DOI: 10.13102/sociobiology.v62i4.392Sociobiology 62(4): 564-570 (December, 2015) Open access journal: http://periodicos.uefs.br/ojs/index.php/sociobiology ISSN: 0361-6525 Behavioral Responses of Apis mellifera Adult Workers to Odors from Healthy Brood and Diseased Brood Introduction Chalkbrood disease is an invasive mycosis in Apis mellifera ligustica (A. mellifera) produced by Ascosphaera apis (A. apis), which mainly affects larvae (Aronstein & Murray, 2010). Some colonies survive in areas that are seriously damaged by A. apis, which indicates the existence of tolerance in some genotypes of honey bees (Wallner, 1999). Hygienic behavior is the natural mechanism of disease resistance in honey bee colonies. One important aspect of hygienic behavior is the ability of individual bees to detect and respond to stimuli from diseased larvae early in the progression of the infection. The quick and efficient detection and removal of diseased larvae by these bees prevents the transmission of the disease throughout the colony. Unhealthy brood emit disease-related odorants, which trigger hygienic bees to recognize and remove diseased brood (Masterman et al., 1999; Spivak et al., 2003). The key factor in the tolerance of A. mellifera strains to chalkbrood disease is the honey bee’s sensitive olfactory perception that triggers the hygienic behavior (Francesco Abstract Studies of adult workers’ responses to infected brood were undertaken to isolate discrete volatile compounds that elicited honeybee hygienic behavior. Using a freeze-killed brood assay, we determined that in healthy colonies adult workers recognized and emptied infected cells with a 95% clearance rate. SPME-GC-MS results emptied >95% infected cells indicated differences in the composition and relative content of volatile compounds released by healthy and diseased brood. Additionally, we determined that the main volatile compound released from the pathogen Ascosphaera apis was phenethyl alcohol. The Y-tube olfactometer indicated that 10- to 20-day-old workers of healthy colonies, but only 15-day-old workers of diseased colonies, were significantly sensitive to differences in characteristic volatile compounds. This information could facilitate honey bee selection based on mechanisms that contribute to chalkbrood disease tolerance. Sociobiology An international journal on social insects HX Zhao1, Q Liang2, JH Lee2, XF Zhang1, WZ Huang1, HS Chen 1, YX Luo1 Article History Edited by Michael Hrncir, UFERSA, Brazil Received 04 April 2014 Initial acceptance 10 January 2015 Final acceptance 04 June 2015 Keywords Apis mellifera ligustica, Ascospheara apis, Volatile compounds, Olfactory, Y-tube olfactometer. Corresponding author Yuexiong Luo Guangdong Entomological Institute Guangzhou, 510260, China E-Mail: lyxbee@126.com et al., 2004). In a dark hive, diseased brood emitted special odors, which appeared to be the most important source of recognition cues. Volatile compounds mediated the capability of adult workers to recognize infested cells and empty them. Nazzi et al. (2002) and Nazzi and Della (2004) indicated that infected brood emitted short-chain unsaturated hydrocarbons to induce honey bee hygienic behavior. Masterman et al. (2001) suggested that the characteristic compounds of diseased brood inspired the behavioral response of adult workers. Swanson et al. (2009) detected three volatile compounds from larvae infected with A. apis that were absent from healthy larvae. Thus, we used the SPME-GC-MS (Solid phase microextraction- Gas chromatography and Mass spectrometry) method to measure these differences of volatile compounds (Swanson et al., 2009). Previously, Kraus (1990) indicated that adult workers produced an alarm pheromone repellent to Varroa destructor (V. destructor). Kraus (1993, 1994) used a Y-tube olfactometer to measure mite behavior, which is triggered by the odors of capped- cells over those of other life stages. The odors of capped-cells RESEARCH ARTICLE - BEES 1 - Guangdong Entomological Institute, Guangzhou, China 2 - Fujian Agriculture and Forestry University, Fuzhou, China Sociobiology 62(4): 564-570 (December, 2015) 565 belong to Volatile compounds produced by larvae. Swanson et al. (2009) indicated that chalkbrood-infected bee larvae emitted characteristic volatile compounds, which contained phenethyl acetate, 2-phenylethanol, and benzyl alcohol. The Y-tube olfactometer bioassay was used to test the orientation behavior of adult workers to volatile compounds. Our research aimed to measure the sensitivity of the workers olfactory perception to the characteristic volatile compounds of diseased brood. Here, we tested the ability of individual hygienic and non-hygienic bees to discriminate healthy brood from diseased brood. Colonies displaying diseased or healthy traits were tested for hygienic behavior using a freeze-killed brood assay (Spivak & Reuter, 1998a, 1998b), which is an indirect measure of a colony’s hygienic behavior. The number of dead pupae that were in the process of being removed (uncapped and/or partially removed), and the number completely removed from the cells were recorded every 24 h for the trial colonies. In the current study, only those colonies that had uncapped and removed more than 95% of the freeze-killed brood within 48 h were considered as hygienic behavior. We also used SPME-GC-MS to measure the volatile compound differences of diseased brood and healthy brood. Then, we used Y-tube olfactometer to measure the olfactory responses of non-hygienic bees and hygienic bees. The paper aim to veritify behavioral responses of Apis mellifera adult workers to odors from healthy brood and diseased brood. Materials and methods Biological materials Experiments were conducted at Fujian Agriculture and Forestry University during spring of the years 2011 and 2012. The six colonies of A. mellifera was divided into three infected -chalkbrood colonies and three healthy-larvae colonies. Experiments Freeze-killed brood assay measured the hygienic behavior We chose a sealed brood of white-eyed pupae containing approximately 300 cells on each side (5 × 6 cm) from a frame and froze it for 5 min in liquid nitrogen. We recorded the number of sealed cells and put the frame back in the colony. The tests were repeated on the same colony at least twice. Collection of volatiles released by infested-brood and healthy- brood cells Semiochemicals released by infested and uninfected worker brood cells were studied by means of SPME-GC-MS. We collected chalkbrood-infected larvae. Four types of A. mellifera larvae were used: a) five healthy fifth instars; b) five chalkbrood-infected larvae; c) five chalkbrood-infected larvae fed an artificial diet with fungal mycelia; and d) fungal mycelia (mummies) from potato dextrose agar (PDA) medium of A. apis. First, we prepared a larval diet that consisted of 50% royal jelly (Guangdong entomological institute bee industry co., Ltd, China), 6% D-glucose (Amresco, USA), 6% D-fructose, 37% double distilled water, and 1% yeast extract by volume (Aupinel et al., 2005). Prior to adding the diet to each cell, using 24-well tissue culture plates (BD Falcon™, USA) (Evans, 2004; Aupinel et al., 2005; Gregorc & Ellis, 2011), we homogeneously mixed and pre-warmed the food in an incubator 35ºC. On consecutive days, we transferred larvae to a clean culture plate containing fresh diet. We fed the larvae 350 µL of the diet daily (Aupinel et al., 2007). For the diseased brood we maintained the 350 µL of larvae food in individual wells, but added 1.0 × 108/ mL A. apis spores (Vandenberg & Shimanuki, 1987). A. apis gradually infected the larvae, reaching from the abdomen to the head by 5 d. The pathogenic fungus of A. apis was cultured on PDA medium for 5 days at 35ºC in darkness. PDA medium contains potato 200 g, anhydrous dextrose glucose 20 g, agar 20 g, distilled water 1,000 mL (Reynald et al., 2004). Healthy brood and diseased brood were removed from the 24-well plate with sterile forceps and placed in flasks (50 mL) that were then sealed with foil. Additionally, we analyzed fungal mycelia (mummies) from PDA medium of A. apis. Compounds released from healthy and diseased brood were studied by means of SPME-GC-MS. The 75µm CAR/ PDMS SPME fiber coating was exposed to a headspace gas phase for 30 min in the incubator at 35°C before immediate desorption into the GC-MS apparatus. Analytes were separated and analyzed by GC-MS using a Varian 450 gas chromatograph with a mass selective detector (CP-Sil 8 CB-MS 30 m×0.25 mm column, USA). The helium flow rate through the column was 1 mL/min in constant flow mode. The injector (270ºC worked in splitless mode. The initial column temperature was 40°C for 2 min, increasing from 40°C to 120ºC by 3ºC/min, and then rising to 250ºC at 5ºC/ min. The detector was held at 250ºC The electron-ionization mass spectra were obtained at 70 eV of ionization energy. Detection was performed in a full scan mode from 29 to 600 a.m.u. After integration, the fraction of each component in the total ion current was calculated. The above process was repeated three times to measure the volatile compounds of larvae. Y-tube olfactometer bioassay We tested the olfactory responses of different ages (1-,10-,15-, 20-, 25-, 30-, day-old workers) of honey bees to volatile compounds using a Y-tube olfactometer bioassay. The olfactometer apparatus was situated inside an incubator (RT 30ºC, RH 70%, dark room). The mix of volatile compounds were placed in one of the sample holding chambers of the olfactometer while the other sample holding chamber remained empty as a clean air “blank”. All the different aged bees tested remained in situ for 20 min, after which time the sample HX Zhao et al. – Behavioral Responses of Apis mellifera Adult Workers to Odors566 holding chambers were reversed to avoid any positional bias. Clean holding containers were used for next bee samples. Statistical analysis of data Frequencies of honey bees responding to treatment vs. control stimuli were compared using the χ2 tests for equal proportions. “*” indicates significantly different at α=0.05 (0.01
95% of freeze-killed brood within 48 h tended to remove diseased brood rapidly. Thus, we measured the clearance ratio of adult workers to remove the diseased brood. The frozen brood insert into the colonies to assess bee hygienic behavior after 48 h. Three healthy-larvae colonies uncapped and removed more than 95% of the killed brood by 48 h after insertion into the colony (Fig 1). However, in the same time period three diseased-brood colonies only uncapped and removed 45% of the killed brood. Volatile compounds of diseased brood and healthy brood Fig 1. Hygienic behavior of honey bee, Apis mellifera, adult workers in the healthy brood and diseased brood. Fig 2. Gas chromatography–mass spectrometry (GCMS) total ion chromatography of healthy and diseasedhoney bee, Apis mellifera, broods by solid phase microextraction (SPME) method. A: GCMS total ion chromatography of healthy brood by SPME method. B: GCMS total ion chromatography of diseased brood by SPME method. Sociobiology 62(4): 564-570 (December, 2015) 567 Differences in the amount of compounds released by healthy and diseased broods were assessed by comparing the size of the corresponding peaks in SPME-GC-MS analysis performed under the same conditions. Both the chalkbrood- infected brood in the healthy colonies and the chalkbrood- infected brood from an artificial diet with fungal mycelia emitted compounds with some similar ingredients. The results indicated that the differences between volatile compounds of healthy brood and diseased brood are in composition and relative content, especially that of tert-butylbenzene, E-β-ocimene, cosmene, 2-menthene, 2-isopropenyltoluene, and 1,3,5,5-tetramethyl-3- cyclohexadiene (Fig 2). E-β-ocimene was the main volatile compound of healthy brood phase compared with diseased brood. E-β-ocimene made up 1/5th of the relative volatile content from the healthy brood phase, but was only1/10th of the relative content from the diseased brood phase. The amount of tert-butylbenzene emitted by diseased brood was approximately five times more than the amount emitted by healthy brood, and 2-isopropenyltoluene was three times higher from the healthy brood phase than diseased brood phase. The relative contents of 2-menthene, cosmene, and 1,3,5,5-tetramethyl-3-cyclo- hexadiene, also showed some differences. Laboratory inoculation of PDA with chalkbrood disease causing A. apis fungal hyphae, released γ-Decalactone,2,5,5- Trimethyl-1-hexenbenzylalcohol, 2-(1,1-dimethylethyl)-3- methyl-Oxirane, phenethyl alcohol, phenethyl acetate, and benzyl alcohol (Fig 3). Y-tube olfactometer bioassay Fig 3. Gas chromatography–mass spectrometry (GCMS) total ion chromatography of fungal mycelia (mummies) from PDA medium of Ascosphaeraapis by solid phase microextraction (SPME) method. Compared with blank coloies, the 10- to 20-day-old workers of healthy colonies are significantly more sensitive to differences in characteristic volatiles compounds than workers of other ages, while only the 15-day-old workers of diseased colonies display a significantly heightened sensitivity to differences in characteristic volatiles compounds (Fig 4). Newly emerged workers have an immature olfactory sense that leads to a weaker sensitivity. Honey bees take less than 3 min to reach the junction of a Y-tube olfactometer. Even with the variation observed, the open Y-tube olfactometer is an efficient bioassay that showed workers could discriminate between the healthy larvae and diseased larvae. Discussion The honey bee is a model to observe the olfactory system of insects and it was observed that some colonies perform hygienic behavior to eliminate diseased brood. The stimuli triggering hygienic was thought to be olfactory cues emanating from cells containing diseased brood (Boecking & Spivak, 1999), but the identities of these cues were still unknown. Healthy colonies removed 95% frozen brood. However, HX Zhao et al. – Behavioral Responses of Apis mellifera Adult Workers to Odors568 Fig 4. Olfactory responses of honey bees,Apis mellifera,to odors from the characteristic volatile compounds and a blank. A: Olfactory responses of different ages of workers in the healthy colony. B: Olfactory responses of different ages of workers in the diseased colony. Note: * indicated significant different, Significant at α=0.05, (0.01
95% of freeze-killed pupae within 48 h will rapidly remove diseased brood (Swanson et al., 2009). Olfactory stimuli guided the detection and removal of diseased larvae (Masterman et al., 2001; Gramacho & Spivak, 2003; Spivak et al., 2003). Volatile compounds mediate olfactory stimuli. The diseased brood and healthy brood of A. mellifera released different volatile compounds that were distinguished by adult workers, and these differences affected the workers’ behavior. In the healthy colony, the detection of the diseased brood’s odor would trigger workers with even the lowest detection sensitivities, and they would rapidly begin uncapping and removing the diseased brood (Palacio et al., 2010). In a darkened hive, odors emitted from diseased brood appeared to be the most important source of recognition cues. Spivak and Boecking (2001) indicated that olfactory cues mediated the tolerance of A. mellifera strains by triggering the hygienic behavior to clean mites, and that the chemical compounds induced the ability of the bees to recognize infested cells and empty them (Spivak & Downey, 1999). Cells infested with mite emitted different volatile compounds than uninfected cells. Martin et al. (2001) suggested that the honey bee’s hygienic behavior was triggered by the polar compounds of the mite cuticle. Nazzi et al. (2001, 2004) indicated that there might be slight differences in the amount of some short-chain unsaturated hydrocarbons. Martin et al. (2001) showed that mites emitted volatile compounds to induce bee hygienic behavior. Masterman et al. (2001) suggested that olfactory cues from the diseased brood itself stimulates the behavioral response of bees. The body fluid of brood was very important as a stimulus for adult workers to remove treated brood (Aumeier et al., 2002). In our experiment, we used the SPME-GC-MS method to analyze the volatile compounds of healthy brood and diseased brood, which was infected with A. apis. Our results indicated a difference in volatile compound composition and relative contents, especially of tert-butylbenzene and E-β-ocimene. This was the first report on tert-butylbenzene. E-β-ocimene had previously been shown to be emitted during the queens mating and to play a role in regulating worker ovary development (Gilley et al., 2006). Additionally, larvae emit E-β-ocimene, which acts as a hunger signal. The 1- to 2-day-old larvae have a higher need for food than 3- to 4-day-old larvae, so younger larvae produce more E-β-ocimene than older larvae (Maisonnasse et al., 2009, 2010). Although diseased brood and healthy brood released similar volatile compounds, they had significant differences, and they especially emitted different amounts of E-ß-ocimene. We propose that the young infected brood needed little food and did Sociobiology 62(4): 564-570 (December, 2015) 569 not nurse, while the healthy brood needed more food and care. Thus, E-ß-ocimene was emitted as a characteristic compound, which could be used to discriminate between a healthy brood and diseased brood. Stimuli coming from both the diseased brood and the fungal pathogen, A. apis, were considered in our test. To discriminate which volatile compounds came from diseased brood, and were not emitted by A. apis, we measured the volatile compounds of A. apis in the artificial PDA culture medium. Completely different compounds were emitted. A. apis produced benzyl alcohol, phenethyl alcohol, and phenethyl acetate. These three compounds were reportedly emitted by diseased brood (Swanson et al., 2009). The field bioassay of topical applications and paraffin larval dummies revealed that phenethyl acetate induce hygienic behavior (Swanson et al., 2009). In our test, we detected phenethyl alcohol as the main volatile compound of A. apis, accounting for 1/4th of the relevant volatile compounds. However, the relative amount of benzyl alcohol and phenethyl acetate were <5%. In our test, we detected phenethyl alcohol and phenethyl acetate from diseased brood, but we could not detected benzyl alcohol from diseased brood. We found that diseased brood and healthy brood have significantly different volatile compounds, and the relative content of the main volatile compounds were also significantly different. To confirm that honey bees are able to recognize brood cells infested by A. apis, we used a Y-tube olfactometer to test the olfactory responses of workers. We verified the existence different chemical compounds of healthy brood and diseased brood, and then identified, the chemical stimuli that induces the hygienic behavior of bees towards A. apis infested cells. Actually, we chose five volatile compounds and analyzed their effects on the olfactory behavior of honey bees. The 10- to 20-day-old workers showed significant olfactory stimulated behavior in the healthy colony. It has been reported that 15-day- old workers performed hygienic activities (Invernizzi & Corbella 1999, Arathi et al., 2000). Moreover, 15-day-old workers rapidly initiated uncapping behaviors depending on the stimulus from a dead brood (Arathi, 2000). Middle-aged bees (12- to 21-day-old) built and maintained the hive, and received and processed nectar (Johnson, 2003, 2008a). They did not engage in nursing behavior (Ben-Shahar et al., 2002; Ben-Shahar, 2005). Workers displayed age-related division of labor and natural behavioral plasticity (LeConte & Hefetz, 2008; Ben-Shahar, 2005). The change from cell cleaning to nurse and other in-hive tasks were performed by 21-day-old workers (Whitfield, 1987). Our research showed that 10- to 20-day-old workers in hygienic colonies had the ability to discriminate characteristic odors. Only the 15-day-old workers in the non-hygienic colony had sensitive enough olfactory perception to differentiate preferential odors. Thus, 15-day-old workers showed a sensitive olfactory response, which correlated with the workers age. As a social insect, adult workers perform different tasks in keeping with temporal polyethism. The 10- to 20-day- old workers of the healthy colonies could select five volatile compounds by Y-tube olfactometer. These volatile compounds both detected in healthy brood and diseased brood, but they have the different relative contents. Thus, we proposed adult workers of some colonies have olfactory perception sensitive enough to detect diseased brood. This information could facilitate the selection of honey bees with pronounced hygienic behavior based on mechanisms that contribute to chalkbrood disease tolerance. Acknowledgments This work was supported by projects of the Natural Science Foundation of China (31302041) and the Modern Bees Industrial Technology System (CARS-45-KXJ-7 and CARS-45-SYZ-12). References Aronstein KA, Murray KD. (2010). Chalkbrood disease in honey bees. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology, 103: 20-29. Arathi HS, Burns I, Spivak M. (2000). Ethology of hygienic behavior in the honey bee Apis mellifera L. (Hymenoptera: Apidae): behavioral repertoire of hygienic bees. Ethology, 106: 365-379. Aupinel P, Medrzycki P, Fortini D, Michaud B, Tasei JN, Odoux JF. (2007). A new larval in vitro rearing method to test effects of pesticides on honey bee brood. Redia, 90, 91-94. Aupinel P, Fortini D, Dufour H, Tasei, JN, Michaud B, Odoux JF, Pham-Delegue MH. (2005). Improvement of artificial feeding in a standard in vitro method for rearing Apis mellifera larvae. Bulletin of Insectology, 58: 107-111. Aumeier P, Rosenkranz P, Francke W. (2002). Cuticular volatiles, attractivity of worker larvaeand invasion of brood cells by Varroa mites. A comparison of Africanized and European honeybees. Chemoecology, 12: 65-75. Boecking O, Spivak M. (1999). Behavioral defenses of honey bees against Varroa jacobsoni Oud Apidologie, 30: 141-158. Ben-Shahar Y, Leung HT, Pak WL. (2002). Division of labor in honey bee colonies is influenced by cGMP-dependent changes in phototaxis. Journal of Experimental Biology, 206: 2507-2515. Ben-Shahar Y. (2005). The foraging gene, behavioral plasticity, and honey bee division of labor. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 191: 987-994. Evans GW. (2004). The Environment of Childhood Poverty. American Psychologist, 59: 77-92. Francesco N, Giorgio D, Mauro D. (2004). A semiochemical from brood cells infested by Varroa destructor triggers hygienic behaviour in Apis mellifera. Apidologie, 35: 65-70. Gramacho KP, Spivak M. (2003). Differences in olfactory sensitivity and behavioral responses among honey bees bred for hygienic behavior. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 54: 472-479. HX Zhao et al. – Behavioral Responses of Apis mellifera Adult Workers to Odors570 Gregorc A, Ellis JD. (2011). Cell death localization in situ in laboratory reared honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) larvae treated with pesticides[J]. Pesticide Biochemistry and Physiology, 99: 200-207. Gilley DC, DeGrandi H, offman G, Hooper J. (2006). Volatile compounds emitted by live European honey bee(Apis mellifera L.) queens[J]. Journal Insect Physiology, 52: 520-527. Invernizzi C, Corbella E. (1999). Edad de las obreras que realizan comportamiento higiénico y otros comportamientos en lasabejas Apis mellifera. Revista de Etología, 2: 78-87. Johnson BR. (2008a). Global information sampling in the honey bee. Naturwissenschaften, 95: 523-530. Johnson BR. (2003). Organization of work in the honeybee: a compromise between division of labour and behavioural flexibility. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 270: 147-152. Kraus B. (1994). Factors influencing host choice of the honey bee parasite Varroa jacobsoni Oud. Experimental and Applied Acarology, 18: 435-443. Kraus B. (1993). Preferences of Varroa jacobsoni for honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) of different ages. Journal of Apicultural Research, 32: 57-64. Kraus B. (1990). Effects of honey-bee alarm pheromone compounds on the behaviour of Varroa jacobsoni. Apidologie, 21: 127-134. LeConte Y, Hefetz A. (2008). Primer pheromones in social hymenoptera. Annual Review of Entomology, 53: 523-542. Maisonnasse A, Lenoir JC, Beslay D, Crauser D, LeConte Y. (2010). E-β-ocimene, a Volatile Brood Pheromone Involved in Social Regulation in the Honey Bee Colony (Apis mellifera). PLoS ONE, 5(10): e13531. doi: 10.1371/ journal. pone. 0013531. Maisonnasse A, Lenoir JC, Costagliola G, Beslay D, Choteau F. (2009). A scientific note on E-β-ocimene, a new volatile primer pheromone that inhibits worker ovary development in honey bees. Apidologie, 40: 562-564. Masterman R, Ross R, Mesch KA, Spivak M. (2001). Olfactory and behavioral response thresholds to odors ofdiseased brood differ between hygienic and non-hygienic honey bees (Apis mellifera L.). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 187: 441-452. Martin C, Salvy M, Provost E, et al. (2001). Variations in chemical mimicry by the ectoparasitic mite Varroa jacobsoni to the developmental stage of the host honeybee Apis mellifera. Insect Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, 31: 365-379. Masterman R, Smith BH, Spivak M. (1999). Brood odordiscrimination abilities in hygienic honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) using proboscis extension reflex conditioning. Insect Behavior, 13: 87-101. Nazzi F, Milani N, Della VG, Nimis M. (2001). Semiochemicals from larval food affect the locomotory behaviour of Varroa destructor. Apidologie, 32: 149- 155. Nazzi F, Della G. (2004). A semiochemical from brood cells infested by Varroa destructor triggers hygienic behaviour in Apis mellifera. Apidologie, 35: 65-70. Nazzi F, Milani N, Della V. (2002). (Z)-8-Heptadecene from infested cells reduces the reproduction of Varroa destructor under laboratory conditions. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 28, 2181-2190. Palacio MA, Rodriguez E, Spivak M. et al. (2010). Hygienic behaviors of honey bees in response to brood experimentally pin- killed or infected with Ascosphaera apis. Apidologie, 41: 602-612. Reynald FJ, De Giusti MR, Alippi AM. (2004). Inhibition of the growth of Ascosphaera apis by Bacillus and Paenibacillus strains isolated from honey. Revista Argentina de Microbiologia, 36: 52-55. Rothenbuhler WC. (1964). Behavior genetics of nest cleaning in honey bees. Responses of four inbred lines to disease-killed brood. Animal Behavior, 12: 578-583. Spivak M, Masterman R, Ross R, Mesce KA. (2003). Hygienic behavior in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) and the modulatory role of octopamine. Neurobiology, 55: 341-354. Spivak M, Boecking O. (2001). Honey bee resistance to Varroa mites. In: Webster, T., Delaplane, K. (Eds.), Mites of the Honey Bee. Dadant & Sons, Hamilton, Illinois, 205-227. Spivak M, Downey DL. (1998). Field assays for hygienic behavior in disease resistance in honey bees (Apidae: Hymenoptera). Journal of Economic Entomology, 91: 64-70. Spivak M, Reuter GS. (1998a). Honey bee hygienic behavior. American Bee Journal, 138: 283-286. Spivak M, Reuter GS. (1998b). Performance of hygienic honey bee colonies in a commercial apiary. Apidologie, 29, 285-302. Swanson JA, Torto B, Kells SA, et al. (2009). Odorants that induce hygienic behavior in honeybees: identification of volatile compounds in chalkbrood-infected honeybee larvae. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 35: 1108-1116. Vandenberg JD, Shimanuki H. (1987). Technique for rearing worker honeybees in the laboratory. Apiculture Research, 26: 90-97. Wallner K. (1999). Varroacides and their residues in bee products. Apidologie, 30: 235-248. Whitfield CW, Ben-Shahar Y, Brillet C, et al. (1987). The Biology of the Honey Bee, vol. viii. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass. 281. Wilson-Rich N, Spivak M, Fefferman NH, Starks PT. (2009). Genetic, individual, and group facilitation of disease resistance in insect societies. Annual Review of Entomology, 54: 405-423.