Open access journal: http://periodicos.uefs.br/ojs/index.php/sociobiology ISSN: 0361-6525 DOI: 10.13102/sociobiology.v61i3.345-347Sociobiology 61(3): 345-347 (September 2014) Brood hiding test: a new bioassay for behavioral and neuroethological ant research A Szczuka, B Symonowicz, J Korczyńska, A Wnuk, EJ Godzińska Introduction Adult workers of social Hymenoptera usually engage first in intranidal tasks and then switch to extranidal ones (Wilson, 1971; Hölldobler & Wilson, 2009). This transition is often called the transition nurse-forager (e.g., Heylen et al., 2008). However, older ant workers and/or foragers may retain the ability to engage in intranidal brood care (Lenoir, 1979; Sorensen et al., 1984; Seid & Traniello, 2006; Muscedere et al., 2009). Moreover, several studies revealed that ant foragers are more attracted to brood found outside the nest than nurses and show higher readiness to retrieve it to the nest (Weir, 1958; Walsh & Tschinkel, 1974; Lenoir, 1977; 1981). On the basis of these findings Lenoir (1977; 1981) proposed a bioassay acting as a reliable technique of identification of ant foragers: the brood-retrieving test. At the start of the test a simple artificial ant nest (a test tube equipped with a water reservoir and partly covered by a black paper tube to assure darkness in its humid zone) is inclined so that the brood falls on the dry cotton plug closing the other end of the tube. Workers inhabiting the nest may then retrieve brood back to the dark zone close Abstract We describe a new bioassay for behavioral and neuroethological ant research, the brood hiding test. A group of adult ants is taken out of the nest, confined together with brood and exposed to strong light. Ants may interact with brood, and, in particular, transport it to the provided shadowed area. The brood hiding test may be accompanied by administration of neuroactive compounds and/or by measurements of their levels in the brain and/or in specific brain structures. During pilot tests with workers of Formica polyctena the values of the score quantifying ant behavior were positively correlated with the group size. Sociobiology An international journal on social insects Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Warsaw, Poland. Article History Edited by Evandro N Silva - UEFS, Brazil Received 19 November 2013 Initial acceptance 30 March 2014 Final acceptance 08 July 2014 Keywords bioassay, brood, light, group size, Formica polyctena Corresponding author Ewa Joanna Godzińska Laboratory of Ethology Department of Neurophysiology Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology PAS, Pasteur St. 3, PL 02-093 Warsaw, Poland E-Mail: e.godzinska@nencki.gov.pl to the water reservoir. This test was successfully applied to identify about 80% of foragers in small colonies of Lasius niger L. However, it is less suitable for experiments carried out to evaluate the effects of various experimental treatments, as the ants are tested in their home nests, and neither the number of workers, nor the quantity and quality of brood can be easily controlled. Moreover, it is difficult to disentangle ant responses to humidity and illumination. Therefore, we developeda new bioassay for behavioral and neuroethological ant research, the brood hiding test. During the test a group of adult ants is taken out of the nest and confined together with brood in a container exposed to strong light. The ants may interact with brood and, in particular, transport it to the provided shadowed area. Experimental settings recommended for the application of that test to study the behavior of workers of the wood ant Formica polyctena Först (Table 1) were chosen on the basis of the results of 20 pilot tests during which workers taken from a laboratory colony fragment (1-20) were confined together with 5 homocolonial pupae in various types of containers exposed to strong white light (Fig. 1). Single workers did not SHORT NOTE A Szczuka et al. - Brood hiding test 346 show brood hiding behavior and their interactions with brood were limited to antennal contacts. More advanced interactions with brood appeared as a function of increasing group size. The values of the score quantifying the outcome of each test (1-9) were highly significantly positively correlated with the number of workers tested together (Spearman rank correlation test: r = 0.8536, P < 0.000001). (Fig. 1). This preliminary result was confirmed by another experiment in which brood hiding test was applied to compare the behavior of various subclasses of workers of F. polyctena (Szczuka et al. unpublished results). In that experiment foragers collected from the trails performed less well than workers from other experimental groups. As reported by Lenoir (1977; 1981), during the brood-retrieving tests brood is retrieved mostly by foragers. Responses to brood displayed by ants during the brood hiding test and the brood-retrieving test are thus mediated by at least partly different proximate mechanisms. The brood hiding test may be used to investigate such questions as behavioral polymorphism within ant colonies, ontogeny of ant behavior, nestmate and species recognition, and behavioral differences between ants from various Fig 1. The results of 20 min pilot tests (n = 20) investigating the responses of workers of the red wood ant Formica polyctena to homocolonial brood (5 pupae) exposed to strong white light (5000 lx) in various types of containers containing a shadowed area. Ant behavior was quantified as a score (1-9) denoting the most advanced form of interactions with brood observed during the test: 1: antennal contacts; 2: seizing; 3: attempt at transport (a short episode of transport no longer than 2- 3 s); 4. transport; 5. hiding of 1 pupa in the hadowed area; 6. hiding of 2 pupae; 7. hiding of 3 pupae; 8. hiding of 4 pupae; 9. hiding of 5 pupae. Two classes of nestmate workers were tested: nurses (taken out of artificial nest chambers from among workers employed in intranidal brood care), and foragers (taken from the foraging area of the same artificial nest). Element of experimental procedure Description Experimental arena an open cylindrical glass container (5 cm high, inner diameter 10 cm) with the walls coated with Fluon® Shadowed area a small (25 mm x 30 mm x 5 mm) rectangular shelter made of aluminum foil with one of its longer side walls (facing the center of the arena) left open to allow the ants to enter the shadowed zone Source of illumination strong white light (5000 lx) produced by the lamp “Fotovita FV- 10” (ULTRA-VIOL sp. j.). Chemical cues left by nestmates present (20 homocolonial ants are allowed to walk inside the container during 1 h preceding the first test carried out on a particular day) Brood 5 homocolonial worker pupae Time during which the ants are allowed to settle after the introduction into the test container and before the introduction of brood 30 min Duration of the test 15 or 20 min Recording of ant behavior digital video camcorder Analysis of behavioral recordings software for the analysis of video recordings of behavior [for instance, “The Observer Video-Pro” (Noldus Information Technology)] Table 1. Experimental settings recommended for the application of the brood hiding test to study worker-brood interactions in the red wood ant Formica polyctena Först. These findings provide a new example of the crucial role of group size in the mediation of the expression of specific behavior patterns in social Hymenoptera. The phenomenon of critical (threshold) group size necessary for the expression of a particular behavior trait was first described by Chauvin (1954) and then reported in many other studies [reviewed in Wilson (1971) and Szczuka & Godzińska (2004a)]. Particularly clear-cut effects of group size on the expression of a specific behavior pattern were documented in a series of experiments during which workers of F. polyctena were confronted with dead adult houseflies (Musca domestica L.) offered to them in the foraging areas of their nests (Szczuka & Godzińska 1997; 2000; 2004a; 2004b). The values of the score quantifying the responses of ants to prey increased as a function of increasing group size, and prey retrieval was observed only in groups counting at least 30-40 workers. Several studies also reported similar relationships between worker group size and the degree of escalation of ant aggressive behavior (Roulston et al., 2003; Tanner, 2006; 2008; Buczkowski & Bennett, 2008). During our pilot tests brood hiding scores tended to be lower in the case of foragers than in the case of nurses Sociobiology 61(3): 345-347 (September 2014) 347 systematic groups related both to phylogenetic distance and differences in ecology. Groups of individuals tested together may be homogenous, but may also consist of individuals belonging to different castes and/or worker subclasses. Not only homocolonial, but also allocolonial and/or allospecific brood may be used, and the tested ants may be subjected to the situation of choice between various categories of brood. Behavior of the tested ants may be quantified by assigning a score to the outcome of each test, or by video recording the tests and analyzing the recordings by means of an appropriate software. The brood hiding test may also be accompanied by the administration of neuroactive compounds delivered by various techniques including acute and chronic oral administration, injections, and topical (transcuticular) application. The variables quantifying ant behavior may also be analyzed as a function of levels of specific neuroactive compounds in the brain or specific brain structures. Such more complex versions of the brood hiding test may be applied to study neurobiological processes underlying such phenomena as task-related differences in responses to brood, effects of experience on expression of worker behavior, worker cooperation and communication, and inter-individual variability of behavior. Acknowledgements This study was supported by the project N N303 3068 33 (2007-2011) of the Ministry of Science and Higher Education (Poland). References Buczkowski, G. & Bennett, G. W. (2008) Aggressive interactions between the introduced Argentine ant, Linepithema humile and the native odorous house ant, Tapinoma sessile. Biological Invasions, 10: 1001-1013. doi: 10.1007/s10530-007-9179-9. Chauvin, R. 1954. Aspects sociaux des grandes fonctions chez l’abeille. La theorie du superorganisme. Insectes Sociaux, 1: 123- 131. Heylen, K., Gobin, B., Billen, J., Hu, T. – T., Arckens, L. & Huybrechts, R. S. (2008) Amfor expression in the honeybee brain: A trigger mechanism for nurse–forager transition. Journal of Insect Physiology, 54: 1400-1403. doi: 10.1016/j. jinsphys.2008.07015. Hölldobler, B. & Wilson, E. O. (2009) The superorganism. New York: W. W. Norton. Lenoir, A. (1977) Sur un nouveau test éthologique permettant d’étudier la division du travail chez la fourmi Lasius niger L. Comptes Rendus de l’Acadèmie de Sciences, Paris, , Série D, 284: 2557-2559. Lenoir, A. (1979) Le comportement alimentaire et la division du travail chez la fourmi Lasius niger. Bulletin Biologique de la France et de la Belgique, 113: 79-314. Lenoir, A. (1981) Brood retrieving in the ant, Lasius niger L. Sociobiology, 6: 153-178. Muscedere, M. L., Willey, T. A. & Traniello, J. F. A. (2009) Age and task efficiency in the ant Pheidole dentata: young minor workers are not specialist nurses. Animal Behaviour, 77: 911-918. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.018. Roulston, T. H., Buczkowski, G. & Silverman, J. (2003) Nestmate discrimination in ants: effect of bioassay on aggressive behavior. Insectes Sociaux, 50: 151-159. Seid, M. A. & Traniello, J. F. A. (2006) Age-related repertoire expansion and division of labor in Pheidole dentata (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): a new perspective on temporal polyethism and behavioral plasticity in ants. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 60: 631-644. Sorensen, A. A., Busch, T. M. & Vinson, S. B. (1984). Behavioral flexibility of temporal subcastes in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta, in response to food. Psyche, 91: 319-331. Szczuka, A. & Godzińska, E. J. (1997). The effect of past and present group size on responses to prey in the ant Formica polyctena Först. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 57: 135-150. Szczuka, A. & Godzińska, E. J. (2000) Group size: an important factor controlling the expression of predatory behaviour in workers of the wood ant Formica polyctena Först. Biological Bulletin of Poznan, 37: 139-152. Szczuka, A. & Godzińska, E. J. (2004a). The role of group size in the control of expression of predatory behavior in workers of the red wood ant Formica polyctena (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Sociobiology, 43: 295-325. Szczuka, A. & Godzińska, E. J. (2004b). The effect of gradual increase of group size on the expression of predatory behavior in workers of the red wood ant Formica polyctena (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Sociobiology, 43: 327-349. Tanner, C. J. (2006). Numerical assessment affects aggression and competitive ability: a team-fighting strategy for the ant Formica xerophila. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B., 273: 2737-2742. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3626. Tanner, C. J. (2008). Aggressive group behavior in the ant Formica xerophila is coordinated by direct nestmate contact. Animal Behaviour, 76: 1335-1347. Walsh, J. P. & Tschinkel, W. R. (1974) Brood recognition by contact pheromone in the imported fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Animal Behaviour, 22: 695-704. Weir, J. S. (1958) Polyethism in workers of the ant Myrmica (Part II). Insectes Sociaux, 5: 315-339. Wilson, E. O. (1971) The insect societies. Cambridge, MA: Belknap/Harvard University Press.