LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 

 
LLT Journal: A Journal on Language and Language Learning 

 http://e-journal.usd.ac.id/index.php/LLT 

Sanata Dharma University, Yogyakarta, Indonesia 

 

304 

 

 

HOW TEACHERS IN AN INFORMAL ENGLISH TRAINING 

INSTITUTION TREAT YOUNG LEARNERS’ ORAL ERRORS 
 

Ihsan Nur Iman Faris1 and Dian Budiarti2* 
1Universitas Pendidikan Indonesia, Indonesia 

2UIN Sunan Gunung Djati Bandung, Indonesia 

ihsan.nif@upi.edu1 and dianbudiarti@uinsgd.ac.id2  

*correspondence: dianbudiarti@uinsgd.ac.id 

https://doi.org/10.24071/llt.v26i1.5341 

received 7 November 2022; accepted 5 April 2023 

 

Abstract 

Corrective feedback needs to be used carefully to treat young learners’ oral errors 

as it should not inhibit learners from communicating. This study aims to investigate 

the use of corrective feedback strategies by informal English training center 

teachers in Bandung. Classroom observation and coding were used to collect and 

categorize data regarding errors committed by students and teachers’ use of 

corrective strategies.  The study found that pronunciation errors (49%) were most 

frequently committed, followed by grammar (27%), vocabulary (20%), and the use 

of L1 (4%) errors. To treat those errors, the teachers apply corrective feedback 

strategies, which mostly were in the form of recast (46%) and elicitation (35%). 

Other corrective feedback strategies such as explicit correction (11%), 

metalinguistic feedback (6%), repetition (1%), and paralinguistic signal (1%) were 

found less frequently. This study suggests teachers consider several factors such as 

types of errors, learning factors, and learners’ factors when deciding the corrective 

feedback strategies.  

 

Keywords: considerations, oral errors, teachers’ corrective feedback 

 

Introduction  

English for young learner programs should aim mainly to encourage children 

to speak English more confidently (Alakrash & Razak, 2021; Harmer, 2007). Since 

the improvement of fluency should be prioritized over accuracy, error correction 

should not be conducted excessively. Nevertheless, corrective feedback needs to be 

given to boost learning and prevent repetitions of errors (Ellis, 2009; Gebhard, 

2009; Li, 2018). To ensure the provision of feedback does not inhibit learners from 

speaking, teachers need to understand how to give proper feedback to children.  

Corrective feedback should not be addressed to every oral error made by 

learners since it may hinder the flow of communication or fluency which is the main 

objective of language learning (see, for example, Chu, 2011; Ok & Ustact, 2013; 

Shahini & Shahamirian, 2017; Widiati & Cahyono, 2006). Hilliard (2014) suggests 

that spoken languages are not as rigid as written languages. The focus on grammar 

is viewed as secondary as grammar acquisition will follow automatically when 

students are fluent enough to speak and able to formulate the rules by themselves 

mailto:ihsan.nif@upi.edu1
mailto:dianbudiarti@uinsgd.ac.id2
https://doi.org/10.24071/llt.v26i1.5341


 

LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 
 

305 

 

(Gordon, 2007).  However, some experts see correction as necessary even in the 

early stages of learning as it can boost learning when given appropriately according 

to children’s affective and developmental stages (see for example Ellis, 2009, 2017; 

Harmer, 2007; Lyster & Saito, 2010; Panova & Lyster, 2002). This notion is also 

supported by Ellis (2017) and Chu (2011) suggesting giving corrective feedback to 

avoid fossilization. Ellis (2017) further emphasizes that some errors, called global 

errors, should be treated since it hinders the understanding of messages, while 

others, local errors, can be ignored. The explanations show the intricacy and 

complexity of the giving corrective feedback process for children. Teachers need to 

understand various factors such as children’s English learning levels, children’s 

characteristics, types of errors, and types of corrective feedback before deciding to 

give corrective feedback.        

The unique and complex situations in the classroom require teachers to 

understand various types of corrective feedback to treat errors. In general, 

corrective feedback is categorized into explicit and implicit feedback (Ellis et al., 

2006). Lyster & Ranta (1997) further categorize corrective feedback into (1) recast 

(2) explicit correction (3) clarification request (asking for reformulation) (4) error 

repetition (5) elicitation (eliciting the reformulation directly), and (6) metalinguistic 

feedback. Panova & Lyster (2002) and Ellis (2009) add translation and 

paralinguistic signals to the list of corrective feedback strategies. Of the strategies 

mentioned, metalinguistic feedback, elicitation, paralinguistic signal, and explicit 

correction belong to explicit corrective feedback strategies, while the rest belong to 

implicit corrective feedback strategies.   

Some reports suggest that teachers favor the use of explicit feedback to 

correct children’s oral errors (Choi & Li, 2012; Maolida, 2013). The preference 

might be due to the effectiveness of explicit corrective feedback in correcting and 

improving learners’ grammar understanding (Ellis et al., 2006; Rezaei & 

Derakhshan, 2011) and their noticeability of students’ errors (Granena & Yilmaz, 

2019; Sheen & Ellis, 2011).   

Research on the use of oral corrective feedback to treat learners’ errors has 

been conducted in various contexts in Indonesia. Yusuf et al. (2017) found that the 

use of multimodal feedback effectively improves students’ understanding of the 

lessons. In terms of the type of corrective feedback used, Fathimah (2017) found 

that implicit feedback, especially recast, was used more frequently by teachers to 

treat adolescent students’ oral errors. She further explains that the teachers use 

recast due to the type of errors, learning objectives, and learners’ relatively low 

proficiency level. Input-providing strategy in explicit correction is viewed as 

effective in improving teenage learners speaking skills, especially when given in 

the scaffolding process since it indicates learners’ incorrect production while giving 

the correct ones at the same time (Tersta, 2017).  Interestingly, despite students’ 

low proficiency, teachers in young learner English programs also use explicit 

corrective feedback to address kindergarten students’ oral errors. Maolida (2013) 

suggests that young learners can understand grammar concepts better when 

grammatical errors are corrected explicitly. Nevertheless, the research does not 

explain what specific types of corrective feedback are used to treat errors. Further 

research needs to be conducted to investigate what types of corrective feedback are 

suitable for treating young learners’ errors as an inaccurate use of corrective 



 

LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 
 

306 

 

feedback types can inhibit learners from speaking and hinder the achievement of 

fluency which is the main objective of English for young learner programs.   

Research investigating the types of corrective feedback teachers use to treat 

errors needs to be conducted as the findings can help teachers determine the 

appropriate strategy to correct their students’ errors (Russell, 2009). Much research 

has been conducted to reveal what general types of feedback teachers use to treat 

young learners’ errors, but research investigating how specific types of corrective 

feedback are used to treat particular errors is relatively scarce. This research aims 

to fill the lacuna by revealing what types of errors are committed by young learners 

and what corrective feedback strategies the teachers use. 

 

Method  

This research was conducted in three English for young learner classes in one 

English training center in Bandung. Observations were conducted to collect the data 

from the site. The study observed the use of corrective feedback of three classes in 

the institution, namely ‘Class A’, ‘Class B’, and ‘Class C’. Each teacher in each 

class holds a bachelor of English education or English literature degree and has 

been teaching English at the site for at least six months. The class consisted of four 

to five students aged 7-12 years old. The class which lasted for 100 minutes per 

meeting was observed three times, making the number of classroom observations 

12 or 1200 minutes in total.  The instruments used to collect the data from the 

observation were video recording, audio recording, and field notes. 

After the data were collected, the data analysis process was conducted to 

interpret the sets of data. This study categorizes students’ errors based on Lyster & 

Ranta (1997) and Yang (2016), classifying language production errors into 

pronunciation errors, grammar, vocabulary, and use of L1 errors. As for the 

corrective feedback strategies, this study categorizes the strategies mainly based on 

Lyster & Ranta (1997), Ellis (2009), and Panova & Lyster (2002). They categorize 

corrective feedback strategies into recast, explicit correction, translation, 

clarification request, metalinguistic feedback, elicitation, repetition, and 

paralinguistic signals. The findings then are displayed in frequency and 

percentages. 

 

Findings and Discussion 

Types of errors committed by young learners  

 This study found that the types of errors the young learners committed the 

most frequently were pronunciation, grammar, vocabulary, and use of L1 errors, 

respectively. The details are as follows.  

  
Table 1. Distribution of students’ oral errors 

Types of Errors Number of Occurrences in Each Class Total 

Class A Class B Class C F % 

Pronunciation 23 18 51 92 49% 

Grammar 15 12 24 51 27% 

Vocabulary 11 13 13 37 20% 

Use of L1 4 2 2 8 4% 

Total 53 45 90 188 100% 



 

LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 
 

307 

 

The table above shows that among the three classes observed, Class C’s 

students committed more errors compared to Class A and Class B’s students, while 

the fewest average number of errors was found in Class A’s class. The most 

frequent oral errors that the students commit are pronunciation errors with 92 

occurrences (49%), followed by grammar errors, vocabulary errors, and use of L1 

errors, which occurrences are 51 (27%), 37 (20%), and 8 (4%), respectively. 

This finding corroborates several experts (see, for example, Aktuğ, 2015; 

Brown, 2016; Choi & Li, 2012; Yang, 2016) stating that phonological or 

pronunciation errors were committed the most frequently by young English 

learners. Gordon (2007) suggests that the focus of most young learners’ classes on 

fluency and confidence development might correlate with the tendency of 

pronunciation errors to appear frequently in the class. Moreover, the L1 transfer 

process might also influence pronunciation learning (Gass & Selinker, 2008). 

Bahasa Indonesia, students’ L1, is a phonetic language whose spelling and 

pronunciation tend to be similar (Karlina et al., 2020). The habit of using the L1 

might interfere with English pronunciations as English, a non-phonetic language, 

usually has spelling different from the pronunciation (Gass & Selinker, 2008). 

Regarding grammar errors, they might occur frequently in young learners’ 

classes as teachers may ignore this type of error depending on the objectives and 

perspectives of the teachers on the importance of grammar. In general, EFL young 

learners’ classes mainly aim to encourage students to communicate fluently and 

confidently (Cameron, 2001; Gordon, 2007); thus, grammar has not been viewed 

as the main objective of EFL young learners’ classes since the learning of the 

abstract concept might inhibit students to speak openly. Furthermore, Gordon 

(2007) suggests that children will learn grammar eventually, after formulating the 

grammatical rules on their own and when their cognitive ability to understand the 

concept is ready. He (Gordon, 2007) warns that the use of corrective feedback tends 

to be ineffective, or even harmful when they are given too frequently. 

The finding of vocabulary errors as the third most frequently found errors in 

this study is quite interesting as the limited numbers may imply that the students 

possess the understanding of grammatical behavior, collocations associations, 

registers, and associations of the vocabulary required to use the vocabulary 

accurately (Nation, 1990; Tiley & Rentler, 2022; Young-Davy, 2014). The students 

may have enough opportunities to practice the words communicatively. 

Considering that the students only learn in the classroom for about three hours a 

week, which in general is quite short, they may also use English outside the training, 

making their vocabulary use relatively accurate.  

The use of L1 error in this study is the least frequently found. Firstly, this 

finding might be because not all L1 use in the class was considered an error. L1 

errors were counted only when the use is unsolicited or responded to by the 

teachers. In other words, the categorization of L1 as an error depends on the 

commitment or requirement to use English in classroom activities.  A discussion 

and casual conversation not relevant to the objective of the lesson might not be 

categorized as errors. Therefore, the low frequency of L1 errors found in the study 

might be encouraged by the strict application of commitment to using English 

during the main activities. Nevertheless, the use of L1 during activities in the 

classrooms might be due to students’ limited vocabulary (Lyster, 1998; Lyster & 

Ranta, 1997; Shin et al., 2020). 



 

LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 
 

308 

 

 

Teachers’ corrective feedback strategies 

 This study categorizes teachers’ corrective feedback strategies into recast, 

translation, explicit correction, clarification request, metalinguistic feedback, 

elicitation, repetition, and metalinguistic feedback (Ellis, 2009; Lyster & Ranta, 

1997; Panova & Lyster, 2002). The frequency and percentage of each corrective 

feedback strategy are as follows: 

 
Table 2. Distribution of teachers’ corrective feedback strategies 

Feedback strategy Ms. Rini Mr. Adi Ms. Dwinda 
Total 

F % 

Recast 18 16 54 88 46% 

Elicitation 24 19 25 68 35% 

Explicit correction 17 0 5 22 11% 

Metalinguistic feedback 6 2 3 11 6% 

Repetition 0 2 0 2 1% 

Paralinguistic signal 0 0 2 2 1% 

Translation 0 0 0 0 0% 

Clarification request 0 0 0 0 0% 

TOTAL 65 39 89 193 100% 

 

 Of eight corrective feedback strategies, six categories were used to address 

students’ errors. Recast was the most frequently used strategy, followed by 

elicitation, explicit correction, metalinguistic feedback, repetition, and 

paralinguistic signal. The finding echoes several experts (see, for example, 

Ahangari & Amirzadeh, 2011; Brown, 2016; Panova & Lyster, 2002; Sauro, 2009) 

reporting the predominant use of recast in the class for addressing students’ errors. 

A detailed discussion of each corrective feedback strategy is presented below. 

 Recast belongs to input-providing strategies giving learners the correct 

versions of their erroneous utterances. This study found recast to be the most 

frequently used with 88 occurrences (41.9%). The data also indicate that all teachers 

in the classes observed that recast is favored. However, being the most favored 

corrective feedback strategy does not make recasting the most effective in 

addressing the errors. Sheen & Ellis (2011) suggest that students tend to ignore 

recast as corrections as this strategy is often mistaken as confirmation of students’ 

correct utterances. Lyster & Saito (2010) suggest teachers be more explicit in using 

recast as a correction strategy to encourage students to notice their errors.    

Elicitation is a corrective feedback strategy that encourages learners to correct 

their errors (Lyster & Ranta, 1997). This strategy belongs to explicit correction 

since it demands learners to stop and think about the correction. This study found 

elicitation as the second most frequently used corrective feedback with 71 

occurrences (33.8%). The teachers mostly use completion, asking for students to 

pause before continuing their speech, as elicitation. Unlike recast, elicitation is 

viewed as more effective in addressing students’ errors (Behroozi & Karimnia, 

2017). This study found that 90% of the use of elicitation can encourage responses 

from students. According to Ahangari & Amirzadeh (2011), elicitation encourages 

students’ notice, which is an essential element in language acquisition. Similarly, 

Ellis et al. (2006) support the use of elicitation to address students’ errors as it can 

draw students’ attention to their erroneous language production. 



 

LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 
 

309 

 

Explicit correction is similar to recast as it provides inputs. However, it 

belongs to a different category than recast as it explicitly indicates that students 

commit errors by saying “no” or “incorrect” (Lyster & Ranta, 1997); thus, unlike 

recast, explicit correction can interrupt the flow of communication (Sheen & Ellis, 

2011). Interestingly, even though the explicit correction strategy requires students 

to stop and repeat the correct forms given by teachers, not all explicit correction in 

this study was followed by students’ uptakes (Yilmaz, 2016). Further investigation 

needs to be conducted to find the exact reasons, yet the possible explanation for the 

absence of uptakes despite the provision of correct inputs is that students do not 

understand the correction or see the urgency of repeating the teachers’ correction 

as the communication has been understood. Moreover, students might not give 

uptake since they are inhibited to speak after realizing that they commit errors 

(Krashen, 1982).  

Metalinguistic feedback is an explicit correction and output prompting 

strategy which requires teachers to give comments, information, or questions that 

can serve as clues for students to correct their errors (Lyster & Ranta, 1997). This 

strategy might be more suitable to correct grammar errors as they can act as mini-

lessons (Hashemian & Farhang-Ju, 2018). Nevertheless, this strategy might not be 

suitable for correcting young learners’ errors as the grammar explanation is too 

abstract (Cameron, 2001). In this research, metalinguistic feedback was used only 

11 times, but it generated almost 90% correct answers from students.  The finding 

confirms Taipale (2012) suggesting that output-prompting strategies such as 

metalinguistic feedback are effective in addressing students’ oral errors. 

Repetition as a corrective feedback strategy is different from recast as it does 

not provide correct forms as inputs (Lyster & Ranta, 1997). Teachers attempt to 

draw students’ attention to their errors by rising and stressing the intonation when 

repeating the errors. As for paralinguistic signals, the corrective feedback strategy 

uses gestures and facial expressions to indicate that students have committed errors 

(Lyster & Ranta, 1997). These two strategies were not used frequently by teachers 

most probably because they were considered ineffective. Katayama (2006) suggests 

that repetition and paralinguistic signals are ambiguous; thus, students rarely take 

them as forms of feedback and input.  

Panova & Lyster (2002) suggest that translation is specifically used to treat 

the use of L1 error. The use of this corrective feedback strategy is easy to notice as 

it uses a different code. This corrective feedback strategy might be suitable for 

young learners whose vocabulary is still limited. Nevertheless, teachers should not 

overuse this corrective feedback strategy since it might discourage learners from 

using the target language. Storch & Wigglesworth (2003) suggest that teachers 

sharing L1 background with students might indicate that the use of L1 is acceptable 

when using this corrective feedback strategy. 

  

Conclusion 

This study investigated the oral errors committed by young learners and what 

corrective feedback strategies teachers use to correct the errors. The study found 

that students committed pronunciation, grammar, vocabulary, and the use of L1 

errors. Of all the types of errors committed, pronunciation errors were found the 

most frequently, followed by grammar, vocabulary, and the use of L1 errors.  



 

LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 
 

310 

 

The teachers in this research used six of eight types of corrective feedback 

strategies: recast, elicitation, explicit correction, metalinguistic feedback, 

repetition, and paralinguistic signal. Of all the corrective feedback strategies, recast 

was used the most frequently due to its non-intervening and input-providing nature.  

From the findings on the oral errors committed by the students, the teachers 

can reflect on their own students’ oral errors and then plan the corrective feedback 

strategies which are suitable for treating the most-frequently errors committed by 

their students. Teachers then can consider the findings of corrective feedback 

strategies used by the teachers in this research to reflect on their efficacy in 

addressing the students’ types of error. The rate of uptake on certain types of error 

can be used as the basis for determining which corrective feedback is suitable for 

each teacher’s condition. 

This research did not investigate which particular strategies were effective in 

addressing certain oral errors. Thus, future research needs to analyze what 

corrective feedback strategies should be used to treat certain errors. More 

investigation on reasons why teachers prefer a certain type of corrective feedback 

strategies also needs to be conducted to get a clearer picture of which corrective 

feedback strategies should be used to treat learners’ oral errors. 

 

References  

Ahangari, S., & Amirzadeh, S. (2011). Exploring the teachers’ use of spoken 

corrective feedback in teaching Iranian EFL learners at different levels of 

proficiency. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 29, 1859–1868. 

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.11.435  
Aktuğ, B. (2015). Common pronunciation errors of seventh grade EFL learners: A 

case from Turkey. Ankara: Middle East Technical University. 

Alakrash, H. M., & Razak, N. A. (2021). Technology-based language learning: 

Investigation of digital technology and digital literacy. Sustainability, 13(21), 

1-17. https://doi.org/10.3390/su132112304  

Behroozi, B., & Karimnia, A. (2017). Educational context and ELT teachers’ 

corrective feedback preference: Public and private school teachers in focus. 

International Journal of Research in English Education, 2(2), 10–15. 

https://doi.org/10.18869/acadpub.ijree.2.2.10  

Brown, D. (2016). The type and linguistic foci of oral corrective feedback in the L2 

classroom: A meta-analysis. Language Teaching Research, 20(4), 436–458. 

https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168814563200  

Cameron, L. (2001). Teaching languages to young learners. Cambridge: 

Cambridge University Press. 

Choi, S.-Y., & Li, S. (2012). Corrective feedback and learner uptake in a child 

ESOL classroom. RELC Journal, 43(3), 331–351. 

https://doi.org/10.1177/0033688212463274  

Chu, R. (2011). Effects of teacher’s corrective feedback on accuracy in the oral 

English of English-majors college students. Theory and Practice in Language 

Studies, 1(5), 454-459. https://doi.org/10.4304/tpls.1.5.454-459  

Ellis, R. (2009). Corrective feedback and teacher development. L2 Journal, 1(1), 3-

18. https://doi.org/10.5070/L2.V1I1.9054  

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.11.435
https://doi.org/10.3390/su132112304
https://doi.org/10.18869/acadpub.ijree.2.2.10
https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168814563200
https://doi.org/10.1177/0033688212463274
https://doi.org/10.4304/tpls.1.5.454-459
https://doi.org/10.5070/L2.V1I1.9054


 

LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 
 

311 

 

Ellis, R. (2017). Oral corrective feedback in L2 classrooms: What we know so far. 

In H. Nassaji & E. Kartchava (Eds.), Corrective feedback in second language 

teaching and learning (1st ed., p. 16). New York: Routledge. 

Ellis, R., Loewen, S., & Erlam, R. (2006). Implicit and explicit corrective feedback 

and the acquisition of L2 grammar. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 

28(02), 339-368. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263106060141  

Fathimah, N. M. (2017). Teachers’ corrective feedback on students’ oral 

production in EFL classrooms. Bandung: The Indonesia University of 

Education. 

Gass, S. M., & Selinker, L. (2008). Second language acquisition (3rd ed.). New 

York: Routledge. 

Gebhard, J. G. (2009). Teaching English as a foreign or second language (2nd ed.). 

Ann Arbor: the University of Michigan Press. 

Gordon, T. (2007). Teaching young children a second language. Westport: Praeger. 

Granena, G., & Yilmaz, Y. (2019). Corrective feedback and the role of implicit 

sequence-learning ability in L2 online performance. Language Learning, 69, 

127–156. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12319  

Harmer, J. (2007). The practice of English language teaching (4th ed.). Harlow: 

Pearson Longman. 

Hashemian, M., & Farhang-Ju, M. (2018). Effects of metalinguistic feedback on 

grammatical accuracy of Iranian field (in) dependent L2 learners’ writing 

ability. Journal of Research in Applied Linguistics, 9(2), 141–161. 

https://doi.org/10.22055/RALS.2018.13797   

Hilliard, A. (2014). Spoken grammar and its role in the English language classroom. 

English Teaching Forum, 4, 2–13. 

Karlina, Y., Rahman, A., & Chowdhury, R. (2020). Designing phonetic alphabet 

for bahasa indonesia (PABI) for the teaching of intelligible English 

pronunciation in Indonesia. Indonesian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 9(3), 

724–732. https://doi.org/10.17509/ijal.v9i3.23223  
Katayama, A. (2006). Perceptions of JFL students toward correction of oral errors. 

In K. Bradford-Watts, C. Ikeguchi, & M. Swanson (Eds.), JALT2005 (pp. 

1248–1264). Tokyo: JALT. 

Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. 

Oxford: Pergamon. 

Li, S. (2018). Corrective feedback in L2 speech production. In J. Liontas (Ed.), The 

TESOL encyclopedia of English language teaching (1st ed., pp. 1–9). 

Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 

Lyster, R. (1998). Recasts, repetition, and ambiguity in L2 classroom discourse. 

Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 20(1), 51–81. 

https://doi.org/10.1017/S027226319800103X  

Lyster, R., & Ranta, L. (1997). Corrective feedback and learner uptake. Studies in 

Second Language Acquisition, 19(1), 37–66. 

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263197001034  

Lyster, R., & Saito, K. (2010). Interactional feedback as instructional input. 

Language, Interaction and Acquisition, 1(2), 276–297. 

https://doi.org/10.1075/lia.1.2.07lys  

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263106060141
https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12319
https://doi.org/10.22055/RALS.2018.13797
https://doi.org/10.17509/ijal.v9i3.23223
https://doi.org/10.1017/S027226319800103X
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263197001034
https://doi.org/10.1075/lia.1.2.07lys


 

LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 
 

312 

 

Maolida, E. H. (2013). A descriptive study of teacher’s oral feedback in an ESL 

young learner classroom in Indonesia. K@ta, 15(2), 117-124. 

https://doi.org/10.9744/kata.15.2.117-124  

Nation, I. S. P. (1990). Teaching and learning vocabulary. New York: Newbury 

House. 

Ok, S., & Ustact, H. Y. (2013). Preferences of ELT students on the strategies 

instructors use in the correction of grammar errors. International Journal of 

Business and Social Science, 4(8), 244–254. 

Panova, I., & Lyster, R. (2002). Patterns of corrective feedback and uptake in an 

adult ESL classroom. TESOL Quarterly, 36(4), 573–595. 

https://doi.org/10.2307/3588241  

Rezaei, S., & Derakhshan, A. (2011). Investigating recast and metalinguistic 

feedback in task-based grammar instruction. Journal of Language Teaching 

and Research, 2(3), 655-663. https://doi.org/10.4304/jltr.2.3.655-663  

Russell, V. (2009). Corrective feedback, over a decade of research since Lyster and 

Ranta (1997): Where do we stand today? Electronic Journal of Foreign 

Language Teaching, 6(1), 21–31. 

Sauro, S. (2009). Computer-mediated corrective feedback and the development of 

L2 grammar. Language Learning & Technology: A Refereed Journal for 

Second and Foreign Language Educators, 13(1), 96–120. 

Shahini, G., & Shahamirian, F. (2017). Improving English speaking fluency: The 

role of six factors. Advances in Language and Literary Studies, 8(6), 100–104. 

http://dx.doi.org/10.7575/aiac.alls.v.8n.6p.100  

Sheen, Y., & Ellis, R. (2011). Corrective feedback in language teaching. In E. 

Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning 

(Vol. 2, pp. 593–610). New York: Routledge. 

Shin, J. Y., Dixon, L. Q., & Choi, Y. (2020). An updated review on use of L1 in 

foreign language classrooms. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural 

Development, 41(5), 406–419. 
https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2019.1684928  

Storch, N., & Wigglesworth, G. (2003). Is there a role for the use of the L1 in an 

L2 setting?. TESOL Quarterly, 37(4), 760–769. 
https://doi.org/10.2307/3588224  

Taipale, P. (2012). Oral errors, corrective feedback and learner uptake in an EFL 

setting. Jyväskylä: University of Jyväskylä. 

Tersta, F. W. (2017). An Analysis of oral corrective feedback to scaffold students 

in learning spoken English. Bandung: The Indonesia University of Education. 

Tiley, W., & Rentler, B. (2022). Activating accurate vocabulary use in spoken 

English: Collocation-focused classroom action research at a Japanese 

University. APU Journal of Language Research, 7, 38–59. 

https://doi.org/10.34409/apujlr.7.0_38  

Widiati, U., & Cahyono, B. Y. (2006). The teaching of speaking in Indonesian 

context: The state of the art. BAHASA DAN SENI, 34(2), 269–292. 

Yang, J. (2016). Learners’ oral corrective feedback preferences in relation to their 

cultural background, proficiency level and types of error. System, 61, 75–86. 

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2016.08.004  

https://doi.org/10.9744/kata.15.2.117-124
https://doi.org/10.2307/3588241
https://doi.org/10.4304/jltr.2.3.655-663
http://dx.doi.org/10.7575/aiac.alls.v.8n.6p.100
https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2019.1684928
https://doi.org/10.2307/3588224
https://doi.org/10.34409/apujlr.7.0_38
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2016.08.004


 

LLT Journal, e-ISSN 2579-9533, p-ISSN 1410-7201, Vol. 26, No. 1, April 2023, pp. 304-313 
 

313 

 

Yilmaz, Y. (2016). The role of exposure condition in the effectiveness of explicit 

correction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(1), 65–96. 

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263115000212  

Young-Davy, B. (2014). Explicit vocabulary instruction. Ortesol Journal, 31, 26–

32. 

Yusuf, F. N., Widiati, U., & Sulistyo, T. (2017). Multimodal feedback provision in 

improving pre-service teachers’ competence. Indonesian Journal of Applied 

Linguistics, 7(2), 239-246. https://doi.org/10.17509/ijal.v7i2.8126  
  
 

https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263115000212
https://doi.org/10.17509/ijal.v7i2.8126