SEXIST LANGUAGE IN INDONESIAN VIA METAPHOR AND 

TRANSITIVITY: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY 

Luh Putu Laksminy, Gede Primahadi Wijaya Rajeg 

Udayana University 

putu_laksminy@unud.ac.id, primahadi_wijaya@unud.ac.id  

Abstract 

This paper discusses how stereotypes and/or attitudes and positions of women and men are reflected in sexist language, 

focusing on metaphorical sexist expressions and the study of syntactic structures such as transitivity and grammatical 

relations. Based on qualitative analysis of web-based linguistic data and two novels from a female author, we found that 

metaphorical sexist expressions in Indonesian conceptualise women as ANIMALS, COMMODITIES/GOODS, FOODS, 

and DUMPING SITES, indicating negative attitudes towards women. In terms of the transitivity analysis, we illustrate 

that the women character is portrayed as the undergoer of the action enacted by the man-actor. Overall, the findings 

indicate that unfavourable attitudes towards women can still be found in language and that could call for a change in 

language use without discriminating against women. 

Keywords: sexist language, metaphor, transitivity, sociolinguistics 

I INTRODUCTION 

The study of language and gender, according to Meyerhoff (2006, p. 201), is characterised by the 

debate around the pros and cons associated with different ways of conceptualising the relationship 

between language and society. According to Coates (2007, p. 62), gender differences in language use are 

directly related to the relative social power of male speakers and the relative powerlessness of female 

speakers. In social groups, this notion made women to be often seen as a minority while men are the 

majority (Coates, 1986, p. 12); such an imbalance view is known as sexism, a behaviour that maintains 

inequality between men and women, so that sexist attitudes stereotype someone based on gender, rather 

than evaluating individual achievements and abilities (Holmes, 2001, p. 305) (cf. §2.2). This non-

parallel treatment of men and women is a feature present in most languages and language communities 

(Pauwels, 2003, p. 553). Actions that view men as prototypes for human representation led women to a 

status that makes them someone who is “invisible”, “unnatural (marked)”, and “subsumed” (Pauwels, 

2003, p. 553). At the heart of feminist activists and experts is the attention to changing language that is 

discriminatory against women and which seems to downplay women-related activities. A related 

endeavour is to change the way women are represented in advertisements, newspapers and magazines, 

as well as the way they are addressed in texts and in interactions (Mills, 2008). 

In this paper, we discuss and illustrate how stereotypes (see §2.3) and/or attitudes and positions of 

women and men are reflected in language. The rationale of this aim is that until now, the representation 

of women and men is still seen as asymmetrical, which could be reflected in language, which is called 

sexist language (Wareing, 2004, p. 76; Mills, 2008, p. 38; Weatherall, 2002, p. 12; Payne, 2001, p. 112) 

(see §2.4 for details). The aim is in line with Holmes’ (2001, p. 305) assertion that the focus of research 

on sexism (§2.2) and sexist language (§2.4) lies in how language is used to convey negative attitudes 

towards women; the expression evoking such an attitude is also the focus of this paper. However, until 

recently, it was still being debated whether language could actually be sexist towards men, or only 

towards women (Wareing, 2004). 

Eckert and McConnell-Ginet (2003, p. 215) and Holmes (2001, p. 305) note that sexist language is 

often expressed metaphorically. Following up on this, this paper will present data on sexist language 

with metaphorical nuances (§2.5 and §4.1) that evoke semantic derogations for women. In addition to 

metaphorical expressions, the syntactic aspect of transitivity (§2.6 and §4.2) in the use of language in the 

text will also be discussed to show one’s perspective on women in relation to their position with men. 

Through the analysis of what is said and how it is said (R. Lakoff, 1975, p. 1), if she is a woman, or 

talking about women whoever they are, one can gain insight into how a person feels—about herself, 

about women. This is because language conveys attitudes (Holmes, 2001, p. 305). 

mailto:putu_laksminy@unud.ac.id
mailto:primahadi_wijaya@unud.ac.id


2 | Luh Putu Laksminy, Gede Primahadi Wijaya Rajeg Lingual (Vol. 15, No.1, 2023) 

 

II MATERIALS AND METHOD 

2.1 THEORETICAL CONCEPTS 

2.1.1 SEX OR GENDER? 

In sociolinguistics, sex is increasingly limited to the biological and physiological differences 

between men and women (Meyerhoff, 2006, p. 201). According to Eckert and McConnell-Ginet (2003, 

p. 10), and Wareing (2004, p. 76), sex is a biological classification, usually fixed before birth and based 

primarily on reproductive potential. In other words, sex is something that we possess and can be defined 

according to objective and scientific criteria—namely the number of X chromosomes in a person 

(Meyerhoff, 2006, p. 202). 

In contrast, gender denotes a social identity that emerges or is built through social actions 

(Meyerhoff, 2006, p. 201). This is in line with Eckert and McConnell-Ginet’s (2003, p. 10) emphasis 

that “gender is not something we are born with and not something we have, but something we do—

something we perform” (boldface in original). Gender is a socially constructed category based on sex 

and most societies now operate based on two types of gender, namely masculine and feminine (Coates, 

2007, p. 63). Meyerhoff (2006, p. 202) concludes that gender is a social property: something acquired or 

built through our relationships with others and through individual adherence to certain cultural norms 

and restrictions. Gender is a social elaboration of sex, and “gender builds on biological sex” (Eckert & 

McConnell-Ginet, 2003, p. 10). It is worth noting that this paper will not pursue LGBTQ issues any 

further. 

2.1.2 SEXISM 

Mills (2008, pp. 1–2) emphasises that sexism, like racism and other forms of language 

discrimination, is rooted in greater social power, wider power inequalities, and finally, the ongoing 

conflict between men and women. especially in public activities, regarding who has the right to certain 

positions and resources. 

Sexism is not just a statement that seems to constantly focus on gender when it is irrelevant. A 

statement can be considered sexist if it is based on outdated beliefs and stereotypes when it is used to 

refer to certain women (e.g., “Look at you crying over this film”—suggesting women are very 

emotional) (Mills, 2008, p. 2). A further factor for a statement being judged as sexist is when it 

emphasises that male experience is human experience in general, and when the statement is based on the 

presupposition that any activity related to women is secondary to that of men (e.g., “Women tennis 

players get lower prize money at Wimbledon because the game is less exciting”) (Mills, 2008, p. 2). 

From these two examples, it can be seen that none of the linguistic elements (i.e., words) in those two 

expressions is sexist; it is the belief system expressed that is sexist, which sees that women are certainly 

different from and inferior to men (Mills, 2008, pp. 3, 10). 

In sum, it is essential to note that sexism is not only seen from the elements of language but also 

from beliefs or discourses about men and women that are displayed in and conveyed through the 

language (Mills, 2008, p. 3). We ought to be able to distinguish between statements that can be 

classified as sexist (based on stereotypical knowledge or the gender discourse that appears to underlie 

them) and linguistic features that indicate sexism in most contexts (Mills, 2008, p. 11). 

2.1.3 STEREOTYPES 

When talking about sexism, the notion of “stereotype” is often brought up, and it is often assumed 

that we all have access to the same stereotypes. Stereotypes are hypothesised based on structural 

inequalities, although it is important not to generalise too much about stereotypes of men and women (as 

they are formed from specific contexts) (Mills, 2008, p. 126). According to Bourdieu (cited in Mills, 

2008, p. 126), stereotypes can be defined as “a fixed set of characteristics than as a range of possible 

scripts or scenarios (sets of features, roles, and possible narrative sequences) that we hypothesise”. 

Bourdieu’s view suggests that some extreme aspects, which are seen and imagined, from the behaviour 

of several out-group members, are assumed and then these features are generalised to the group. 

Assumptions about stereotypes usually (i) reflect judgments about men and women, and (ii) establish 

notions, often unconsciously, about what is appropriate (Mills, 2008, pp. 126–127). Geeraerts (2008, pp. 

26–27) states that “stereotypes and prototypes alike involve semantic information that is salient within a 

category but that is not sufficient to adequately characterise the category as a whole”. Stereotypes are 

prototypes viewed from a social angle and are social entities (Geeraerts, 2008, p. 27). 



Sexist Language in Indonesian via Metaphor and Transitivity: An Exploratory Study 3 

 

2.1.4 SEXIST LANGUAGE 

Holmes (2001, p. 305) states that sexist language is a medium that can be used by a culture or 

society to convey its values from one group to another, and from generation to generation. Sexist 

language displays the inequality of men and women as if members of one sex seem less completely 

human and less complex and have fewer rights than members of the other sex (Wareing, 2004, p. 76; 

Payne, 2001, p. 112). Sexist language also displays stereotypes of women and men, which sometimes 

harms the men, but more often impacts the women (Wareing, 2004, p. 76; Weatherall, 2002, p. 12). 

Fiksdal (2008, p. 422) indicates that gender differences in language exist in the way men and 

women use linguistic features (e.g., phonology, syntax, metaphors [cf. §2.5 and §4.1]) or certain 

discourse strategies (e.g., turn allocation), amongst others. Moreover, Mills (2008, p. 10) notes that 

sexist language can refer to a wide range of linguistic elements, from (i) generic pronouns (e.g., the 

English pronoun he used to refer to both male and female), (ii) words suffixed with -ette indicating 

female (e.g., usherette), nouns referring to male and female (e.g., landlord and landlady, or manager and 

manageress, which seem to have a range different meanings), (iii) swearing expressions that seem 

different for boys and girls (cf. Ningrum et al., 2021), (iv) names given to us and used for parts of our 

bodies, etc.  

Eckert and McConnell-Ginet (2003, p. 215) and Holmes (2001, p. 305) also state that sexist 

language that conveys negative attitudes towards women is often expressed metaphorically (to be 

explained in §2.5 below and illustrated in §4.1). Much of what is termed ‘sexist language’ is felt to be 

offensive/derogatory because problematic attitudes lie behind metaphorical identifications with women 

and their sexes, for example, commodities, small animals, and with fruit or other desserts (Eckert & 

McConnell-Ginet, 2003, p. 220). 

2.1.5 METAPHOR 

The concept of metaphor in this paper is adopted from the Conceptual Metaphor Theory (CMT) 

(G. Lakoff & Johnson, 1980, 1999; Kövecses, 2010), a contemporary theory of metaphor widely 

recognised in the last 30 years proposing metaphor as a central cognitive process. Contrary to the 

classical view of metaphor, Lakoff and Johnson (1980, 1999) argue that metaphor permeates humans’ 

everyday life, including not only language but also thoughts and actions. Metaphor plays a very 

important role in the human mind as the mechanism to conceptualise the world and its activities (G. 

Lakoff & Johnson, 1980, p. 3; Deignan, 2005, p. 13; Gibbs, 2008, p. 3). As an example, the way we 

understand and talk about abstract concepts such as emotions is largely metaphorical in nature. For 

instance, across many different languages (cf. Kövecses, 2000) including Indonesian (I. M. Rajeg, 2013; 

G. P. W. Rajeg, 2014), anger can be talked about using vocabulary from the domain of fire (e.g., api 

kemarahan berkobar ‘blazing fire of anger’; memadamkan kemurkaan ‘extinguish [the] wrath’; data 

from Table 1 in G. P. W. Rajeg (2014, p. 165)). In CMT, metaphor is viewed to arise from the 

interaction between the brain, body, language and culture (Gibbs, 2008, p. 4). Concerning the role of 

sexist language in conveying negative attitudes towards women (cf. §I), metaphorical language could 

play a central role as indicated by Charteris-Black (2005, p. 13): 

“Metaphor influences our beliefs, attitudes and values because it uses language to activate 

unconscious emotional associations and it influences the values that we place on ideas and 

beliefs on a scale of goodness and badness. It does this by transferring positive or negative 

associations of various source words to a target metaphor.” 

We will, in §3.1, discuss data manifesting Charteris-Black’s quote above. 

2.1.6 TRANSITIVITY 

Sexism as discussed in §2.2 and §2.4 can be evoked not only via specific words, but also via the 

entire utterance, a sentence, or a longer text (Wareing, 2004, p. 82). One aspect in the analysis of a 

sentence relevant to sexist language is “transitivity”. 

Transitivity is one part of the Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL) theory developed by Halliday 

(1994) (see also Eggins, 2004, for an introduction). As viewed in SFL, transitivity serves to convey the 

type of process or experience: action, event, conscious process, and relationships (Halliday, 1994). 

Halliday (1994, p. 101) adds that the process which is expressed through language is the result of our 

conception of the world or point of view. The transitivity model provides a way to find out how certain 

linguistic structures in a text point to different viewpoints or mindsets towards the world or one's 



4 | Luh Putu Laksminy, Gede Primahadi Wijaya Rajeg Lingual (Vol. 15, No.1, 2023) 

 

ideology, both traditional and in special circumstances. The viewpoint in this paper is related to the view 

towards sex and gender. 

The transitivity analysis in this paper is guided by the question of “who or what does what to 

whom or what” (Mills, 2008, p. 69). This question is also central to the study of syntax. Concerning 

sexism, in many literary works, there is often a tendency for female characters to be grammatically 

presented as objects of the actions of other characters; as an end-point of other actions, and in a position 

as an object compared to a subject position (Mills, 2008, p. 69). Wareing (1994, as cited in Mills, 2008) 

states that even in women’s literary works, where initially the female characters are active and confident, 

there is still a tendency for the characters to be shown as passive at certain crucial moments in the text, 

for example in sexual scenes (see §3.2 for some evidence and the discussion). 

2.2 METHODOLOGY 

The illustrative Indonesian data in this study will be taken from authentic language use on the web 

and two novels by a female author Oka Rusmini (Rusmini, 2000, 2003); this author is famous for her 

novels that focus on women's portrayal, especially against the socio-cultural background of Bali. The 

web-based data is given the URL via the footnote while the novel source is provided with an in-text 

citation. If none of these sources was given, the data would be provided introspectively by the authors. It 

is important to emphasise at the outset that the nature of this paper is mainly qualitative and exploratory. 

Further studies with a larger pool of datasets are needed to further substantiate the proposal presented in 

this paper. 

III RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 

In the following two sections, examples of Indonesian sexist expressions will be presented, 

together with the discussion of textbook-example from English as a comparison (citation to the author 

will be given in the main text after the example). In §3.1, we show several forms of sexual metaphors 

and their impact on women’s views. Then, §3.2 presents a discussion on sexist language as reflected in 

the sentence’s syntactic structure related to transitivity. 

3.1 METAPHORICAL SEXIST EXPRESSIONS 

In the Conceptual Metaphor Theory (CMT; §2.5), metaphor is defined as a process of 

understanding a conceptual domain (the so-called the target-domain or tenor) using knowledge from 

another conceptual domain (namely the source domain or vehicle) (Kövecses, 2010). The examples 

include thinking and talking about (i) LIFE via the concept of TRAVEL/JOURNEY (Hidupnya sedang 

melalui jalan terjal ‘his life is going through a steep road’), (ii) ARGUMENTATION via the concept of 

WAR/FIGHT (Bersilat lidah ‘crossed tongues’), or (iii) LOVE via the concept of INSANITY (Dia 

tergila-gila pada dirimu ‘(s)he is crazy about you’), amongst others.  

According to Eckert and McConnell-Ginet (2003, p. 215), vocabularies and concepts related to sex 

and gender are widely used as the metaphorical source domain to talk about another concept (see the 

linguistic examples for Indonesian below). “Sexual metaphor” highlights a marked tendency to use the 

conception of male-female differences in structuring speech and thought from a myriad of other 

differences (Haste, 1994 as cited in Eckert & McConnell-Ginet, 2003, p. 215). Particularly, sexual 

metaphor plays a central role in maintaining a sex-gender system that supports male dominance (Eckert 

& McConnell-Ginet, 2003, p. 215). One example is the feminists’ notion that English is a sexist 

language discriminating against women through metaphors as reflected in such words as bitch, ho (slang 

for whore), pimp, and faggot (Holmes, 2001, p. 305). The animal is another domain used in metaphor to 

characterise women as inferior to men. For example, the use of bitch for women indicates its negative 

traits compared to the use of studs and wolves for men (Holmes, 2001, p. 306).  

Sex and gender can also be used as the target domain or tenor of a metaphor, for example, using 

elements from the sports field as a source domain for understanding (hetero)sexual relations, as in the 

following English expression: “I didn't get to first base with him”, which projects images of men 

actively defeating women as in a baseball game (Eckert & McConnell-Ginet, 2003, p. 220). Much of 

what is termed ‘sexist language’ is felt to be offensive/derogatory because problematic attitudes lie 

behind metaphorical identifications with women and their sexes, for example, commodities, small 

animals, and with fruit or other desserts (Eckert & McConnell-Ginet, 2003, p. 220). In the remainder of 

this section, we present examples of Indonesian metaphorical sexist expressions, starting with (4-1). 

(3-1) Hai manis, apa kabar-mu di sana?  



Sexist Language in Indonesian via Metaphor and Transitivity: An Exploratory Study 5 

 

hi sweet what news-your at there 

‘Hi sweetie, how is it going with you over there?’ 

Expression hai manis ‘hi sweet(ie)’ in (3-1) is often used to greet women. The word manis ‘sweet’ 

is used metaphorically by using the image of food, especially saccharine. Metaphorical expression based 

on the saccharine concept is also common in English, for example, sweetie, honey, and sugar (Holmes, 

2001, p. 306). The use of terms such as ‘sweet’, which initially were neutral or loving, could eventually 

acquire a negative connotation when the term was used to address women with a focus on women as 

sexual objects (Holmes, 2001, p. 306). In Indonesian, it can be said that there are almost no food-related 

terms that seem appropriate to refer to men. Example (4-2) shows gender bias reflected in language 

based on animal metaphor. 

(3-2) keluar dari sini kamu wanita jalang (...)  
go.out from here you woman wild/untreated.animal 

‘get out of here, you bitch (lit. wild/untreated/non-domesticated animal) 

The animal-related adjective jalang ‘undomesticated; wild’ in (3-2) often modifies wanita 

‘woman’, illustrating how women are described as being like animals. The use of animal comparisons 

shows a less positive picture of women than men (Holmes, 2001, p. 305). Here is another example of an 

animal-based sexist metaphorical idiom. 

(3-3) mahasiswi (...)  menjadikan ayam kampus sebagai profesi 
female.univ.student make  chicken campus as profession 

untuk memenuhi kebutuhan hidup  

for fulfilling  need  life 

‘a female university student (...) chose prostitute (lit. campus chicken) as a profession to fulfil life 

necessities’ 

Example (3-3) shows that woman working as a prostitute is metaphorically referred to as ayam 

‘chicken’ the whole idiom ayam kampus meaning ‘campus prostitute; that is a female student who also 

works as a prostitute’. These animal-based expressions for women also exist in English, for example, 

chick, bitch, cougar, and kitten. Even though neutrally ayam is not necessarily bad, the aspect of chicken 

highlighted in its use in the idiom ayam kampus could be its helplessness as a pet. The helplessness 

aspect is then mapped to the target domain of a woman, who is easy to handle and turns into a prostitute. 

Compare this with the use of animal metaphors for men which often have at least some positive 

components, such as sexual prowess (e.g., macan ‘tiger’ or kuda ‘horse; stallion’). 

In the previous discussion, we show how sex/gender is metaphorically construed using another 

domain, such as animal. Next, as stated by Eckert and McConnell-Ginet (2003, p. 216), sex/gender can 

in turn be used as a metaphorical source domain for discussing various topics (see (3-4) and (3-5)). 

Women and their activities are often the primary source of the metaphors by which men construct a 

distinct sense of self, their relationship to the world, and their relationships with one another (Eckert & 

McConnell-Ginet, 2003, p. 217).  

(3-4) kiper M. Ridho (...) ingin gawang-nya tetap perawan  
goalkeeper NAME want goalpost-his stay virgin 

‘the goalkeeper, M. Ridho, (...) wants to keep his goalpost virgin’ 

(3-5) sebuah skema permainan cantik melahirkan gol keempat  
a scheme play; game pretty give.birth.to goal fourth 

‘a beautiful game plan resulted in (lit. give birth to) the fourth goal’ 

Examples (3-4) and (3-5) are commonly uttered (mostly by men) in sports, especially football (a 

sport identical to men). Again, these expressions contain words sexist towards women (i.e., perawan 

‘virgin’, cantik ‘pretty; beautiful’, melahirkan ‘give birth to’) because these words refer to women’s 

conditions, characteristics, and activities. Intuitively, we never find the corresponding expressions that 

use words sexist towards men in that context. For example, replacing perawan ‘(female) virgin’ with 

perjaka ‘(male) virgin’ in (3-4) into gawangnya tetap perjaka ‘the goal is still (male) virgin’ sounds 

infelicitous. The same is true when we replace cantik ‘pretty; beautiful’ with tampan/ganteng 

‘handsome’ in (3-5); it sounds unnatural and infelicitous. 

Although the use of sexist words in (3-4) and (3-5) does not seem to evoke negative and offensive 

attitudes towards women, they clearly demonstrate how the realm of women is more conventionally 

used in the domain identical to men (i.e., football sport). This then could indicate the imbalance in the 

position of women and men. The construal that a football goalpost can be “virgin” as in (3-4) entails that 



6 | Luh Putu Laksminy, Gede Primahadi Wijaya Rajeg Lingual (Vol. 15, No.1, 2023) 

 

the female sexual organ could be conceptualised as if it is a metaphorical goal that must be conquered as 

in a game of football. This is shown in (3-6) below. A similar image, namely sangkar ‘cage’, is also 

attested (3-7). 

(3-6) David mulai membobol gawang gadis itu 
NAME start break.into goalpost girl that 

dengan sangat hati-hati  ia tahu jika dia masih virgin  

with very be.careful he know if she still virgin 

‘David starts penetrating (lit. broke into the goalpost/vagina of) that girl with care, knowing that 

she is still a virgin’ 

(3-7) Kelamin  yang menghinggapi sangkar perempuan itu.  
sex.organ which overtake  cage woman  that 

‘(it knows precisely how many) sex organs (i.e., penis) which have overtaken the cage of that 

woman.’ 

Despite the similarity in form between a goalpost and a cage, (3-6) and (3-7) suggest that women 

are construed as objects that must be conquered, in this case for example, sexually, as well as other 

things (e.g., power, domination, etc.). This contrasts with the image used to construe the male sexual 

organ. In Indonesian, the penis can be referred to as senjata ‘weapon’ (3-8) or even rudal ‘missile’ (3-9).   

(3-8) Memperbesar ukuran senjata pria  
enlarge  size weapon male 

‘Enlarging the size of the penis (lit. male weapon)’ 

(3-9) ia menendang rudal pria itu 
(s)he kick  missile male that 

membuat sang pria kesakitan  

make  the male get.hurt 

‘(s)he kicked the man’s penis (lit. the man’s missile) making him hurt’ 

The implication of these metaphors is the view that men are portrayed as strong and powerful 

compared to women with respect to their same biological aspect (i.e., the construal of their sexual 

organ). The different choice of metaphor for women and men in examples (3-6) to (3-9) could be viewed 

as sexism because it tends to highlight the weak side of women. It raises gender bias in society regarding 

the views on women. The following examples illustrate other metaphorical expressions evoking 

negative attitudes towards women as if they are metaphorical objects for men. 

(3-10) Data from Rusmini (2000, p. 26) 
lelaki dengan bebasnya memilih perempuan-perempuan baru 

man with freely  choose women new 

untuk mengalirkan limbah laki-laki-nya. 

for stream  waste male-his 

‘men freely chose new women to stream their manly waste (i.e., sperm)’ 

The implication of (3-10) is that women are viewed metaphorically as the ‘dumping place’ to 

channel men’s sexual desires; this desire is metonymically evoked by the word limbah ‘waste’ in limbah 

laki-lakinya ‘men’s manly waste/semen’. The opposite of this metaphor as applied to men is 

theoretically not the case. 

Next, other examples in (3-11) and (3-12) show that women’s body is construed as food that men 

want to taste and eat. 

(3-11) Data from Rusmini (2000, p. 28) 
laki-laki yang sering menjilati tubuh Sekar 

men who often lick  body NAME 

dengan  mata  mereka .... 

with eyes their 

‘men who often lick Sekar’s body through their eyes....’ 

(3-12) Sebelum menikah  siri,  Farhat mengakui sempat  
before be.married unregistered NAME admit  ever.once 

“mencicipi” tubuh molek Regina.  

taste  body comely NAME 

‘Before committing the unregistered marriage, Farhat admitted that he has once tasted Regina’s 

comely body’ 



Sexist Language in Indonesian via Metaphor and Transitivity: An Exploratory Study 7 

 

Finally, (3-13) below indicates that women are considered as a commercial commodity that can be 

sold. Therefore, women who are prostitutes are often referred to as barang ‘goods’, which evokes a 

negative connotation. 

(3-13) Data from Rusmini (2000, p. 211) 
Dari   orang-orang  Sadri    mendengar, Kendran menjual  seluruh tubuh-nya. 

from   people            NAME   hear             NAME    sell         entire    body-her 

‘Sadri heard from people that Kendrad sells her entire body’ 

In sum, all the metaphorical expressions above indicate that women are still considered inferior, 

subordinate, and discriminated against men. 

3.2 SEXIST EXPRESSIONS VIA TRANSITIVITY 

Transitivity analysis in this paper examines “who does what to whom” (Mills, 2008, p. 69). Such 

an analysis captures actions performed by actors against patients. We investigate the semantic roles men 

and women fill in the sentence. For that reason, we identify the use of passive and active sentences with 

transitive verbs with men and female participants. The following extracts exemplify transitivity in 

sentences with sexist nuances. 

(3-14) Data from Rusmini (2000, pp. 5–6) 
Keringat laki-laki itu benar-benar membuat=nya mabuk  

sweat man that truly  make=her drunk 

‘The sweat of that man truly made her (i.e., Sadri) drunk’ 

In (3-14), the change of state that Sadri experienced (namely being drunk, that is in this case losing 

control of his feelings) is not directly caused by the action of the man (namely Putu Sarma) (an example 

of indirect causation). However, semantically, an aspect of Putu Sarma (namely his sweat) had an 

impact on changes in Sadri’s (emotional) state. The role of the woman character as the affected here 

further suggests the view that women tend to be emotional and helpless. 

Next, the examples in (3-15) to (3-17) below show the passive forms of transitive verbs sentuh 

‘touch’ perkosa ‘rape’, and peristri ‘cause to become a wife’. In these sentences, women characters are 

the undergoer of the action performed by the men-actor. This further indicates the positioning of women 

as the end point of an action chain. 

(3-15) Data from Rusmini (2000, p. 56) 
“Bagaimana mungkin seorang penari Joged 

how  possible a dancer name.of.a.dance 

yang tubuh-nya  biasa di-sentuh laki-laki 

which body-her usually PASS-touch men 

bisa menasihati cucu-ku  dengan baik.” 

can advise  grandchild-my with good 

‘How is it possible that a Joged dancer, whose body usually gets touched by men, can offer good 

advice to my grandchildren’ 

(3-16) Data from Rusmini (2000, pp. 36–37) 
ibu Sekar di-perkosa oleh lebih dari tiga laki-laki. 

mother NAME PASS-rape by more from three man 

‘Sekar’s mother was raped by more than three men.’ 

(3-17) Data from Rusmini (2003, p. 13) 
Adik-nya  itu sudah di-per-istri  Bhuana, laki-laki muda 

younger.sister-her that already PASS-CAUS-wife NAME man young 

‘her younger sister has been made wife by Bhuana, a young man...’ 

One other active-voice example in (3-18) below further illustrates that semantically, perempuan-

perempuan ‘women’ are the endpoint or undergoers of the direct action by the man-actor. 

(3-18) Data from Rusmini (2000, p. 61) 
benih laki-laki jalang yang telah meniduri 

seed man wild which already have.sex.with (lit. sleep on) 

perempuan-perempuan tidak terhormat. 

women   not honored 

‘(the) seed of a wild man who has slept with (lit. have sex with) dishonourable women’ 

In both active and passive sentences above, the transitive verb is associated with the action 

performed by the male character in the novels. In other words, it is the men who initiate the action 



8 | Luh Putu Laksminy, Gede Primahadi Wijaya Rajeg Lingual (Vol. 15, No.1, 2023) 

 

against the object. In contrast, the women characters are cast in passive and helpless roles as they 

undergo the result of the men’s actions or the men’s features (see (3-14)). 

The tendency for women to fill in the undergoer role (e.g., that of patient or experiencer of an 

event) can be identified in the way women present themselves in interactions. Due to stereotypical 

views, some women write themselves as helpless victims, and their texts will contain many “disenabling 

metaphors, disenabling lexis, and disenabling syntactic structures” (Burton 1982, as cited in Mills, 2008, 

p. 69). For example, women are more likely to say, “You'll never guess what happened to me” than 

“You'll never guess what I've just done”. Moreover, as we have seen in the above examples in this 

section, certain types of verbal habits (e.g., presenting someone as the recipient of an action) are seen as 

stereotypical for women (Mills, 2008, p. 69). 

Overall, it can be said that the syntactic selection in the data above shows a conventional gender 

assignment of men and women. The data we have presented in this section could indicate the language 

of conquest and belonging, and the female characters seem unable to resist. We want to show that 

women are in a position determined by men. In other words, women have a weaker position than men. 

Women are used as complements, models, and objects of satisfaction and sexual victims. 

IV CONCLUSION 

Sexism involves behaviour that maintains inequality between men and women, so that sexist 

attitudes stereotype someone based on gender, rather than on individual achievement and ability 

assessments. Sexist language displays the inequality of men and women as if members of one sex do not 

appear to be fully human. 

This paper discusses sexism shown through language or expressions of sexism (§2.1.4) in 

Indonesian. Sexism can be created using sexual metaphors which sometimes demean women (§3.1). It is 

found that metaphorical sexist expressions can conceptualise women as ANIMALS, 

COMMODITIES/GOODS, FOODS, and DUMPING SITES. It was also found that events associated 

with women's biological reproduction (e.g., childbirth) were used as topics of sexist expression for 

women which tended to eventually lead to negative stereotypes. 

In the analysis of transitivity (§3.2), the author uses syntactic-semantic structures displaying 

participants interacting in a stereotypical portrait of gender relations between men and women. This 

happens because there are still stereotyped views, even from the women themselves, so that they 

(women) are portrayed as helpless victims (or objects of the action of men). Moreover, certain types of 

verbal habits (e.g., presenting someone as the recipient of an action) are seen as stereotypical for 

women. This is following what was proposed by Halliday (1994) (presented in §2.1.6) that the 

transitivity model provides a way to discover how a particular linguistic structure of a text refers to 

different viewpoints or mindsets towards the world or one’s ideology. Future studies can conduct 

quantitative analysis of the distribution of the semantic role male and female characters occupy in the 

sentences in which they occur. 

REFERENCES 

Charteris-Black, J. (2005). Politicians and Rhetoric. Palgrave Macmillan UK. 

Coates, J. (1986). Women, men, and language: A sociolinguistic account of sex differences in language. 

Longman. 

Coates, J. (2007). Gender. In C. Llamas, L. Mullany, & P. Stockwell (Eds.), The Routledge companion 

to sociolinguistics (pp. 62–68). Routledge. 

Deignan, A. (2005). Metaphor and corpus linguistics. John Benjamins Publishing Company. 

Eckert, P., & McConnell-Ginet, S. (2003). Language and gender. Cambridge University Press. 

Eggins, S. (2004). Introduction to Systemic Functional Linguistics: 2nd Edition. A&C Black. 

Fiksdal, S. (2008). Metaphorically speaking: Gender and classroom discourse. In G. Kristiansen & R. 

Dirven (Eds.), Cognitive sociolinguistics: Language variation, cultural models, social systems 

(pp. 419–448). Mouton de Gruyter. 

Geeraerts, D. (2008). Prototypes, Stereotypes, and Semantic Norms. In G. Kristiansen & R. Dirven 

(Eds.), Cognitive sociolinguistics: Language variation, cultural models, social systems (pp. 21–

44). Mouton de Gruyter. 

Gibbs, R. W. (2008). Metaphor and thought: The state of the art. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge 

handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 3–13). Cambridge University Press. 

Halliday, M. A. K. (1994). An Introduction to Functional Grammar (2nd ed.). Edward Arnold. 



Sexist Language in Indonesian via Metaphor and Transitivity: An Exploratory Study 9 

 

Holmes, J. (2001). An Introduction to Sociolinguistics. Longman. 

Kövecses, Z. (2000). The concept of ANGER: Universal or culture specific? Psychopathology, 33, 159–

170. 

Kövecses, Z. (2010). Metaphor: A practical introduction (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. 

Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. The University of Chicago Press. 

Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to 

Western thought. Basic Books. 

Lakoff, R. (1975). Language and Woman’s Place. Harper & Row. 

Meyerhoff, M. (2006). Introducing Sociolinguistics. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203966709 

Mills, S. (2008). Language and sexism. Cambridge University Press. 

Ningrum, E. E., Netra, I. M., & Rajeg, G. P. W. (2021). Swearing Expressions by Main Male and 

Female Characters in Night School Movie. Udayana Journal of Social Sciences and Humanities 

(UJoSSH), 5(2), 77–80. https://doi.org/10.24843/UJoSSH.2021.v05.i02.p04 

Pauwels, A. (2003). Linguistic Sexism and Feminist Linguistic Activism. In J. Holmes & M. Meyerhoff 

(Eds.), The Handbook of Language and Gender (pp. 550–570). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 

https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470756942.ch24 

Payne, K. E. (2001). Different But Equal: Communication Between the Sexes. Greenwood Publishing 

Group. 

Rajeg, G. P. W. (2014). Metaphorical profiles of five Indonesian quasi-synonyms of ANGER: Multiple 

distinctive collexeme analysis. Proceedings of the International Congress of the Linguistic 

Society of Indonesia 2014, 165–170. https://doi.org/10.4225/03/58578ddba1fd2 

Rajeg, I. M. (2013). Metafora emosi Bahasa Indonesia [Disertasi doktoral, Universitas Udayana]. 

https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.7886180 

Rusmini, O. (2000). Tarian Bumi. Tera. 

Rusmini, O. (2003). Kenanga. Grasindo. 

Wareing, S. (2004). Language and gender. In L. Thomas, S. Wareing, I. Singh, J. S. Peccei, J. 

Thornborrow, & J. Jones (Eds.), Language, society and power: An introduction (2nd ed, pp. 75–

92). Routledge. 

Weatherall, A. (2002). Gender, Language and Discourse. Routledge. 

https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203988817.