Microsoft Word - Sianturi.docx


 ISSN: 2311-1550 

 
2022, Vol. 9, No. 3, pp. 528-544 

 
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution ShareAlike 4.0 International License. 

 
 

 

Relationships Between Cognitive Styles and Indigenous Students’ 
Mathematics Academic Outcomes   

Murni Sianturi1, Riska Suliantin2 and Hariani Fitrianti3 

1Department of Social Sciences, The University of New South Wales, NSW, Australia 
2Muhammadiah Junior High School, Merauke, Indonesia 

3Musamus University, Papua, Indonesia 

Abstract: This article explores the link between cognitive styles and Indigenous students’ mathematics 
academic outcomes. There were three different groups of Indigenous West Papuan students participating 
in this study: 9 junior high school students, 12 senior high school students, and 46 university students. 
Data were collected from the results of the group embedded figure test and the previous semester's scores 
and analysed using a quantitative approach. In contrast to previous studies, the results indicated no 
significant correlation between cognitive styles and mathematics academic outcomes for junior and senior 
high school students. However, the different cognitive styles showed a significant contribution for 
university students. As Indigenous students pursue a higher level of education, their cognitive styles 
would influence their mathematics academic outcomes. 

Keywords: Indigenous students, cognitive styles, mathematics academic outcomes, mathematics 
learning achievement, gender. 

Introduction 
The contested discourses about the success of Indigenous students have continued to be discussed 
among researchers (Chung et al., 2021; Harrison, 2011; Lowe, 2017;  Sianturi et al., 2018). 
Underperformance among Indigenous students has been found globally (Abdullah et al., 2013; 
Burridge et al., 2012; Dhir, 2015; Chen, 2016; Jacob, 2017; Sianturi et al., 2018). For example, after 
several years of implementing a range of improvement strategies in Australia, the percentage 
attainment rates of Indigenous students in terms of literacy (Department of the Prime Minister and 
Cabinet, 2018; Riley & Webster, 2016) and numeracy performance lag behind those of non-Indigenous 
students (Department of the Prime Minister and Cabinet, 2018).  

The literature also notes that the literacy and numeracy performances of Indigenous West Papuan 
students are lower than non-Papuan students (BPS, 2020; Sianturi et al., 2018). According to the latest 
report on schooling annual evaluation in 2019, junior high and senior high school students' average 
scores on compulsory subjects were below the national average (Kementrian Pendidikan dan 
Kebudayaan [Indonesian Ministry of Education and Culture] (Kemendikbud), 2019). Indigenous West 
Papua students in Merauke Regency have the lowest scores in mathematics out of all the subjects 
offered in schools (Kemendikbud, 2019). Based on a report from previous research, it was discovered 
that West Papuan students in higher education had only a 43 out of 100 score on the basic 
mathematical ability test (Sianturi & Suryani, 2017). 



 529 

The mathematics academic achievement gap between Indigenous and non-Indigenous students has 
become a worldwide issue. The ongoing legacy of colonisation that persistently has impacts on 
Western mainstream education, which numerically assesses the academic success of Indigenous from 
colonial lenses, and the global neoliberal views that prioritise standardised scores, perpetuates placing 
these students in a marginalised position (Rigney et al., 2020;  Sianturi, Chiang, et al., 2022). Despite 
the constant educational disadvantage faced by Indigenous students, there remains a limited 
understanding of the source of the educational gap and of ways to improve Indigenous students' 
outcomes and close the gap.  

Although governments and schools have attempted to increase the students' learning achievement, 
the attempts seem to achieve less. For example, the Indonesian government strives to enhance Papuan 
students' academic success, making them equivalent to their Indonesian peers, however, what the 
government has undertaken provides limited space for Papuan students to multiply their potential 
(Rahmawati, 2004; Sianturi, Chiang, et al., 2022). Why? Because these efforts are more likely to 
assimilate them into a community different from them (Sianturi, Chiang, et al., 2022). This community 
is inaccessible and culturally divisive (Yunkaporta, 2009). If the focus of the government and teachers 
is only on how to make the scores of Indigenous students equal to non-Indigenous students, then the 
divide will never be closed. Only if all parties can see the uniqueness and distinctive characteristics of 
Indigenous students and their needs, and set aside the intention of equating all students with a 
standardised value, then the conceptualisations of knowledge and learning will be more inclusive 
(Munroe et al., 2013; Perso, 2012). This would encourage teachers to focus on efforts to understand 
students and dig deeper into their potential and the way they think and learn so that teachers will be 
able to design lessons that are appropriate and accessible for them (Hogg & Volman, 2020; Sianturi et 
al., 2018). 

One of students’ characteristics seen as influencing the success of students' mathematical performance 
that teachers might consider is cognitive style. Several studies have shown that there is a significant 
correlation between the classification of students' cognitive styles on their mathematics learning 
outcomes (Azlina et al., 2018; Jantan, 2014; Nur & Palobo, 2018). That is, the classification of certain 
cognitive styles of students affects them in approaching mathematics, which leads to their success in 
learning that subject. Considering the low performance of Indigenous West Papuan students in 
mathematics, our study focused on identifying the relationship between their mathematics academic 
outcomes, cognitive styles, and gender.  

Literature Review 
Cognitive Styles and Indigenous Students’ Ways of Learning 

Cognitive styles refer to the terminology used to describe an individual’s unique way of receiving and 
processing information (Umaru, 2013; Witkin et al., 1977). Individuals may proceed with cognition 
differently, including attention, memory, awareness, understanding, perception, and reasoning 
(Macneil, 1980; Umaru, 2013). Although cognitive styles are popular in psychological disciplines, 
cognitive styles have become expansive in educational fields. A growing literature has shown the 
relevance of cognitive styles to students’ learning process (Jantan, 2014; Singer et al., 2017; Umaru, 
2013), in which they are considered a determining element of students’ uniqueness in perceiving and 
managing information from the environment (Kozhevnikov, 2007; Singer et al., 2017).  



 530 

 

Cognitive styles are distinct from cognitive ability, which is often measured by intelligence tests 
(Umaru, 2013; Witkin et al., 1977). Although cognitive style and cognitive ability probably influence 
learning practice performance, several cognitive styles may match some practices more than others 
and vice versa (Kozhevnikov, 2007; Riding & Sadler-Smith, 1997; Singer et al., 2017; Witkin et al., 
1977). Cognitive styles reflect qualitative rather than quantitative differences between individuals in 
their thinking processes (Riding & Sadler-Smith, 1997; Vega-Vaca & Hederich-Martínez, 2015; Witkin 
et al., 1977). While cognitive ability emphasises students’ intellectual level more, cognitive style is 
more about students’ ways of thinking, their steady mode of intellectual and perceptual functioning 
(Umaru, 2013), and their attitudes toward interpreting situations and resolving matters in mind 
(Desmita, 2012; Umaru, 2013). The students’ preferred method of organising information leads them 
to develop knowledge (Witkin et al., 1977), enables combination in a cross-ability and personality, and 
ultimately manifests it in several activities (Desmita, 2012).   

Witkin et al. (1977) studied individuals' ways of thinking profoundly and discovered two 
classifications of cognitive styles: field-independent (FI) and field-dependent (FD). The characteristics of a 
person classified as FI are: 1) proceeding with information in part patterns and analytic ways and 
having a tendency to one’s own self-defined purpose; 2) feeling challenged to learn and comprehend 
social information and contents and, therefore, requiring certain assistance (Blanton, 2004; Witkin et 
al., 1977);  3) developing inductive reasoning to interpret inputs (Saville-Troike, 2012) one’s own 
structures in unstructured situations (Blanton, 2004; Witkin et al., 1977), and feeling efficient working 
alone (Vega-Vaca & Hederich-Martínez, 2015; Volkova & Rusalov, 2016); 4) not being influenced by 
criticism and more introverted (Vega-Vaca & Hederich-Martínez, 2015); 5) tackling problems without 
explicit guidance and instructions (Witkin et al., 1977). Meanwhile, characteristics of a person 
classified as FD are: 1) proceeding with information in a whole pattern and defined structures and 
having a tendency to require reinforcement; 2) showing more interest in social contents and, therefore, 
memorising social information well (Blanton, 2004; Witkin et al., 1977); 3) developing deductive 
reasoning to interpret inputs (Saville-Troike, 2012), having difficulty with structured learning material 
(Blanton, 2004; Witkin et al., 1977), and enjoying working in a team (Volkova & Rusalov, 2016); 4) 
being influenced by criticism and more extroverted (Vega-Vaca & Hederich-Martínez, 2015); 5) 
needing clear instruction mnemonics or metaphors to solve problems (Blanton, 2004). 

Research in Indigenous education suggests recognising Indigenous children’s ways of learning 
(Krakouer, 2015; Yunkaporta, 2009). How Indigenous students think is distinct from non-Indigenous 
students. For instance, Harris (1984) asserted that Australian Aboriginal students have unique 
elements of learning that are the same as other non-Aboriginal students have. While non-Aboriginal 
pupils get used to school-based learning, Aboriginal children prefer informal learning. However, 
many parties, even the teachers themselves, do not understand this difference, thus generalising the 
learning approaches between Indigenous and non-Indigenous students (Hughes & More, 1997). 
Yunkaporta (2009) continued similar research and found eight Indigenous ways of learning: 1) 
Deconstruct/Reconstruct: scaffolding and framing, working from a whole pattern to parts; 2) Learning 
Maps: explicitly visualising processes; 3) Community Links: embracing local perspectives and learning 
from the community; 4) Symbols and Images: utilising images, metaphors, and symbols to understand 
concepts; 5) Non-verbal: using intra-personal and kinaesthetic skills to think, organise information, and 



 531 

learn; 6) Land Links: place-based learning, linking content to local land and environment; 7) Story 
Sharing: approaching learning through narrative; 8) Non-linear: producing innovations and 
understanding by thinking laterally or combining systems (pp. 46-50). 

The concepts Yunkaporta (2009) discussed resonate with the FD classification criteria — there are 
some commonalities. Evidence corroborates that some Indigenous groups (e.g., Native American and 
Australian Aboriginal learners) tend to be more FD (Pewewardy, 2002; Rasmussen et al., 2004). 
However, it should be noted that this prevalence is not general for all Indigenous peoples; even within 
one group, the tendency to be FD cannot be generalised (Rasmussen et al., 2004). This is further 
explained by Kozhevnikov (2007), that although cognitive styles tend to be identical and stable in each 
individual, they are not consistent over time because changes may occur in line with responses to 
specific environmental situations. 

Cognitive Styles and Mathematics Learning  

There is a link between students’ cognitive styles and their mathematical achievement (Jantan, 2014).  
This positive correlation has drawn several researchers’ interest to its profound impacts on more 
exceptional mathematical skills: creativity (Azlina et al., 2018; Singer et al., 2017), problem-solving 
(Ademola, 2015; Faradillah et al., 2018), mathematical communication (Pratiwi et al., 2013) and 
particular subject areas, such as geometry (Singer et al., 2017) and algebraic reasoning tasks 
(Chrysostomou et al., 2013; Rosita, 2018). These studies’ findings underscored the importance of 
delving in more substantial depth into how cognitive styles influence mathematics learning. Singer et 
al. (2017) considered cognitive style a good predictor of students' mathematical creativity in a 
comprehensive analysis. Some of these studies also provided recommendations for the idea of 
dynamic balance in teaching mathematics by embracing students' cognitive styles (Chrysostomou et 
al., 2013; Singer et al., 2017). 

Cognitive Styles and Gender 

The literature has recognised the relationship between gender and mathematics performance 
(Goodchild & Grevholm, 2009; Lin et al., 2020; Munroe, 2016). There is a higher performance rate 
among males in mathematics than among females. During the mathematics learning process, 
compared to their male peers, girls tend to exhibit a less positive attitude towards mathematics 
(Munroe, 2016). For questions in which they are uncertain of the answer, girls are less likely to take a 
risk (Goodchild & Grevholm, 2009). The discrepancy also appears in ways they approach 
mathematical problems. When determining the solutions process and presenting their results, boys 
are more likely to use diagrams, while girls prefer to utilise traditional methods and narratives 
(Munroe, 2016). 

It is evident that there is a tendency for males to perform better on mathematics tests than females, 
when it comes to learning mathematics, and according to Arnup et al. (2013), these discrepancies 
might be partly explained by the differences in the cognitive styles of the individuals. Their study 
involving 190 primary school pupils aged between 8 and 11 years reported the relationship between 
cognitive styles, mathematics performance, and gender. Based on the results presented, there was a 
difference in the way boys and girls perform mathematics based on their cognitive styles. It was found 
that there was a significant interaction between gender and cognitive styles in the context of 



 532 

mathematics learning. Compared to girls with the FI classification, boys with that classification 
performed significantly better. 

Moreover, Pathuddin et al. (2019) specifically studied the characteristics of male students with 
different cognitive styles in solving mathematics problems. Boys with the FI cognitive style were 
typically capable of reshaping and connecting new information to the knowledge they already 
possessed and organised in resolving problems. Contrary, it is difficult for boys with the FD 
classification to solve problems effectively as they feel challenged to organise their approach and 
connect new information to their existing knowledge. 

These previous studies focused more on the relations to mathematics performance, cognitive styles, 
and gender among students in general. Although findings of studies that explored the prevalence of 
Indigenous students’ cognitive classifications were presented in the previous discussion, those studies 
did not examine the relationships to mathematics academic performance, cognitive styles, and gender. 
Therefore, a study that identifies the relationships between mathematical performance, cognitive 
styles, and gender among Indigenous students is considered necessary. Moreover, this is because 
Indigenous students have different ways of learning from students in general. 

Research Objectives and Questions 
This research profoundly investigated the relationships between Indigenous students’ cognitive 
styles, gender, and mathematics academic outcomes. This study is critical to help teachers develop 
teaching strategies more relevant to Indigenous students' characteristics and uniqueness and is 
accessible and inclusive for Indigenous students. This study adds to the literature on cognitive styles 
and Indigenous students' mathematical skills. The following research questions (RQs) were used to 
accomplish the objective of this study: 

RQ1: Is there a significant association between gender and Indigenous students’ cognitive style 
classification? 

RQ2: Is there a significant relationship between cognitive styles and mathematics academic 
outcomes among Indigenous West Papuan students? 

Research Framework 
This study was structured according to the research framework illustrated in Figure 1. In the present 
study, the relationship between cognitive styles, genders, and mathematics academic outcomes was 
examined among Indigenous West Papuan students who were enrolled in this study. There are three 
main pieces of information required in order to identify such a relationship, including gender, 
cognitive style classification, and mathematics academic outcomes. This study first examined whether 
or not there is a relationship between gender and cognitive styles. We identified if Indigenous West 
Papua students' gender affects their prevalence of cognitive style. The relationship between cognitive 
style classification and mathematics academic outcomes was sequentially investigated. Through 
correlation analysis, we intended to understand if the prevalence of the cognitive style of Indigenous 
West Papua students affected their mathematics academic outcomes. 

 

 



 533 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 
Figure 1: Research Framework 

Methods 
Research Methodology 

This study examined the relationship between cognitive styles, genders, and mathematics academic 
outcomes among Indigenous West Papuan students. In case-study research methods, researchers can 
explore the data closely within the context of a particular situation or issue in order to assess its 
relevance (Zainal, 2007). Typically, a small number of individuals, or limited geographical areas, are 
chosen as the subjects of a case study  (Creswell, 2014; Zainal, 2007). There is a weakness that a case 
study may not have a broad application, due to the fact the statistical requirements may not be met 
when analyzing small data sets. If that is the case, case studies could be limited in their application 
(Davies & Beaumont, 2011). There is no one particular tradition of social scientific research that 
inspired case studies. "Case study is defined by individual cases, not by the methods of inquiry used" 
(Stake, 1994, 236). Depending on the nature of the case, it can be complex or simple (Stake, 1994). It is 
also possible for case studies to be quantitative and/or qualitative. Using quantitative analysis, case 
studies can include both empirical and analytical elements (Mills et al., 2010).  

Because our study concentrated on a small group of Indigenous West Papuan students inhabit in the 
Merauke area who had a specific case in terms of low mathematics performance, we considered our 
study a simple case study using quantitative analysis, as we discussed in the previous section 
regarding the low mathematics performance of Indigenous West Papuan students at different school 
levels. Depending on each child's age and grade level, as well as the academic area in which the child 
was studying, the role of cognitive style in relation to academic achievement could vary (Saracho, 
1988). We were interested in identifying how the cognitive styles differ for each level. Research on 
cognitive styles using GEFT is generally intended for individuals or groups of young people over ten 
years old, particularly research that specifically discusses the area of mathematics. Therefore, in our 

Relationship 
between 

Gender and 
Cognitive Style 

Relationship 
between 

Cognitive Style 
and 

Mathematics 
Academic 
Outcomes 

 

Indigenous 
West Papuan 

Student 

GEFT 
Score 

Cognitive 
Style 

Gender 

Field 
Independent

t 

Field 
Dependent 

Mathematics 
Academic Outcomes 



 534 

study, we intended to examine these three different levels (i.e., junior and senior high schools, and 
university). 

Population and Sample 

Although, in order to sample cases in a quantitative study it is necessary to have some understanding 
of both the cases and the populations within which they are drawn, Thompson (1999) and Uprichard 
(2013) asserted that we need to know what kinds of methods are most appropriate to deal with certain 
cases. Some cases in quantitative studies employ non-probability (non-random) sampling for some 
consideration (Vehovar et al., 2016). As a way of optimising response rates, non-probability sampling 
is also introduced in quantitative research (Asiamah et al., 2022). Non-probability sampling methods 
are often used by researchers for practical reasons. There are specific criteria that are used to select 
non-probability samples, such as: practicality (data can be collected more easily and efficiently 
through a sample); necessity (not all populations can be studied because they are too large or 
inaccessible); cost-effectiveness (there is expense involved — equipment, travel, etc.) and a reduction 
in participant enrollment manageability (using smaller datasets, the process of storing and conducting 
statistical analyses is easier and more reliable) (Asiamah et al., 2022; Uprichard, 2013; Vehovar et al., 
2016). This sampling method will help particularly early career epistemologists who might have to 
consider cost-effectiveness (since many of them might not receive funding or sponsorship), conduct 
feasible studies and obtain an understanding of reporting their practices plausibly (Asiamah et al., 
2022). To bridge tensions between methodological and practical activities (Uprichard, 2013), 
researchers must determine the limits of inference (Asiamah et al., 2022) and whether or not it is 
intended to generalise population. As long as there is researcher awareness of the epistemological 
perspectives they adopt, the door is opened to a set of other discussions. 

Although this study employed a quantitative approach, some practical consideration was made to 
select non-random sampling. As early-career researchers, cost-effectiveness, practicality, and necessity 
were the most reasonable justification that we took into account. Therefore, it is less likely that we 
could make statistically valid inferences about the population at large based on nonrandom selection 
methods rather than based on a probability sample.  

Our study focused on a group of Indigenous West Papuan students who lived in the Merauke District 
and who had a specific case in terms of low mathematics performance. The number of Indigenous 
Papuan people is decreasing compared to non-Indigenous, and, in general, Indigenous Papuans live 
in remote areas, where access to them is quite difficult and costly. Therefore, we identified schools and 
universities that we could reach, as well as classes and majors that have a fairly large proportion of 
Indigenous Papuan students. We decided to select two schools and one department of a local 
university. For confidentiality purposes, the names of the schools and university were anonymous. 
After discussing with homeroom teachers at each school and the head of the Department of Primary 
Teacher Education, we recruited the seventh grade for the junior high school, the eleventh grade for 
the senior high school and the second- and third-year students for the university. All participants 
were Indigenous West Papuans (n = 67), in which n = 9 were junior high school, n = 12 senior high 
school, and n = 46 university students (Table 1). 

 

 



 535 

Table 1: Demography of Participants 

Criteria 
Junior High 

School Senior High School University n 
Male 
Female 
n 

4 
5 
9 

8 
4 
12 

15 
31 
46 

27 
40 
21 

Tool Used 

A group embedded figure test (GEFT) was given to gather information about students’ cognitive 
styles. GEFT is a set of psychometric tests developed by Witkin et al. (1977) and commonly used to 
classify individuals as FI or FD learners. GEFT takes 15 minutes to complete. It consists of three 
subcategories: subcategory I (a part of training, three minutes to complete), which consists of seven 
items; subcategories II and III (main parts, six minutes to complete for each), respectively, comprise 
nine items − one point for each correct answer and zero for the wrong answer. The maximum total 
score is 18, and the minimum is zero. If a GEFT taker gains a total score in the range of 0 to 9, he/she is 
classified as FD, while in the range 10-18, he/she is classified as FI (Kamaruddin et al., 2004).  

The GEFT is a standardised test and has been tested across a wide range of cultures to ensure its 
reliability and validity. Since the GEFT was published, some scholars have tested and employed it, 
and it shows a strong correlation ranging from .60 to .90 (Fyle, 2009; Jantan, 2014; Lis & Powers, 1979; 
Panek et al., 1980; Witkin et al., 1971). This correlation remains strong even when it is translated in 
Indonesian (Kamaruddin et al., 2004;  Nugraha & Awalliyah, 2016; Puspananda & Suriyah, 2017) and 
employed in the Merauke Regency (Nur & Palobo, 2018; Nur & Nurvitasari, 2017). Puspananda and 
Suriyah (2017) in their research on factor analysis on the GEFT to measure cognitive styles found that 
the GEFT instrument has one dominant dimension, namely, cognitive styles. The validity and 
reliability show the internal consistency of the GEFT, and, therefore, GEFT is ready to use 
(Puspananda & Suriyah, 2017). This consideration led our study to use a translated version of GEFT 
(Kamaruddin et al., 2004;  Nur & Nurvitasari, 2017) without testing it. 

Data Collection and Analysis 

There were two kinds of data gathered in this study, one was the information about students' 
cognitive style classifications, and another was students’ mathematics learning achievements. 
Students’ cognitive styles were collected from the GEFT results of each student. As this study is a 
continuation of previous research (Sianturi, Suliantin, et. al., 2022), where the cognitive style 
classifications of the junior and secondary high school students in that study were used for this study. 
We distributed GEFT to university students (Appendix A). 

Students’ mathematics academic outcomes for junior and secondary high school levels were gathered 
from mathematics academic scores from the latest semester. Meanwhile, the mathematics academic 
outcomes of university students were collected from their grades in their geometry and measurement 
course. All scores here use percentage grading systems with the scale of 0-100.   

We employed Fisher’s exact test value, independent t-test, and Nonparametric Correlation (Spearman 
Rho) assisted by SPSS Statistic 26 to analyse the data (Creswell, 2014; Creswell & Guetterman, 2019). 
Fisher’s exact test value was utilised to determine whether there is a significant association between 



 536 

gender and students’ cognitive style classification (RQ1). Meanwhile, independent t-test, and 
Nonparametric Correlation (Spearman Rho) were employed to identify whether there is a significant 
relationship between cognitive styles and mathematics academic outcomes among Indigenous West 
Papuan students (RQ2). 

Ethical Clearance 

Ethical approval was granted from the local ethics committees of the local university, the head of the 
department, and the principals of each school. All participants provided informed consent. The names 
of each school and university were kept anonymous for confidentiality purposes.  

Results  
Gender Preferences  

As our data sample sizes were too small and data violated an assumption for Chi-Square tests, we 
used exact Fisher’s exact test value to discover whether the number of FI and FD learners was 
associated with the gender of the students. Table 2 shows that the p-value of 1.000 (for junior high 
school and university students) and the p-value of .333 (for senior high school students) were greater 
than .05. Thus, we concluded there was no significant association between gender and students’ 
cognitive style classifications (FI and FD).  

Table 2: Cognitive Style Classifications and Gender 

Seventh Grade Value df 

Asymptotic 
Significance (2-
sided) 

Exact Sig. (2-
sided) 

Exact Sig. (1-
sided) 

Pearson Chi-Square .032a 1 .858   
Continuity correction .000 1 1.000   
Likelihood ratio .032 1 .858   
Fisher’s exact Test    1.000 .722 
Linear-by-linear Association .029 1 .866   
N of Valid cases 9     
Eleventh Grade      
Pearson Chi-Square 2.182b 1 .140   
Continuity correction .136 1 .712   
Likelihood ratio 2.385 1 .122   
Fisher’s exact Test    .333 .333 
Linear-by-linear Association 2.000 1 .157   
N of Valid cases 12     
University students      
Pearson Chi-Square 1.012c 1 .314   
Continuity correction .055 1 .814   
Likelihood ratio 1.622 1 .203   
Fisher’s exact Test    1.000 .499 
Linear-by-linear Association .99 1 .320   
N of Valid cases 12     
a. 4 cells (100.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is .89.  
b. 3 cells (75.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is .33. 
c.

 2 cells (50.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is .65. 

 
 



 537 

Cognitive Styles and Learning Achievement 

Table 3 provides the relationship between GEFT results and student mathematics academic outcomes. 
For junior high school students, it shows p-values = .412 (p > .05) and Spearman correlation coefficient 
(r) = .313 and for senior high school students, p-values = .181 (p > .05) and Spearman correlation 
coefficient (r) = .414. Differently, for university students, p-values = .002 (p <. 01) and Spearman 
correlation coefficient (r) = .438.  
Table 3: Correlation between Mathematics Learning Achievement and GEFT Scores 

Criteria  Junior High School  Senior High School University 
 Mean SD r p* Mean SD r p* Mean SD r p** 

Learning 
achievement 62.44 5.28 .313 .412 37.67 8.08 .414 .181 

 
60.33 
 

 
19.70 

 
.438 

 
.002 

 
GIFT  4.33 4.12 

 
  3.33 2.77 

 
 

 
  1.72 3.17 

  

             
n 9    12    46    

*95% confidence level 

**Significance at the .01 level 

Meanwhile, students’ cognitive styles did not significantly affect either junior high school or senior 
high school students’ learning outcomes; they significantly affected the learning outcomes of the 
university students. Although the cognitive styles of junior high school and senior high school 
students positively affected their learning outcomes, the correlation was small.  

Table 4: The Effect of Cognitive Styles on Mathematics Academic Outcomes 

Criteria  
Junior High 
School  Senior High School University 

 F pa t p* F pa t p* F pa t p* 

Math outcomes 2.815 .137 .768 .472 b b 2.609 .026 2.5222 

 

.119 

 

2.213 .032 

FI/FD             
a Levene’s test, variance is homogenous at 95% confidence level 
b Levene's test is not computed because there are less than two non-empty groups. 

*Significance at the .05 level 

Prior to using independent t-test, we tested whether the data were normally distributed. The results of 
normality test for each level showed p-values = .151 (p < .05), p-values = .200 (p < .05), p-values = .000 
(p < .05), which means all data distributed normally and independent t-test could be applied 
(Appendix B). The effect of cognitive style classifications on mathematics academic outcomes is 
described in Table 4. It showed that p-values =.472 (p > .05), p-values =.026 (p < .05), and p-values 
=.032 (p < .05), for each junior high school, senior high school, and university students, in order. The 
differences in FI or FD classifications only positively influenced mathematics academic outcomes of 



 538 

the senior high school and university students. If we compare the correlation value of junior high 
school, senior high school, and university students and the description in Table 4, we discover that 
students' cognitive styles provide greater influence as students age. Students with the FI classification 
were more likely to have better learning outcomes than students with the FD classification, in line 
with their ages. Table 5 shows that p-values = .014 (p <. 01) and Spearman correlation coefficient (r) = 
.369. It shows that the relationship between the FD and mathematics outcomes of university students 
was significantly correlated.  

Table 5: Relationship between Field-Dependent and Mathematics Outcomes of University Students 

Variable r p**  
Math outcomes .369 .014  

FD      

**Significance at the .01 level 

Discussion  
In previous and recent research, it has been noted that cognitive styles have a positive connection with 
many cognitive attributes (Umaru, 2013; Volkova & Rusalov, 2016) and mathematics (Chrysostomou 
et al., 2013; Jantan, 2014; Rahman, 2013; Singer et al., 2017; Suranata et al., 2019). However, our 
findings support a slightly different point of view. This study’s findings revealed that Indigenous 
students’ cognitive styles, both junior high school and senior high school, did not significantly 
correlate with students' mathematics learning outcomes. Although the correlation was positive, the 
cognitive styles showed a small contribution to supporting Indigenous students in achieving their 
learning outcomes. Meanwhile, higher education students' cognitive styles significantly influenced 
their learning outcomes. However, it was seen that the positive correlation became more noticeable as 
the students’ grade level increased. A small correlation for younger students was also discovered by 
Jantan (2014), where cognitive styles were more likely to influence students’ outcomes as the child 
matures.  

This may occur because of the way teachers deliver mathematics material that is not relevant to the 
students’ environment. Yunkaporta (2009) argues that educators often focus on linear viewpoints, a 
part of Western-oriented pedagogy, within school practices, which further marginalises Indigenous 
students by preventing them from reconstructing their own knowledge. The learning outcomes 
collected in this study were analysed together with GEFT scores that were mathematics academic 
outcomes measured according to standard scores. Given the small contribution of cognitive styles to 
mathematics academic outcomes for younger students, this may be acceptable. A study involving 
Papuan students aged 10-12 years by Sianturi et al. (2018) also found that the delivery of learning 
materials for Papuan students was often monopolised by the dominant culture, making it difficult for 
students to grasp mathematics learning. Furthermore, another study stated that the education 
implemented in many schools in West Papua attempted to alienate Papuan students from their 
environment and culture (Sianturi, Chiang, et al., 2022).  

However, it should be noted that this needs to be studied with a qualitative approach. The aim is to 
see how Indigenous students' ways of learning, culture, the environment, and even their gender, 
affects Indigenous Papuans' cognitive styles prevalence and the ways students capture information 



 539 

conveyed during the mathematics learning process. Previous studies that discovered a positive 
correlation between cognitive styles and mathematical achievement involved students in general (e.g., 
Ademola, 2015; Azlina et al., 2018; Chrysostomou et al., 2013; Faradillah et al., 2018; Pratiwi et al., 
2013; Rosita, 2018; Singer et al., 2017). Thus, more similar studies involving Indigenous students’ 
mathematics learning are required. Topics about the relevance of Indigenous ways of learning and 
cognitive styles to students' mathematical abilities, but not taken from standardised mathematics 
outcomes, might be worth considering for further research. 

Other than the insignificant differences between cognitive style and learning outcomes against 
previous studies' results (Scholar & Abdurauf, 2015), our results also identified slight differences 
according to gender. Our research results indicated no significant association between Indigenous 
students' gender and cognitive style classification. In other words, no matter whether an Indigenous 
West Papuan student was a boy or a girl, his or her cognitive style would not be affected. However, 
we could not articulate this statement strongly because of the limited number of participants we 
studied.  

Conclusion and Recommendations 
This study sought the relationship between cognitive styles and mathematics academic outcomes 
among Indigenous West Papuan students. There is no significant correlation between the Indigenous 
students’ cognitive styles and mathematics academic outcomes at the junior and senior high school 
level. Indigenous students with different cognitive styles might show a small improvement in 
accomplishing their learning outcomes. As Indigenous students pursue a higher level of education, 
their cognitive styles will influence their mathematics academic outcomes.  

The findings of this study provide recommendations for school practices and policymakers. Teachers 
should recognise Indigenous students' cognitive styles when developing teaching instructions and 
assessments. Knowing Indigenous students’ diverse characteristics and needs might help teachers 
create more culturally appropriate lessons for students. As cognitive styles might influence students’ 
mathematics academic outcomes as their school level increase, teachers might consider their cognitive 
classifications for each school level.  

The results of the study should be viewed within the context of several limitations. Considering the 
small number of participants and the fact that we employed non-probability sampling in this study, it 
is not possible to generalise the results of this study. Thus, a similar procedure with a larger sample 
size could be applied for future research with probability sampling. Studies using qualitative 
approaches to find the relationship among Indigenous students' ways of learning, cognitive styles, 
gender, culturally responsive approaches, and their impacts on mathematical performance might be 
considered for future research.  

Acknowledgements: We are thankful for all students who dedicated their time and energy to take part in this 
research. We also express our gratitude to the homeroom teachers and principals of the junior and senior high 
schools, and the head of the Department of Primary Teacher education who supported and facilitated this 
research. The meaningful input and feedback from two anonymous peer-reviewers and colleagues that 
improved this article is also highly appreciated. 

 



 540 

Funding: There was no financial support received by the authors from any organisation for the work submitted 
for publication. 

Disclosure Statement: The authors reported no potential conflict of interest. 

References 
Abdullah, R. Bin, Mamat, W. H. W., Amir Zal, W. A., & Ibrahim, A. M. Bin. (2013). Teaching and learning 

problems of the Orang Asli Education: Students’ perspective. Asian Social Science. 
https://doi.org/10.5539/ass.v9n12p118 

Ademola, K. (2015). Predicting academic success of junior secondary school students in Mathematics through 
cognitive style and problem solving technique. Journal of Education and Practice, 6(4), 72-79. 

Arnup, J. L., Murrihy, C., Roodenburg, J., & McLean, L. A. (2013). Cognitive style and gender differences in 
children’s mathematics achievement. Educational Studies, 39(3), 355–368. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/03055698.2013.767184 

Asiamah, N., Mensah, H. K., & Oteng-Abayie, E. F. (2022). Non-probabilistic sampling in quantitative clinical 
research: A typology and highlights for students and early career researchers. International Journal of Applied 
Research on Public Health Management, 7(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.4018/IJARPHM.290379 

Azlina, N., Amin, S. M., & Lukito, A. (2018). Creativity of field-dependent and field-independent students in 
posing mathematical problems. Journal of Physics: Conference Series, 947(1), 1-6. https://doi.org/10.1088/1742-
6596/947/1/012031 

Blanton, E. L. (2004). The influence of students’ cognitive style on a standardized reading test administered in three 
different formats. The University of Central Florida Orlando. 

Burridge, N., Whalan, F., & Vaughan, K. (2012). Indigenous education: A learning journey for teachers, schools 
and communities. In Indigenous Education: A Learning Journey for Teachers, Schools and Communities. 
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-6091-888-9 

Chen, S. (2016). Dawning of hope: Practice of and reflections on indigenous teacher education in Taiwan. Policy 
Futures in Education, 14(7), 943-955. https://doi.org/10.1177/1478210316656312 

Chrysostomou, M., Pitta-pantazi, D., Tsingi, C., Cleanthous, E., & Christou, C. (2013). Examining number sense 
and algebraic reasoning through cognitive styles. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 83, 205-223. 
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-012-9448-0 

Chung, C., Huan, H., Tseng, K., Jung, R., & Chen, C. (2021). Transformation from traditional schools to 
alternative schools: Curriculum leadership of the principals of Taiwanese aborigines. Asia Pacific Education 
Review, 22(1), 53-66. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12564-020-09663-9 

Creswell, J. W. (2014). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches (4th ed.). SAGE 
Publications, Ltd. 

Creswell, J. W., & Guetterman, T. C. (2019). Educational research: Planning, conducting, and evaluating quantitative 
and qualitative research (6th ed.). Pearson. 

Davies, M., & Beaumont, T. J. (2011). Case studies: Research methods. In Study Skills for International 
Postgraduates (pp. 1-7). The University of Melbourne. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-230-34553-9 

Department of the Prime Minister and Cabinet. (2018). Closing the gap: Prime Minister’s Report 2018. 
https://www.pmc.gov.au/sites/default/files/reports/closing-the-gap-2018/employment.html 

Desmita, O. (2012). Psikologi Perkembangan Peserta Didik [Developmental Psychology of Students]. PT. Rosdakarya. 
Dhir, R. K. (2015). Indigenous peoples in the world of work in Asia and the Pacific. International Labour Organization. 

www.ilo.org/publns 



 541 

Faradillah, A., Hadi, W., & Tsurayya, A. (2018). Pre-service mathematics teachers ’ reasoning ability in solving 
mathematical non-routine problem according to cognitive style. Journal of Physics: Conference Series, 948(1), 1–
6. 

Fyle, C. O. (2009). The effects of Field Dependent/Independent style awareness on learning strategies and outcomes in an 
instructional hypermedia module. Florida State University. 

Goodchild, S., & Grevholm, B. (2009). An exploratory study of mathematics test results: What is the gender 
effect? International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 7, 161-182. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-
007-9114-7 

Harris, S. (1984). Aboriginal learning styles and formal schooling. The Aboriginal Child at School, 12(4), 3–23. 
Harrison, N. (2011). Teaching and learning in Aboriginal education (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. 
Hogg, L., & Volman, M. (2020). A synthesis of funds of identity research: Purposes, tools , pedagogical 

approaches, and outcomes. Review of Educational Research, 90(6), 862–895. 
https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654320964205 

Hughes, P., & More, A. J. (1997). Aboriginal ways of learning learning styles. Annual Conference of the Australian 
Association for Research in Education, 1-21. 

Jacob, M. M. (2017). Indigenous studies speaks to American sociology: The need for individual and social 
transformations of Indigenous education in the USA. Social Sciences, 7(1), 1–10. 
https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci7010001 

Jantan, R. B. (2014). Relationship between students’ cognitive style (Field-Dependent and Field-Independent 
cognitive styles) with their mathematic achievement in primary school. International Journal of Humanities 
Social Sciences and Education (IJHSSE), 1(10), 88–93. www.arcjournals.org 

Kamaruddin, M. I. Bin, Bakar, Z. B. A., Surif, J. Bin, & Li, W. S. S. (2004). Relationship between cognitive styles, levels 
of cognitive thinking and chemistry achievement among form four science students. Universiti Teknologi Malaysia. 

Kemendikbud. (2019). Laporan Hasil Ujian Nasional  — Papua [National Examination Results Report - Papua]. 
https://bit.ly/3fbZsYr  

Kozhevnikov, M. (2007). Styles in the context of modern psychology: Toward an integrated framework of 
cognitive style. Psychological Bulletin, 133(3), 464–-481. 

Krakouer, J. (2015). Literature review relating to the current context and discourse on Indigenous cultural 
awareness in the teaching space: Critical pedagogies and improving Indigenous learning outcomes through 
cultural responsiveness. Australian Council for Educational Research, November, 1–21. 
https://research.acer.edu.au/indigenous_education/43/ 

Lin, A. Y., Wu, C., Chen, Z., Ku, P., Lin, Y., Wu, C., Chen, Z., & Ku, P. (2020). How gender pairings affect 
collaborative problem solving in social-learning context. International Forum of Educational Technology & 
Society, 23(4), 30-44. https://www.jstor.org/stable/26981742 

Lis, D. J., & Powers, J. E. (1979). Reliability and validity of the Group Embedded Figures Test for a grade school 
sample. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 48, 660–662. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1979.48.2.660 

Lowe, K. (2017). Walanbaa warramildanha: The impact of authentic Aboriginal community and school 
engagement on teachers’ professional knowledge. Australian Educational Researcher, 44(1), 35–54. 
https://doi.org/10.1007/s13384-017-0229-8 

Macneil, R. D. (1980). The relationship of cognitive style and instructional style to the learning performance of 
undergraduate students. Journal of Educational Research, 73(6), 354-359. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1980.10885265 

Mills, A. J., Durepos, G., & Wiebe, E. (2010). Quantitative analysis in case study. In Encyclopedia of Case Study 
Research (pp. 761-764). SAGE Publications, Inc. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412957397.n279 



 542 

Munroe, E. A., Borden, L. L., Murray Orr, A., Toney, D., & Meader, J. (2013). Decolonizing Aboriginal education 
in the 21st century. McGill Journal of Education, 48(2), 317–337. https://doi.org/10.7202/1020974ar 

Munroe, K. L. (2016). Impact of open approach on students’ understanding of mathematical concepts: A gender 
comparison. Graduate School for International Development and Cooperation, 22(2), 85–96. 
https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Impact-of-Open-Approach-on-Students’-Understanding-
Munroe/a8fcb07b59eb2f7be012929c2d6ed346ff616dbc 

Nugraha, M. G., & Awalliyah, S. (2016). Analisis gaya kognitif field dependent dan field independent terhadap 
penguasaan konsep fisika siswa kelas VII [Field dependent and field independent cognitive style analysis on 
the mastery of physics concepts for class VII students]. Prosiding Seminar Nasional Fisika (E-Journal) SNF2016, 
V(Oktober), 71–76. https://doi.org/10.21009/0305010312 

Nur, A. S., & Nurvitasari, E. (2017). Geometry skill analysis in problem solving reviewed from the difference of 
cognitive style students junior high school. Journal of Educational Science and Technology, 3(3), 205–210. 
https://doi.org/10.26858/est.v3i3.4130 

Nur, Andi Saparudin, & Palobo, M. (2018). Profil kemampuan pemecahan masalah matematika siswa ditinjau 
dari perbedaan gaya kognitif dan gender [Profile of students' mathematical problem solving abilities in 
terms of differences in cognitive style and gender]. Kreano, Jurnal Matematika Kreatif-Inovatif, 9(2), 139-148. 
http://journal.unnes.ac.id/nju/index.php/kreano%0AProfil 

Panek, P. E., Funk, L. G., & Nelson, P. K. (1980). Reliability and validity of the Group Embedded Figures Test 
across the life span. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 117 1-1 174. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1980.50.3c.1171 

Pathuddin, Budayasa, I. K., & Lukito, A. (2019). Metacognitive activity of male students: Difference field 
independent-dependent cognitive style. Journal of Physics: Conference Series, 1218(1), 1-10. 
https://doi.org/10.1088/1742-6596/1218/1/012025 

Perso, T. F. (2012). Cultural responsiveness and school education with particular focus on Australia’s first peoples: A 
review & synthesis of the literature. Menzies School of Health Research, Centre for Child Development and 
Education, Darwin Northern Territory. 

Pewewardy, C. (2002). Learning styles of American Indian/Alaska Native students: A review of the literature 
and implications for practice. Journal of American Indian Education, 41(3), 22–56. 
https://www.jstor.org/stable/24398583 

Pratiwi, D. D., Sujadi, I., & Pangadi. (2013). Kemampuan komunikasi matematis dalam pemecahan masalah 
matematika sesuai dengan gaya kognitif pada siswa kelas IX SMP Negeri 1 Surakarta tahun pelajaran 
2012/2013 [Mathematical communication skills in solving math problems in accordance with the students’ 
cognitive styles at year 9 SMP Negeri 1 Surakarta academic year 2012/2013]. Jurnal Pembelajaran Matematika, 
1(5), 525–538. https://jurnal.fkip.uns.ac.id/index.php/s2math/article/view/3525/2459 

Puspananda, D. R., & Suriyah, P. (2017). Analisis faktor pada group embbeded figures test untuk mengukur 
gaya kognitif [Factor analysis on group embedded figures test to measure cognitive styles]. Seminar 
Matematika Dan Pendidikan Matematika UNY 2017, 225–230. 
http://repository.ikippgribojonegoro.ac.id/id/eprint/1384%0A 

Rahman, A. (2013). Pengajuan masalah matematika ditinjau dari gaya kognitif dan kategori informasi [The 
submission of mathematical problems in terms of cognitive style and information categories]. Jurnal Ilmu 
Pendidikan, 19(1), 244–251. https://doi.org/10.17977/JIP.V19I2.4220 

Rahmawati, A. (2004). Papuan ethno-political conflict: causes, contexts , and policy implication. Naval Postgraduate 
School. 

Rasmussen, C., Sherman, J., & Baydala, L. (2004). Learning patterns and education of Aboriginal children: A 
review of the literature. The Canadian Journal of Native Studies, 24(2), 317-342. 
http://www3.brandonu.ca/cjns/24.2/cjnsv24no2_pg317-342.pdf 



 543 

Riding, R. J., & Sadler-Smith, E. (1997). Cognitive style and learning strategies: Some implications for training 
design. International Journal of Training and Development, 1(3), 199–208. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-
2419.00020 

Rigney, L., Garrett, R., Curry, M., & Macgill, B. (2020). Culturally responsive pedagogy and mathematics through 
creative and body- based learning: Urban Aboriginal schooling. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013124519896861 

Riley, T., & Webster, A. (2016). Principals as Literacy Leaders with Indigenous Communities (PALLIC) building 
relationships: One school’s quest to raise Indigenous learners’ literacy. Teaching Education, 27(2), 136-155. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/10476210.2015.1049261 

Rosita, N. T. (2018). Analysis of algebraic reasoning ability of cognitive style perspectives on field-dependent 
field-independent and gender. Journal of Physics: Conference Series, 983(1). https://doi.org/10.1088/1742-
6596/983/1/012153 

Saracho, O. N. (1988). Cognitive Styles and Young Children’s Learning. Early Child Development and Care, 30(1-4), 
213–220. https://doi.org/10.1080/0300443880300118 

Saville-Troike, M. (2012). Introducing second language acquisition. Cambridge University Press. 
Scholar, T. M., & Abdurauf, R. A. (2015). Cognitive styles Field Dependence / Independence and scientific 

achievement of male and female students of Zamfara State College of Education Maru, Nigeria. Journal of 
Education and Practice, 6(10), 58–64. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1081653.pdf 

Sianturi, M., Chiang, C. L., & Hurit, A. A. (2018). Impact of a place-based education curriculum on Indigenous 
teacher and students. International Journal of Instruction, 11(1), 311–328. 
https://doi.org/10.12973/iji.2018.11122a 

Sianturi, M., Chiang, C. L., & Sumarsono, A. (2022). The preservation of the identity and culture of the 
Indigenous Lanninese people of West Papua, Indonesia, through education (Preservación de la identidad y 
la cultura del pueblo Lani, comunidad indígena de la provincia Indonesia de Papúa Occidental, a través de 
la educación). Culture and Education, 34(2), 424–465. https://doi.org/10.1080/11356405.2022.2034287 

Sianturi, M., & Suryani, D. R. (2017). Performansi matematika dasar siswa Papua calon guru sekolah dasar [The 
basic mathematical performance of Indigenous students in higher education]. In M. Palobo, A. K. Hidayat, 
O. Dadi, & N. R. Marwa (Eds.), Inovasi Pendidikan dan Pembelajaran dalam Rangka Percepatan Pembangunan di 
Daerah Pinggiran [Educational and Learning Innovation in the Context of Accelerating Development in Outlying 
Areas] (pp. 178–190). FKIP Musamus University. 

Sianturi, M., Suliantin, R., & Sianturi, I. A. J. (2022). Culturally relevant teaching: A case study on Indigenous 
students’ cognitive styles and difficulties in processing information. [Manuscript submitted for publication]. 

Singer, F. M., Vouca, C., & Pelczer, I. (2017). Cognitive styles in posing geometry problems: Implications for 
assessment of mathematical creativity. ZDM, 49(1), 37–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-016-0820-x 

Stake, R. E. (1994). Case studies. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of Qualitative Research (pp. 236-
247). Sage Publications. 

Suranata, K., Rangka, I. B., Ifdil, I., Ardi, Z., Dharsana, I. K., Suarni, N. K., & Gading, I. K. (2019). Exploring of 
mathematics learning difficulties for students based on heterogeneous group and cognitive style in 
elementary school. Journal of Physics: Conference Series, 1157, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1088/1742-
6596/1157/3/032091 

Thompson, C. (1999). If you could just provide me with a sample: Examining sampling in qualitative and 
quantitative research papers. Evidence-Based Nursing, 2(3), 68–70. 

Umaru, Y. (2013). Influence of reflective and impulsive cognitive styles on students’ achievement in mathematics 
among senior secondary school students. Ife PsychologIA, 21(2), 123–128. 



 544 

Uprichard, E. (2013). Sampling: Bridging probability and non-probability designs. International Journal of Social 
Research Methodology, 16(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/13645579.2011.633391 

Vega-Vaca, M. L., & Hederich-Martínez, C. (2015). The impact of a cooperative learning program on the 
academic achievement in mathematics and language in fourth grade students and its relation to cognitive 
style. New Approaches in Educational Research, 4(2), 84–90. https://doi.org/10.7821/naer.2015.7.124 

Vehovar, V., Toepoel, V., & Steinmetz, S. (2016). Non-probability sampling. In C. Wolf, D. Joye, T. W. Smith, & 
Y. Fu (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of Survey Methodology (pp. 329–345). SAGE. 

Volkova, E. V., & Rusalov, V. M. (2016). Cognitive styles and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 
99, 266-271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.097 

Witkin, H. A., Moore, C. A., Goodenough, D. R., & Cox, P.  (1977). Field dependent and field-independent 
cognitive styles and their education implications. Review of Educational Research, 47(1), 1-64. 
https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543047001001 

Witkin, H., Oltman, P. K., & Raskin, E. (1971). Manual Embedded Figures Test, Children Embedded Figures Test, Grup 
Embedded Figures Test. Consulting Psychology Press, Inc. 

Yunkaporta, T. K. (2009). Aboriginal pedagogies at the cultural interface [James Cook University]. 
http://eprints.jcu.edu.au/10974/%0A 

Zainal, Z. (2007). Case study as a research method. Jurnal Kemanusiaan, 9(June), 1-6. 
https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/11784113.pdf 

Authors: 
Murni Sianturi is an Indigenous researcher with 7 years’ experience working with Indigenous communities. She 
is currently a Scientia PhD Candidate at the University of New South Wales. She has more intentionally focused 
on issues influencing Indigenous West Papuan students, with a specific interest in mathematics learning, 
identity, and culture. Specifically, building on her experiences as a mathematics teacher at all school levels and a 
local university lecturer, her work is concerned with schooling practices and Indigenous students’ identities, 
impacting their experiences, culture, identities, and learning achievements. Her current research project focuses 
on using technology to facilitate partnerships between schools and Indigenous West Papuan parents. 
(http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2898-7084). Email: m.sianturi@unsw.edu.au 
Riska Sulantin is a mathematics teacher at Muhammadiah Junior High School, Merauke, Papua. Born and 
raised in the remote area of West Papua, Riska has an interest in educating children living in rural areas. In 
addition to her career as a teacher, she is also actively involved in community activities. Email: 
rsuliantin@gmail.com 
Hariani Fitrianti is a lecturer at Musamus University, Papua. Her research interests aim to explore students’ 
mathematical learning and statistical analysis in educational research. Email: harianifitrianti@unmus.ac.id 
 

Cite this paper as: Sianturi, M., Sulantin, R., & Fitrianti, H. (2022). Relationships between cognitive styles and 
Indigenous students’ mathematics academic outcomes. Journal of Learning for Development, 9(3), 528-544.