child. The survey was sent to student email groups and it was expected that 
about 200 students would receive the survey in their emails. However, only 90 students (22 male, 78 female) re-
sponded and filled in the survey.

Table 2. What is a Child? Closed-Ended Survey

The child is a human below the age of ------ years.
18 years 15 years 10 years Numeric age does not apply

Agree Disagree Not sure
The child is a physically immature human
The child is social and expressive
The child learns through imitating (copying) adults



JULY 2021 40 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

The child is self-confident
The child is energetic and active
The child is crying and demanding
The child is delicate and fragile
The child is a helpless human who depends on adults
The child is a curious explorer
The child is beautiful and attractive
The child is intelligent and sharp
The child learns by following adults
The child is stupid and nonsense
The child is an inexperienced human
The child is stubborn and inflexible
The child is selective and choosy in his/her matters
The child is jealous
The child is loving and adorable
The child has an imaginary world
The child is naughty and wild
The child can set goals and follow them
The child is a teacher
The child is disturbing and irritating
The child is sensitive
The child is clever and smart
The child joyful and funny
The child is creative
The child is innocent
The child is selfish
The child is playful
The child is cute and sweet

Data analysis

Qualitative data collected during phase 1 was analyzed using inductive (bottom-up) qualitative content analysis 
and theoretically directed categories (top-down). All the sentences used to describe the child were underlined as 
meaning units. I carefully assigned codes to the meaning units (see Table 3) and clustered the codes in subthemes 
and themes. Themes were placed in their corresponding theoretical categories derived from the literature (as de-
scribed earlier). To improve analytical validity, I shared the codes and clusters with one of my colleagues to get her 
feedback. Her feedback helped to remove ambiguities and refine codes and clusters. 

Table 3. Content Analysis of the Responses. Examples of Meaning Units and Emerging Codes

Meaning units Codes



JULY 2021 41 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

Children are naughty and they make too much mess. They 
become irritating to seek attention.

naughty, disorganized, irritating, attention 
seeker

Children are easy-going. They play in the mud and do not care 
how they look like. They enjoy their life. They do not know 
right or wrong. 

comfortable, playful, joyful, indifferent, 
ignorant, innocent

Children are cute and innocent. When they start talking, they 
talk too much. Sometimes, they are annoying too. 

cute, innocent, talkative, annoying

The children are beautiful and lovely. However, when they start 
crying they do not stop until they do not get what they want. 

beautiful, loving, stubborn, demanding

A child is a flower, beautiful but can wither if mishandled. 
Children need adults to take care of them. They cannot walk 
and talk themselves. 

cute, beautiful, delicate, fragile, vulnerable, 
dependent, developing

Before data collection in phase 2, the survey items were examined by the same colleague who provided feedback 
during phase 1, and a few items were revised for clarity. Data collected during phase 2 was quantitively analyzed, 
and the percentages of the responses were calculated to describe results in connection with the themes and theo-
retical categories that had emerged in phase 1.

Findings
The findings convey key points to build an understanding of the multiple, interrelated, and contextual aspects 
of the conceptualization of the child, explaining how respondents view and describe a child. Asking only one 
open-ended question during phase 1 helped to narrow down the data in line with a theoretical understanding 
of the concept of the child. Corresponding to four theoretically directed thematic categories, 12 themes emerged 
from the data (see Table 4), with 31 subthemes. It was an interesting aspect of the findings in phase 1 that none 
of the participants mentioned any numerical age to define child, whereas all the sentences revealed the child 
described to be an infant or toddler. It appeared that, contrary to adults (parents or grandparents), who may con-
ceptualize children based on their responsibilities towards children and the value of children in their lives, late 
adolescents described children as they had observed or interacted with them in their everyday lives. For example, 
one of the participants wrote:

They are innocent but they are also wild. They cry too much and want to play all the time. Children are 
naughty and create troubles for the adults. Sometimes it is amusing to play with them, but they can get 
wild too. They look cute when they are playing. 

Another participant described the child as weak and vulnerable:

The child is cute. The child is a beautiful innocent human. The child is weak and sensitive. The child 
needs adults’ protection to grow strong. The child is a future adult. The child is also naughty, and act 
smartly sometimes. 

It was also noted that several participants provided contrasting characteristics of the child in their responses, for 
example, portraying a child as an idiot and nonsense and at the same time describing it as naughty and clever. 
Similarly, participants described the child as “active” and “passive” simultaneously and did not draw a distinctive 
line between human-being and human-becoming. 

Table 4 shows themes and subthemes that emerged from the qualitative data analysis during phase 1 and the per-
centage scores against each subtheme that were collected through the online survey during phase 2. 

Table 4. Concept of a Child: Themes, Subthemes, and Survey Results (%)

Category Themes Subthemes Agree Disagree Not Sure



JULY 2021 42 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

Human Becoming Immature Physically immature 56.5 33.7 9.8
Passive Learns by following 80.4 12 7.6

Inexperienced human 69.2 23.1 7.7
Learns through imitating 91.3 4.3 4.3

Dependent Helpless and dependent 64.1 25 10.9
Human Being Active Social and expressive 69.6 17.4 13

Energetic and active 50 35.9 14.1
Agentic Self-confident 41.6 41.6 16.9

Sets goals 51.6 39.6 8.8
Selective and choosy 64.8 25.3 9.9
Curious explorer 74.7 12.1 13.2

Competent Creative 84.4 8.9 6.7
Imaginative 53.3 35.6 11.1
Clever and smart 74.7 16.5 8.8
Intelligent and sharp 76.9 11 12.1
A teacher 48.4 34.1 17.6

Evil Child Idiot Stupid and nonsense 16.5 71.4 12.1
Possessive Stubborn and inflexible 42.9 33 24.2

Crying and demanding 37 55.4 7.6
Evil Naughty and wild 60 17.8 22.2

Disturbing and irritating 33 50.5 16.5
Jealous 54.5 29.5 15.9
Selfish 27.8 53.3 18.9

Innocent Child Beautiful Beautiful and attractive 89 5.5 5.5
Cute and sweet 90.1 5.5 4.4

Vulnerable Innocent 87.9 6.6 5.5
Delicate and fragile 83.3 3.3 13.3
Sensitive 92.3 4.4 3.3

Romantic Loving and adorable 92.3 4.4 3.3
Joyful and funny 92.3 4.4 3.3
Playful 92.3 3.3 4.4

The evil child and the innocent child

Several responses described the child as “naughty.” However, the word “naughty” as it emerged from the data was 
not related to “cuteness.” It was linked with irritating behaviour. Similarly, describing the child as an idiot (such 
as nonsense and stupid) and possessive (such as stubborn) were seen as negative characteristics that respondents 
associated with being a child. Interestingly, respondents also described jealousy, selfishness, and wildness as innate 
behaviours of the child. Hence, I clustered the codes representing negative descriptions of the child as an “evil” 
child who is idiotic, possessive, naughty, and innately evil. Overall, this thematic category was comparatively less 
reported in the data.



JULY 2021 43 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

The most-reported words describing the child were cute, beautiful, joyful, loving, and innocent. Beautiful and ro-
mantic are two subthemes that are closely connected and that give a romanticized image of childhood. However, I 
clustered the words presenting a physical view of a romanticized “beautiful” child (such as cute, attractive, sweet) 
separately from the words presenting an interactive view of a romanticized child (such as adorable, joyful, playful). 
Respondents who described the child as “innocent” contradicted the concept of the child as “evil.” The data also 
showed a close connection between the romanticized image of the child with the notion of fragility and vulnera-
bility of the child. This conceptualization leads to the protective role of adults.

Comparing the “agreed” and “disagreed” responses in the survey results (Phase 2, see Figure 1) showed that the 
majority of the respondents conceptualized children as “innocent,” whereas the number of responses agreeing on 
the “evil child” was significantly low. Interestingly, conceptualizing the child as “innocent” romanticized the image 
of the child, but also placed them in a fragile and vulnerable group who are dependent on adults for their protec-
tion. This idea necessarily draws a distinctive line between adults and children, separating them based on power 
and strength, as well as the “innocence” that is connected with the romantic world of the children that amuses 
adults. It was also reflected in the responses as the percentage of responses in favour of conceptualizing children 
as human-becomings is slightly higher than conceptualizing children as “human-beings.” One reason that I see is 
that the perception of “innocence” (as disclosed in both phases of data collection) is closely connected with chil-
dren’s immaturity and their dependence on adults. As I already said that respondents in this study assumed that a 
“child” is an infant or toddler, their understandings of “innocence” and “human-becoming” seem relative. 

Child as human-becoming and human-being

Another significant finding in this study is the dichotomy of human-being and human-becoming that appeared 
in contrasting responses in qualitative and quantitative data analysis. While the difference between the percent-
age responses of subthemes related to human-becoming and human-being was not significant, it was evident in 
open-ended responses during phase 1. For example, one participant wrote:

The children are good learners. The children are quick in processing information whatever they ob-
serve. They copy others’ behaviours. The children are players. They are clever and play with the adults. 
They can manage their time and tasks while doing different things.

Another participant views children as passive and active simultaneously. 

The children are dependent on adults. They are helpless. The children are intelligent and creative. They 
are expressive. They are naughty but look cute. They are loveable. They are energetic. 

Respondents conceptualized the child as a human-becoming based on the stage-like progression of the child’s de-
velopment. The child is physically incompetent and lacks several developmental milestones that it can reach over 
time. A biological immaturity, such as being unable to talk, walk, and eat unassisted is linked with being a child. 
This biological immaturity upholds the dependence of the “developing” child on adults, particularly at an early 
age. Another aspect of the concept of a developing child is its cognitive passiveness that respondents related to the 
imitative learning behaviour of the child, describing it as “naïve” and “copycat.” 

Contrary to viewing a child as a human-becoming, respondents also described the child as active, agentic, and 
competent, which leads the concept of the child to its existence as a human being. However, responses showed that 
respondents were referring to a child who is not an infant. For example, they described the talkative and expressive 
child as confident, active, and social. Hence, there was a biological competence in terms of language development 
that they connected with an “expressive” child who seeks attention by acting smartly and setting goals. Many 
respondents pointed out that the characteristics of a child are curiosity, smartness, and creativity, qualities that 
represent an agentic and competent child.



JULY 2021 44 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

Figure 1. Adolescents’ conceptualization of the child.

Discussion
The concept of the child is more complex than the numerical definition of a child that is seen as universal in its 
legal perspective (see Figure 2). Even though a universal conceptualization of children is prevalent in international 
child rights policies (influenced by the UNCRC), a distinction among conceptualizations of children (that vary 
according to the context of the children’s lives) is helpful to examine the successes and limitations of interna-
tional policies. In this connection, Erica Burman (1996) emphasized the distinctions between local, global, and 
globalized conceptions of childhood. She raises concerns about the inadequate conceptual resources that inform 
international policies on children’s rights and well-being. To understand and manage the conflict that a universal 
conceptualization of the child (as appears in international legislation) may cause while transcending sociocultural 
constructs of childhood, it is important to bridge the gap between cultural imperialism and cultural relativism. 
This is possible if the distinctions between local, global, and globalized conceptions of childhood are explicitly 
visible and central in policymaking (Burman, 1996).

The conceptualization of children in different social and cultural contexts situates the value of children, parenting, 
and childcare practices. The four theoretical themes that emerged in this research (as described earlier) present 
mythological images of the child as evil or innocent and debated concepts of the child in the sociology of child-
hood (human-becoming and human-being). In three of the theoretical themes, the child is seen as dependent on 
adults. The evil child needs to be corrected and disciplined. The innocent child needs to be protected and nurtured. 
The child as human-becoming needs to be socialized and developed. Hence, these three themes present a child as 
a human-becoming where adults have a role to shape the naughty, fragile, and incompetent child into a civilized, 
independent, and competent adult. A shift from the Christian discourse on original sin and the evil child to the 
innocent child of the romantic period significantly influenced parenting practices. Despite the change in the “nat-
ural” status of the child from “devil” to “angel,” children remained dependent in an adult-centered world where 
adults commanded and controlled the child’s world. On the other hand, both traditional developmental and so-



JULY 2021 45 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

cialization theories conceptualize a child as a human becoming, an incomplete, biologically immature, vulnerable, 
and incompetent human. The child as human-becoming needs to be protected and cared for, as well as shaped and 
moulded by the society for the society. Besides several articles of the UNCRC (1989) mentioning the fragile nature 
of childhood and the vulnerability of the child, the aim of education, as described in UNCRC (Article 29), is to 
develop and prepare children to reach their full potential and contribute as responsible members of society. The 
developmental markers place the child as a human-becoming who develops in a stage-like progression. However, 
the development of children’s capacities corresponding to their numerical age is derived from Western models. 
When they are applied in non-Western settings, they will not necessarily produce the same results. Similarly, the 
socialization process in relation to non-Western children’s livelihood and education needs (local and academic 
knowledge) varies significantly when compared to children’s lives in the Western context. Overall, the rights-based 
approach (in connection with provision and protection rights that are mostly undisputed) conceptualizes the child 
as a human-becoming who needs protection and training.

An interesting aspect of the UNCRC is its recognition of the rights of the individual child in their own perspective 
and best interest (Articles 3, 12). The best interest of the child, as stated in Article 3, restricts the power practices 
of adults towards children through legislative bodies to ensure the protection and well-being of children. At the 
same time, it recognizes children’s physical incompetence to protect themselves and their psychological incompe-
tence to make decisions in their own best interest. Hence, an adult should find out the best interest of the child in 
different situations. Article 12 emphasizes listening to children’s voices, respecting children’s views, and recogniz-
ing their right to express their concerns in matters related to them. However, these rights are subject to the child’s 
“capability” to form views that relate to “age” and “maturity.” The only marker that informs this “capability” is the 
numerical age of the child that is used to define a child in UNCRC. Here, the new sociology of childhood questions 
the universality of the rights-based approach as stated in UNCRC and presents the child-centered notion of an 
“agentic” child whose world is coconstructed with the adult world. The child is a member of the adult world and a 
“knowing subject” (see James, 2007) who contributes as a social actor in the adult world. The child holds a social 
value that shifts their powerless position to a self-authored status. The agentic child is a human-being who cocon-
structs the world as an active participant. However, because the child is not free from biological and psychological 
developmental stages, the human-being and human-becoming dichotomy is problematic. Recognizing a child as 
a social actor in their sociocultural context and considering their physical and psychological developmental needs 
does not result in a dichotomy (Lee, 2001; Prout, 2004). The child is also a human-becoming in connection with 
child-rearing practices and the socialization process. 

Dominant scientific theories include cognitive development theories and socialization theories. Piaget’s work on 
cognitive development described a universal stage like progression of the child’s cognitive development (from 
immature to mature thinking) that significantly influenced scientific academia (Montgomery, 2008). On the other 
hand, socialization theories presented a “passive learning” approach that focused on child-rearing practices that 
are diverse (in different societies) and not natural (Lee, 2001; Montgomery, 2008). Nevertheless, both perspec-
tives conceptualize children as “human becomings” who are in the “learning phase” of becoming an adult. This 
conceptualization, applied as “natural” and “universal,” presents children in a category different than adults due 
to their innocence and need to be protected from the adult world. This Western model of childhood presented as 
a global model by international agencies, conventions, and policies could not address the significantly different 
experiences of children and diverse models of childhood in the majority world (Montgomery, 2008; Qvortrup, 
2009). Respondents in my study were students residing in urban areas. The survey results show that 36% of the 
respondents believed that a child is a human below the age of 10, and 35% thought that numerical age does not 
define a child. Hence, their perception of the child as a human becoming probably revolves around the biologically 
and psychologically developing child that can be explicitly observed during this age or early childhood. During 
my ethnographic research investigating infant healthcare beliefs and practices in rural areas of Pakistan, I found 
that people perceive a child as an adult according to their participation in everyday life. For example, a child who 
can help with agricultural activities, managing livestock, and taking care of domestic chores and responsibilities 
is perceived as grown up, and the “biological” boundary (based on biological maturity) of childhood is gradually 
diminished (Qamar, 2010, 2019).



JULY 2021 46 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

Figure 2. The concept of a child – theoretical connections.

The UNCRC has been criticized for its so-called universalized conception of children’s lives and parents as pri-
marily responsible for children’s upbringing and well-being. However, children in the global south live in extended 
families, and parenting practices and responsibilities are not limited to parents. The pattern of socialization of 
children, interdependent households, and multiple caregivers make childhood in the global south significantly dif-
ferent from childhood within the nuclear families of the global north (Burman, 1996). Childhood in non-Western 
contexts provides a complex picture of economically and culturally contextualized socialization and development 
of children. School and family are seen as institutions for formal education and traditional socialization respective-
ly. However, besides preparing children to be adult members of society, the traditional socialization (particularly 
in rural contexts) also includes children’s participation in the domestic economy and their recognition as compe-
tent and agentic children. In traditional societies where interdependency is considered the strength of the society 
(and the community) and children are brought up to maintain this strength, we have to look into the concepts of 
being and becoming side by side (Abebe, 2007; Antoniou, 2007; Punch, 2001; Qamar, 2010, 2015).

The codes (subthemes) that emerged in this study provide a variety of words to describe a child that correspond to 
several conceptual categories. The majority of the responses indicate that the child was conceptualized as “inno-
cent” and a “human-being.” Responses also revealed that the respondents conceptualized the child as a human-be-
coming in terms of early age development, where the child is seen as biologically immature. A few responses were 
related to the child as “evil,” mostly considering the child as naughty and irritating. The concept of the evil child 
(seen as jealous, possessive, selfish, demanding, and irritating) suggests an “innate competence” of the child to ma-
nipulate and exploit adults. The idea of innate competence also supports the notion of an “agentic” child who has 
a perspective that should be respected, and whose world should not be separated as a category of passive minors 
who must be shaped before their membership in the adult world. On the other hand, even though the innocent 
child is physically and psychologically fragile and vulnerable, the aesthetic aspect attached to a romantic child 



JULY 2021 47 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

presents a socially visible and active child who is playful and interactive. In either view, the child is a social actor 
who actively contributes to the adult world. 

Advances in the new sociology of childhood presented four research approaches and theoretical paradigms (that 
may overlap) that consider children as human beings and social actors. These four approaches are the socio-struc-
tural child, the minority world child, the socially constructed child, and the tribal child (James et al., 1998). The 
socio-structural child approach views childhood in a structural form that in a wider structure remains uniform 
despite their varied manifestation across different societies. In this approach, childhood has shared common-
alities and remains a phase of life that never disappears. The minority world child is a politicized version of the 
socio-structural child that views children as active beings. Living in the adult-centered world, children are distin-
guished from adults and marginalized. Their perceptions are explored in connection with children’s rights putting 
them in age-based categories; hence, elements of universality and being “global” are there. In contrast, the socially 
constructed child approach conceptualizes childhood as historically and culturally varied and focuses on local 
conceptualizations of childhood with reference to several contextual elements such as class, age, gender, geog-
raphy, ethnicity, religion, etc. As a relativistic analytical approach, socially constructed childhood (as a historical 
and social construct) illuminates the particularities of childhood in time and place. The tribal child approach is a 
politicized version of the socially constructed child. The tribal child approach places children in their own cultural 
world that is different than adults’ but is not adult-centered. Hence, the focus is on the otherness of childhood and 
children’s everyday lives independent of adult-child relations and adult concerns. Childhood is seen as a real social 
world of children and their autonomous participation in it makes their world unique and distinctive (James et al., 
1998; Kjørholt, 2004). However, ignoring adult-child relations is not as simple as seen in this approach because of 
the frequent adult-child interactions between children, parents, and teachers in home and school settings. 

Multiple aspects of the concept of the child provide the diversity of adults’ perspectives on children and childhood 
that make it too complex to give a single universal definition of the child. This study’s findings provide a broader 
canvas on which to paint childhood beyond the limitations of numerical age that restricts the conceptualization of 
children through chronological aging. The diversity of responses revealed in this study places the conceptualiza-
tion of children in various epistemological domains that presents childhood studies as a field of interdisciplinary 
inquiry. 

Conclusion
Mythological images of the child describe children as “evil” or “innocent” and influence parenting and teaching 
strategies. Since both of these images separate childhood from “real” adult life, adults play an authoritative role 
to discipline and protect children. Children are seen as weak, vulnerable, and incompetent. A shift offered by the 
new sociology of childhood liberates children from the conceptualization of incompetency and vulnerability and 
positions them as “beings” in their right. The conceptualization of children in this new perspective is undergoing 
debates about the diversities of children’s competencies in different social contexts and contested chronological 
perspectives of the majority of legal policies. Findings in this study revealed a variety of concepts that young adults 
associated with children. Their descriptions of the child showed a multidimensional yet interconnected image of 
the child that adds to the complexity of the concept of the child and the nature of childhood in any social context. 

Considering diverse interconnected regional and global conceptualizations of childhood, the new sociology of 
childhood emerged as a response to wide changes to global political and economic challenges that affected region-
al contexts. An interdisciplinary childhood study can use diverse theoretical concepts of childhood and children 
in rich interconnected discussions to develop holistic pictures of childhood.

Limitations
The conceptualization of children significantly affects parenting practices, adult-child relationships, and children’s 
schooling. The qualitative questionnaire used in this study had only one open-ended question to be answered by 
late adolescents studying in Pakistani universities. The idea was to capture first-hand spontaneous thoughts of the 



JULY 2021 48 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

adolescents, narrowing the focus to a description of the child as they perceived it. Later, a closed-ended survey 
derived from the emergent subthemes proved to be a valuable instrument to be used for maximum variation sam-
ples. This study should be taken as a pilot study to initiate mainstream projects related to parenting, schooling, and 
parent-child attachments studies.



JULY 2021 49 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

References
Abebe, T. (2007). Changing livelihoods, changing childhoods: Patterns of children work in rural southern Ethiopia. Children’s Geogra-

phies, 5(1–2), 78–93. https://doi.org/10.1080/14733280601108205
Antoniou, L. (2007). An ethnography of children’s participation in domestic work in Nicosia. Childhoods Today, 1(1), 1–25.
Barter, C., & Renold, E. (2000). “I wanna tell you a story”: Exploring the application of vignettes in qualitative research with children and 

young people. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 3(4), 307–323. https://doi.org/10.1080/13645570050178594
Boyden, J. (1990). Childhood and the policy makers: A comparative perspective on the globalization of childhood. In A. James & A. Prout 

(Eds.), Constructing and reconstructing childhood (pp.187–211). Falmer. 
Buckingham, D. (2009). New media, new childhoods. An introduction to childhood studies. In M. J. Kehily (Ed.), An introduction to 

childhood studies (pp. 124–138). Open University Press.
Burman, E. (1994). Innocents abroad: Western fantasies of childhood and the iconography of emergencies. Disasters, 18(3), 238–253. 

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7717.1994.tb00310.x
Burman, E. (1996). Local, global or globalized? Child development and international child rights legislation. Childhood, 3(1), 45–66. 

https://doi.org/10.11770907568296003001004
Burr, R., & Montgomery, H. (2003). Children and rights. In M. Woodhead & H. Montgomery (Eds.), Understanding childhood: An inter-

disciplinary approach (pp. 135–178). Open University Press.
Chant, S., & Jones, G. A. (2003).Youth, gender, and livelihoods in West Africa: Perspectives from Ghana and the Gambia. Children’ s 

Geographies, 3(2), 185–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/14733280500161602
Christensen, P., & Prout, A. (2005). Anthropological and sociological perspectives on the study of children. In S. Greene & D. Hogan 

(Eds.), Researching children’s experience: Approaches and methods (pp. 42–60). SAGE.
Clark, A., & Moss, P. (2011). Listening to young children: The mosaic approach. Jessica Kingsley. 
Corsaro, W. A. (2000). Early childhood education, children’s peer cultures, and the future of childhood. European Early Childhood Edu-

cation Research Journal, 8(2), 89–102. https://doi.org/10.1080/13502930085208591
Corsaro, W. A. (2010). The sociology of childhood. SAGE.
Cunningham, H. (1995). Children and childhood in Western society since 1500. Longman. 
DeMause, L. (1974). The history of childhood. Rowman & Littlefield.
Fass, P. S. (2004). Encyclopedia of children and childhood in history and society. Thomson Gale.
Franklin, B. (1995). The case for children’s rights: A progress report. In B. Franklin (Ed.), The handbook of children’s rights (pp. 3–22). 

Routledge. 
Halcrow, S., & Tayles, N. (2008). The bioarchaeological investigation of childhood and social age: Problems and prospects. Journal of 

Archaeological Method and Theory, 15(2), 190–215. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-008-9052-x
Hartas, D. (2008). The right to childhoods: Critical perspectives on rights, difference, and knowledge in a transient world. Continuum Inter-

national.
James, A. (2007). Giving voice to children’s voices: Practices and problems, pitfalls and potentials. American Anthropologist, 109(2), 

261–72. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4496640
James, A., & James, A. (2012). Key concepts in childhood studies. SAGE.
James, A., Jenks, C., & Prout, A. (1998). Theorizing childhood. Polity Press.
James, A., & Prout, A. (1990). A new paradigm for the sociology of childhood? Provenance, promise, and problems. In A. James & A. 

Prout (Eds.), Constructing and reconstructing childhood (pp. 7–31). Falmer. 
Jenks, C. (1996). Childhood. Routledge. 
Kesby, M., Gwanzura-Ottemoller, F., & Chizororo, M. (2006). Theorizing other, “other childhoods”: Issues emerging from work on HIV 

in urban and rural Zimbabwe. Children’s Geographies, 4(2), 285–202. https://doi.org/10.1080/14733280600807039
Kjørholt, A. T. (2004). Childhood as a social and symbolic space: Discourses on children as social participants in society (Doctoral dis-

sertation). Trondheim: Fakultet for samfunnsvitenskap og teknologiledelse. http://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:125285/
FULLTEXT01.pdf 

Lee, N. (2001). Childhood and society: Growing up in an age of uncertainty. McGraw-Hill. 
Mayall, B. (2012) An afterword: Some reflections on a seminar series. Children’s Geographies, 10(3), 247–355. https://doi.org/10.1080/14

733285.2012.693383
Meyer, A. (2007). The moral rhetoric of childhood. Childhood, 14(1), 85–104. https://doi.org/10.11770907568207072532
Montgomery, H. (2003). Childhood in time and place. In M. Woodhead & H. Montgomery (Eds.), Understanding childhood, an inter-

disciplinary approach. Wiley & Sons. 
Montgomery, H. (2008). An introduction to childhood: Anthropological perspectives on children’s lives. John Wiley & Sons.
Nieuwenhuys, O. (2013). Theorizing childhood(s): Why we need postcolonial perspectives. Childhood, 20(1), 3–8. https://doi.



JULY 2021 50 Vol. 46 No. 2

JOURNAL OF CHILDHOOD STUDIES ARTICLES FROM RESEARCH

org/10.11770907568212465534
Nilsen, R. D. (1990): Førskolebarns kulturelle produksjon i institusjonen barnehage relatert til sosialiseringsprosessen i det moderne 

samfunn. Barn, 8(1), 44–57. 
Priblisky, J. (2001). Nervios and “modern childhood”: Migration and shifting contexts of child life in the Ecuadorian Andes. Childhood, 

8(2), 251–73. http://www.justinecassell.com/CC_Winter05/pdfs/pribilsky_nervios-2.pdf
Prout, A. (2005). The future of childhood: Towards the interdisciplinary study of children. RoutledgeFalmer.
Punch, S. (2001). Household division of labor: Generation, gender, age, birth order, and sibling composition. Work, Employment, and 

Society, 15(4), 803–823. https://doi.org/10.1177095001701400438215
Punch, S. (2002). Research with children: The same or different from research with adults? Childhood, 9(3), 321–341. https://doi.

org/10.11770907568202009003005
Punch, S., & Tisdall, E. (2012). Editorial. Children’s Geographies, 10(3), 241–248. https://doi.org/10.1080/14733285.2012.693375
Qamar, A. H. (2010). Challenges associated with primary education in rural Punjab, Pakistan (Master’s thesis). Institutt for pedagogikk og 

livslang læring. National University of Sciences and Technology Pakistan. 
Qamar, A. H. (2015). Being a child in rural Pakistan. The Delhi University Journal of the Humanities & the Social Sciences, 2, 100–111. 

http://journals.du.ac.in/humsoc/pdf/QAMAR%20Child.pdf
Qamar, A. H. (2019). Infant healthcare belief practices and social value of the child in rural Punjab, Pakistan (Doctoral dissertation). Insti-

tutt for pedagogikk og livslang læring. National University of Sciences and Technology Pakistan.
Qvortrup, J. (2009). Are children human beings or human becomings? A critical assessment of outcome thinking. Rivista Internazionale 

di Scienze Sociali, 631–653. https://www.jstor.org/stable/41625246
Schraube, E., & Osterkamp, U. (2013). The colonization of childhood: Psychological and psychoanalytical explanations of human devel-

opment. In E. Schraube & U. Osterkamp (Eds.), Psychology from the standpoint of the subject (pp. 210–230). Palgrave Macmillan.
Sommer, D., Pramling Samuelsson, I., & Hundeide, K. (2010). Child perspectives and children’s perspectives in theory and practice. Springer.
Sorin, R. (2005). Changing images of childhood: Reconceptualizing early childhood practice. International Journal of Transitions in 

Childhood, 1(1), 12–21.
United Nations. (1989). Convention on the Rights of the Child. https://www.ohchr.org/en/professionalinterest/pages/crc.aspx
Vintimilla, C. D., & Pacini-Ketchabaw, V. (2020). Weaving pedagogy in early childhood education: On openings and their foreclosure. 

European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 28(5), 628–641. https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2020.1817235
Vucic, B. (2017). The colonization of childhood: The critical pedagogy of Janusz Korczak. In A. Odrowąż-Coates & S. Goswami (Eds.), 

Symbolic violence in socio-educational contexts: A postcolonial critique. Wydawnictwo Akademii Pedagogiki Specjalnej im. Marii 
Grzegorzewskiej.