Open access journal: http://periodicos.uefs.br/ojs/index.php/sociobiology ISSN: 0361-6525 DOI: 10.13102/sociobiology.v68i1.5188Sociobiology 68(1): e5188 (March, 2021) Introduction Several experiments have shown that animals can use the geomagnetic field (GMF) for orientation and homing using a magnetoreception process (Wiltschko & Wiltschko, 2005). Social insects, such as bees, ants and wasps, can use magnetoreception for magnetic orientation in guidance and navigation tasks (Wajnberg et al., 2010; Pereira-Bomfim et al., 2015). Unlike magnetic orientation, there are some reports of sensibility to magnetic fields (MF) by insects, where variations in MFs can change common behaviors. Vowles (1954) presented a pioneer study with the ant Myrmica laevinodis (Nylander, 1846). He studied the orientation of ants to gravity and used tiny particles of soft iron cemented to Abstract The present paper aims to study magnetosensibility and seek magnetic nanoparticles in ants. By living in colonies, the social insects developed very efficient methods of nestmate recognition, being less tolerant towards individuals from other colonies. Therefore, any strange behavior between nestmates and/or conspecifics, besides those present in their behavioral repertoire, is not expected. The present paper analyzes whether changes in the intensity of applied magnetic fields on Ectatomma brunneun (Smith) ants can cause changes in the typical pattern of interaction between conspecifics. We used a pair of coils generating a non-homogeneous magnetic field to change the whole local geomagnetic field. Magnetometry studies were done on abdomens and head + antennae using a SQUID magnetometer. The results show that changes in the geomagnetic field affect the usual pattern of interactions between workers from different colonies. The magnetometry results show that abdomens present superparamagnetic nanoparticles and heads present single-domain magnetic nanoparticles. Behavior experiments show for the first time that Ectatomma brunneun ants are magnetosensible. The change in nestmate recognition of Ectatomma ants observed while a magnetic field is applied can be associated with disturbance in a magnetosensor presented in the body based on magnetic nanoparticles. Sociobiology An international journal on social insects Márlon César Pereira1,2, Maria da Graça Cardoso Pereira-Bomfim2, Ingrid de Carvalho Guimarães2, Candida Anitta Pereira Rodrigues1,2, Jilder Peña Serna3, Daniel Acosta-Avalos3, William Fernando Antonialli-Junior2 Article History Edited by Evandro N. Silva, UEFS, Brazil Received 12 April 2020 Initial acceptance 26 January 2021 Final acceptance 19 March 2021 Publication date 31 March 2021 Keywords Aggression, Ectatomminae, magnetometry, insect behavior. Corresponding author Marlon César Pereira Universidade Federal da Grande Dourados, Cidade Universitária Rodovia Dourados-Itahum, Km 12, CEP 79804-970, Dourados-MS, Brasil. E-Mail: marloncesarp@yahoo.com.br different parts of the ant body to generate a magnetic couple, in the presence of a magnetic field of about 300 μT, to be added to the gravitational couple. He aimed to identify where the gravitation sensor is located in the ant body. During the experiments, Vowles observed that the magnetic couple does not alter the orientation to the gravitational force. Still, he observed that ants changed their behavior when the magnetic field was on: they stopped their movement or often cleaning their antennae when the iron particles were located in the funiculi or the scape of both antennae (Vowles, 1954). The study of Kermarrec (1981) showed that Acromyrmex octospinosus (Reich, 1793) ants are sensitive to strong static MFs provided by magnets (of about 19100 μT and 27300 μT), by avoidance reactions including the repeatable movement 1 - Programa de Pós-graduação em Entomologia e Conservação da Biodiversidade, Universidade Federal da Grande Dourados, Dourados-MS, Brazil 2 - Laboratorio de Ecologia Comportamental, Centro de Estudos em Recursos Naturais, Universidade Estadual de Mato Grosso do Sul, Dourados-MS, Brazil 3 - Centro Brasileiro de Pesquisas Fisicas, CBPF, Rio de Janeiro-RJ, Brazil RESEARCh ARTICLE - ANTS Magnetosensibility and Magnetic Properties of Ectatomma brunneun Smith, F. 1858 Ants mailto:marloncesarp@yahoo.com.br Marlon César Pereira et al. – Magnetosensibility of E. brunneum ants 2 of brood by workers. In the same study, no reaction was observed for magnetic fields of about 200 μT, and 500 μT. Anderson and VanderMeer (1993) showed that Solenopsis invicta (Buren, 1972) ants are sensitive to changes in the GMF direction analyzing the time of trail formation in an experimental arena. In those studies, with M. laevinodis, A. octospinosus, and S. invicta, the observed responses to MFs are examples of magnetosensibility, not necessarily related to orientation and homing. A magnetoreception mechanism essentially assumed in ants is the ferromagnetic hypothesis (Johnsen & Lohmann, 2005). It claims that the magnetic field transduction occurs through magnetic nanoparticles located inside some specialized cells associated with the nervous system. In Pachycondyla marginata (Roger, 1861) ants, the search for magnetic nanoparticles involved studies on their isolation (Acosta-Avalos et al., 1999), SQUID magnetometry (Wajnberg et al., 2004), ferromagnetic resonance (Wajnberg et al., 2000), and electron microscopy (Oliveira et al., 2010). Such studies allowed the identification of the antennae as the location of the magnetoreceptor. For other insects, such as Atta colombica (Guérin-Méneville, 1844) and Schwarziana quadripunctata (Lepeletier, 1836) (Lucano et al., 2006; Alves et al., 2014), similar studies identify the antennae as the body part with a higher magnetic signal. Like other social insects, Ants show a high degree of cooperation among the individuals that interact in the colony (Zinck et al., 2008). For instance, workers play an active role in nest construction, colony protection against predators, foraging, and brood care (Ratnieks et al., 2006). The colony’s integrity depends on social interactions and communication among nestmates (Crozier & Pamilo, 1996). Those abilities are well developed in ants, and individuals from a foreign nest are usually attacked and rejected from foreign colonies (Crozier & Pamilo, 1996; Sturgis & Gordon, 2012). However, the level of aggression towards conspecifics non-nestmates, i.e., during nestmate recognition, can vary among species (d’Ettorre & Lenoir, 2010). Ants, in general, control and defend their territory, where they extract resources (Newey et al., 2010). Encounters among foragers from different colonies can generate conflicts that can turn into clashes, resulting even in individuals’ death (Matthews & Matthews, 2010). In some cases, ants can be tolerable to neighbors, implying that nestmate recognition and aggression can be associated with colony-recognition odors from environmentally derived cues, not only to the genetically derived ones (Chen & Nonacs, 2000; Frizzi et al., 2015). As far as we know, magnetosensibility has not been applied to analyzing the aggressive behavior during nestmate recognition in the presence of different magnetic fields. The present study aimed to investigate magnetosensibility in Ectatomma brunneum (Smith). We hypothesized that the usual pattern of interaction among conspecifics could be affected by applied magnetic fields. We also looked for the presence of magnetic material in the ant’s body using SQUID magnetometry techniques. Materials and Methods Behavior experiments We used a pair of coils connected to a digital power supply (Skill Tech, model SKFA-05D) to test the effect of magnetic fields on ants’ intraspecific interactions, as described by Pereira-Bomfim et al. (2015). The coils had a 30 cm diameter with a space of 15 cm between them. They were built with 58 spirals of Cu wire 14 AWG (Figs 1A and 1C), generating a non-uniform static magnetic field (Fig 1B) applied to a plastic container called “arena of encounters” (Fig 1C). The coils’ axis was kept in the South/North direction, oriented with a compass. When the power supply was switched on (15 V, 1.16 A), the horizontal component of the magnetic field (open circles in Fig 1B) inside the coils presented its polarity inverted in the region from 4 cm to 12 cm (Figs 1A and 1B). The intensity of the GMF in the lab was -16 μT horizontal, 21 μT vertical, and 16 μT perpendicular, meaning a total intensity of 31 μT. To measure the intensity of the MF, we used a sheet of graph paper. In this paper, we measured the intensity value in the vertical, horizontal and perpendicular directions at every centimeter. We took all measurements using a gaussmeter (GlobalMag, Model TLMP-HALL 050). The total magnetic field generated is shown in Fig. 1B. The essential characteristics in the generated magnetic field, relative to the standard geomagnetic field, are the inversion in the vertical component (open squares in Fig 1B), changing the sign of the inclination, the increasing of the magnetic field intensity from the center to the periphery, and the U-form of the field intensity. From 5 cm to 9 cm, the magnetic field gets its lower value of about 100 μT (about three times the local value in the lab, see the open triangles in Fig 1B). The magnetic field generated by the pair of coils corresponds with a quadrupolar field generated by an anti- Helmholtz configuration. The electrical current circulates in the opposite direction in each coil (Youk 2005). In contrast, in a Helmholtz configuration, the current circulates in the same direction in each coil generating a dipolar field. This magnetic field configuration is interesting because it changes non-uniformly all the parameters of the local GMF (intensity, inclination and declination) and the increase in MF intensity is very low compared with that generated by magnets, as was done by Vowles (1954) and Kermarrec (1981). We collected a total of 56 foragers from six colonies of E. brunneum from their nests’ entrance. The colonies were in the campus of Universidade Estadual de Mato Grosso do Sul, Dourados, MS, Brazil. After transfer to the laboratory, we kept the individuals in a 250 ml plastic container with holes in the sides and lid to allow air to circulate, with water and molasses ad libitum, wrapped in red cellophane to minimize the stress caused by collection and luminosity. For acclimatization to laboratory conditions (24ºC and 70% of humidity), a period of 24 hours was considered. Then, we performed induced encounters among intraspecific workers. To this end, the Sociobiology 68(1): e5188 (March, 2021) 3 ants were transferred to the “arena of encounters” (Fig 1C), a container of 16cm x 10cm x 10cm, inside which there was a smaller container (6cm x 4cm x 3cm) in the central inner part, where we placed one ant. Then, after one minute, a second worker from another colony was placed in the arena, outside the smaller container. This smaller container was only removed after 1 minute, thus enabling the encounter among the two workers. We induced 24 encounters, peer to peer, among 48 workers of six colonies of the species E. brunneum whose nests were at varying distances from each other (Fig 2). As control of experimental conditions, we induced four nestmate encounters. We used each ant only once in the experiment to avoid pseudoreplication, and at each encounter, we used a new arena. We observed each pair interacting for 45 minutes in sessions of 15 minutes. In the first 15 minutes, the interactions occurred with the coils switched off, i.e., before the change in MF; for the next 15 minutes, the coils were switched on to generate a different MF in them; and in the last 15 minutes, the coils were switched off again. We kept the coils always in the same position to avoid their influence on the behavioral responses. To assess the level of aggression during the encounters, we scored behaviors from 0 to 2 as follows: 0 for ignoring (Thomas et al., 2004), touching and avoiding (Suarez et al., 1999); 1 for attempted seizure, seizure, antennal boxing, body lifting, gaster curling or aggression from one worker (Monnin & Peeters, 1999); and 2 for a fight, identified when both workers execute the aggressive behaviors (modified from Suarez et al., 1999). We observed the evolution in the aggressive behavior during each encounter and recorded scores when the behavior changed. For each encounter, we calculated the arithmetic mean of the score regarding the observed levels of aggression. Fig 1. Pair of coils configuration and the magnetic field generated. A. Representation of the orientation of the MF components measured among the coils. A* represents the arena of encounters. B. Intensity of the magnetic field components as a function of the distance among the coils. The first coil is positioned at x = 1cm and the second coil is at x = 15cm. Also is represented the total intensity of the magnetic field, whose variation corresponds with a quadrupolar field. C. Photography showing the pair of coils, made of 58 spirals of Cu wire 14 AWG. In the middle is shown a plastic box that corresponds to the “arena of encounters”. While functioning, the coils can increase the temperature. To assess whether the increase in temperature could lead to behavioral response changes, we photographed all encounters with a thermal camera (Testo® 870). We took thirty pictures at each step of the experiment from random encounters. With the aid of the software Testo IRSOFT, we selected ten random points of thermal photos from both the arena and ants’ bodies to establish the mean temperature of the ants’ body during the encounters and assess whether the ant temperature varies when coils are switched on/off. SQUID magnetometry To assess whether there are magnetic nanoparticles on the ant’s bodies, we collected individuals of E. brunneum (Eb). Fig 2. Distance in meters among the field colonies of Ectatomma brunneum used in the experimental encounters. Marlon César Pereira et al. – Magnetosensibility of E. brunneum ants 4 Afterward, we separated the heads + antennae (denominated only as head) and abdomens, kept these body parts in the fridge, and preserved them in a solution of 70% alcohol. We used a SQUID magnetometer (MPMS Quantum Design) to search for magnetic material in these body parts. Five abdomens and five heads of Eb were dried and placed inside a gelatine capsule, which we placed in a plastic straw fixed on the magnetometer’s sample holder for magnetization measurements. We took two magnetization measurements as a function of temperature: Zero Field Cooling (ZFC), where the sample is cooled from room temperature in the presence of a null magnetic field; and Field Cooling (FC), where the sample is cooled in the presence of a magnetic field, which was 10000 µT (100 Oe) in our measurements. After cooling the sample, a magnetic field was applied to it, and the magnetization was measured as the temperature increased. The temperature range was between 10 K and 330 K, under a magnetic field of 10000 µT (100 Oe). We took hysteresis measurements at 300 K or 150 K, on a magnetic field range between -1T and +1T (1T = 106 μT). Based on the hysteresis curves, we determined the saturation magnetization (MS), remanent magnetization (MR) and coercive field (HC). MS results from all contributions to magnetization, while MR is solely composed of the ferromagnetic component. As heads and abdomens have different sizes and masses, we calculated MS and MR, dividing the original values by analyzing the parts’ total mass. Statistics We evaluated the difference in aggression levels among the three experimental conditions (coils off, on, and off again) using a Kruskall-Wallis test. To assess whether there were significant differences between the thermal photos’ mean temperature during the same encounters, we applied a Kruskall-Wallis test. We performed all statistical tests using the free software R 3.2.1 version. Results Aggressive behavior We recorded a total of 1571 behaviors during the encounters between workers of E. brunneum. From this total, 43.7% occurred before the change in MF, 27.6% during the change in MF, and 28.7% after the change in MF. The main behaviors performed by workers of E. brunneum were seizure (10.6%), attempted seizure (9.73%) and antennal boxing (9.35%). During controls, we did not observe aggressive behavior (touch and antennation represented 100% of behaviors). The mean level of aggression between workers from different colonies of E. brunneum was 1.5 ± 0.3 before the change in MF, 1.1 ± 0.1 during the change in MF, and 1.3 ± 0.2 after the change in MF (Fig 3). The Kruskal-Wallis test showed significant differences between the groups (χ2 = 17.8 p < 0.05). Fig 3. A. Levels of aggression of Ectatomma brunneum as a function of the distance among colonies. The intensity of the aggression displayed was registered during induced encounters among workers from different colonies, before the change in MF, during the change in MF and after the change in MF. The encounter number is related to the distance among the colonies as shown in Fig 2. Each point represents the mean value of the aggression level and the bar the standard error. B. Boxplots for the levels of aggression displayed by workers of Ectatomma brunneum. Data acquired during the induced encounters, before, during and after changing the values of MF. The Kruskal-Wallis test shows that before, during, and after the coils switching on/off, there were no significant differences in the mean body temperature of E. brunneum workers (χ2 = 1.3 p > 0.052) (Fig 4). Magnetometry Fig 5 shows the ZFC and FC curves for the head and abdomen. All the curves are compatible with the presence of magnetic nanoparticles. For the abdomen, the nanoparticles show a strong dipolar interaction and a smaller size than the head. Sociobiology 68(1): e5188 (March, 2021) 5 As for the head, in low temperatures, we observed a sudden increase in magnetization. This phenomenon is characteristic of paramagnetic contributions due to spins not compensated on the surface of the nanoparticles (Peck et al., 2011). From Fig 5 is observed that the blocking temperature for the abdomen is 282 K that can be associated with nanoparticles with an average diameter of about 24 nm (considering magnetite as the magnetic material). For heads, the blocking temperature must be higher than 340 K associated with the presence of magnetic nanoparticles whose sizes are bigger than 26 nm (considering magnetite as the magnetic material). Fig 6 shows an example of magnetization curves for abdomens and heads after removing the diamagnetic + paramagnetic contributions. Table 1 shows the results at 300 K for the coercive field HC, the remanent magnetization MR and the saturation magnetization MS. Fig 7 shows MS and HC as a temperature function. The results show that the abdomen has higher MS values and lower HC values compared to the head. The value of MR/MS for the abdomen is compatible with the presence of superparamagnetic particles in the paramagnetic Fig 4. Boxplots for the distribution of ant body temperatures before the change in MF, during the change in MF and after the change in MF, for Ectatomma brunneum. Fig 5. Zero Field Cooling (ZFC) and Field Cooling (FC) curves from 10K to 330 K in the presence of a magnetic field of 10000 μT, for heads and abdomens of Ectatomma brunneum. Fig 6. Example of hysteresis curve obtained for heads and abdomens of Ectatomma brunneum. The graph shows the magnetization M as a function of the magnetic field B. The magnetic field increases from - 1T to +1T (1 T = 10000 Oe). The insert shows an amplification in the region from -200 Oe to +200 Oe, where can be observed the remanent magnetization MR and the coercive field HC. The magnetization is relative to the total mass. Marlon César Pereira et al. – Magnetosensibility of E. brunneum ants 6 state, producing higher MS values. On the other hand, those values for the head are compatible with single domain or blocked superparamagnetic nanoparticles, producing higher coercivities. The superparamagnetic fraction (fsp) was calculated as: fsp = [M(150K) - M(300K)]/M(150K) (1) The values obtained for fsp are compatible with the values for MR/MS: the abdomen shows a higher amount of superparamagnetic particles than the head. The crude curves M(B) showed diamagnetic + paramagnetic contributions as a function of temperature. From these outcomes, it is possible to calculate the paramagnetic susceptibility χPM showed in Table 1. The head shows a higher value of χPM compared to the value for the abdomen. That correlates with the low temperature behavior observed for heads in the ZFC-FC curves (Fig 5). PM’s higher value could mean that the nanoparticles in the head are single domains with defects in the surface producing an extra paramagnetic contribution (Peck et al., 2011). nests. This recognition ability and consequent intolerance among themselves are related to competition for resources (Temeless, 1994; Sanada-Morimura et al., 2003). This type of dispute has been described in the ants Pogonomyrmex barbatus (Smith, 1858) (Gordon, 1989), Pristomyrmex punctatus (Smith, 1860) (Sanada-Morimura et al., 2003), Linepithema humile (Mayr, 1868) (Thomas et al., 2004), and the termite Nasutitermes corniger (Motschulsky, 1855) (Dunn & Messier, 1999). We can explain the observed results through the change in the local temperature generated by the coils when they were turned on because of resistive losses. Some studies have reported that temperature changes can affect the behavior of various animal groups (Walther et al., 2002; Deutsch et al., 2008; Dell et al., 2011, 2014; Huey et al., 2012; Gilbert et al., 2014; Sunday et al., 2014; Vasseur et al., 2014; Woods et al., 2015). However, our results demonstrate that the temperature variation in the surface of the ants’ body is not statistically significant (Fig 4), so the temperature is not a stressing factor to explain the ant behavior observed. The fact of ants being intolerants to foreign ants is probably due to the ability to recognize non-nestmates through chemical signals, especially cuticular hydrocarbons already investigated in several studies (Jutsum et al., 1979; Liang & Silverman, 2000; Sorvari et al., 2008; van Zweden et al., 2009; van Zweden & D’ettorre, 2010; Helanterä et al., 2011). When the coils were turned on, the change in MF decreased the aggression score (see scores for During in Fig 3A). For E. brunneum, a significant decrease is observed only for further nests. When the coils were turned off, the aggression score increased, but it did not recover the same level as it had at the beginning of the encounter. Therefore, when the coils were turned on, the change in MF seems to have caused disorientation on the ants, consequently changing the pattern of interaction displayed when the coils were turned off. A piece of evidence that supports the previous comments is that during the encounters with the MF turned on, ants showed “immobility”, an atypical behavior of them, remaining still without even moving the antennae and, after a period, they started to clean their antennae more often than usual. Our results show for the first time that E. brunneum is magnetosensible, being perturbed by local changes in the MF. We cannot claim that MF changes affect the aggressive ant behavior but that changes in MF change the ant behavior. The magnetometry measurements show the presence of magnetic nanoparticles in the head and the abdomen of E. brunneum ants. In each case, the nanoparticles show different magnetic properties at room temperature: in the abdomen, Fig 7. Saturation magnetization (MS) and Coercive field (HC) as a function of the temperature, for heads and abdomens of Ectatomma brunneum. HC (Oe) MR (emu/g) MS (emu/g) MR/MS fsp χPM Abdomen 11 3.9x10-4 1.4x10-2 0.03 0.9 3.1x10-5 Head 160 4.9x10-4 1.3x10-3 0.38 0.1 9.1x10-5 Table 1. Parameters obtained from the hysteresis curve at 300 K: coercive field (HC), remanent magnetization (MR) and saturation magnetization (MS). The superparamagnetic fraction fsp was calculated as indicated by Eq. (1). χPM is the paramagnetic susceptibility. Discussion In normal conditions, under the influence of the geomagnetic field, we found higher levels of aggression among E. brunneum ants (scores for before in Fig 3A) whose nests were further apart in more than 100 m (Fig 2). This finding means that these ants are tolerant to individuals from nests in a radius of 100 m. Pereira et al. (2019) showed that E. brunneum colonies are more tolerant to ants from nearest Sociobiology 68(1): e5188 (March, 2021) 7 the nanoparticles are mainly in the superparamagnetic state, and in the head, the nanoparticles are mainly single domains and superparamagnetic nanoparticles in the blocked state. Magnetometry studies done in other ants have shown that all parts of the ant body are magnetic (Wajnberg et al., 2010), but the abdomen and the head show different magnetic properties. Those studies led to the proposal that the magnetosensor must be in the antennae for Pachycondyla marginata, Atta colombica, and Schwarziana quadripunctata (Wajnberg et al., 2004; Lucano et al., 2006; Alves et al., 2014). Our results show that the abdomen and the head have different magnetic properties, and both can host the magnetosensor responsible for the observed magnetosensibility. In both cases, the type of magnetosensor must be different. On the one hand, the abdomen has an arrangement of superparamagnetic nanoparticles sensitive to the magnetic field intensity. On the other hand, the head has an arrangement of magnetic single domains sensitive to the magnetic field vector’s intensity and direction (Johnsen & Lohmann, 2005). Our results and other studies (Wajnberg et al., 2010) show that ants can detect MF intensity changes. Oliveira et al. (2010) proposed that P. marginata ants can perceive the magnetic field through magnetic nanoparticles in the antennae. Since the orientation of those magnetic nanoparticles and their magnetic moment can be rapidly altered when ants walk inside the “arena of encounters”, because the MF is displaced from the center of the arena (Fig 1B), the increase in cleaning behavior suggests an attempt to adjust the magnetic sensor to the new MF configuration. Another evidence that the MF is perceived by ants and causes changes in behavior is that soon after the coils were turned off, the ants stopped performing the above- listed behaviors, and aggression levels increased again, but not to the same level of aggression shown before the coils were on. Conclusion Our results show that E. brunneum ants tolerate ants from other nests at distances lower than 100 m. For the first time, our results show that E. brunneum ants are magnetosensible, changing their behavior under the effect of applied MF. Their body presents magnetic nanoparticles with different properties in the abdomen and the head. As the MF changes are about three times the local magnetic field, the change in behavior can be associated with disturbance and/or disorientation in some magnetosensor located in the ant’s body. Our results do not discard the presence of a magnetoreceptor based on the radical pair mechanism. Future experiments must address the impact of MF on several ants’ physiological and ethological traits, to establish the origin of the reported magnetosensiblity and understand its biophysical basis. Acknowledgments The authors thank Universidade Federal da Grande Dourados, Universidade Estadual de Mato Grosso do Sul, Centro de Estudos em Recursos Naturais and Centro Brasileiro de Pesquisas Físicas for technical support, Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (Capes) and Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) for financial support, WF Antonialli- Junior acknowledges CNPq (nº 308182-/2019-7) and JP Serna acknowledges CNPq by PCI Grant. Authors’ contribution MC Pereira: conceptualization, methodology, investigation, formal analysis, writing, review & editing MGC Pereira-Bomfim: conceptualization, methodology, writing, review & editing WF Antonialli-Junior: conceptualization, methodology, writing, review & editing IC Guimarães: formal investigation, writing, review & editing JP Serna: formal investigation, writing, review & editing D Acosta-Avalos: formal investigation, writing, review & editing CAP Rodrigues: writing, review & editing References Acosta-Avalos, D., Wajnberg, E., Oliveira, O.S., Leal, I., Farina, M. & Esquivel, D.M.S. (1999). Isolation of magnetic nanoparticles from Pachycondyla marginata ants. Journal of Experimental Biology, 202: 2687-2692. Anderson, J.B. & Vander Meer, R.K. (1993). Magnetic orientation in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Naturwissenschaften, 80: 568-570. doi: 10.1007/BF01149274 Alves, O.C., Srygley, R.B., Riveros, A.J., Barbosa, M.A., Esquivel, D.M.S. & Wajnberg, E. (2014). Magnetic anisotropy and organization of nanoparticles in heads and antennae of neotropical leaf-cutter ants, Atta colombica. Journal of Physics D: Applied. Physics, 47: 435401. doi: 10.1088/0022- 3727/47/43/435401 Chen, J.S.C. & Nonacs, P. (2000). Nestmate recognition and intraspecific aggression based on environmental cues in Argentine ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Annals of the Entomological Society of America, 93: 1333-1337. doi: 10.1603/0013-8746(2000)093[1333:NRAIAB]2.0.CO;2 Crozier, R.H. & Pamilo, P. (1996). Evolution of Social Insect Colonies: Sex Allocation and Kin-Selection. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Dell, A.I., Pawa,r S. & Savage, V.M. (2011). Systematic variation in the temperature dependence of physiological and ecological traits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108: 10591-10596. doi 10.1073/pnas. 1015178108 Dell, A.I., Pawar, S & Savage, V.M. (2014). Temperature dependence of trophic interactions are driven by asymmetry of species responses and foraging strategy. Journal of Animal Ecology, 83: 70-84. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12081 Marlon César Pereira et al. – Magnetosensibility of E. brunneum ants 8 d’Ettorre, P. & Lenoir, A. (2010). Nestmate recognition. In: Lach L., Parr C., Abbott K., editors. Ant Ecology. 1st ed (pp. 194-209). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Deutsch, C.A., Tewksbury, J.J., Huey, R.B., Sheldon, K.S., Ghalambor, C.K., Haak, D. & Martin, P.R. (2008). Impacts of climate warming on terrestrial ectotherms across latitude. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,USA, 105: 6668-6672. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0709472105 Dunn, R. & Messier, S. (1999). Evidence for the opposite of the dear enemy phenomenon in termites. Journal of Insect Behavior, 12: 461-464. Frizzi, F., Ciofi, C., Dapporto, L., Natali, C., Chelazzi, G., Turillazzi, S. & Giacomo, Santini. (2015) The rules of aggression: How genetic, chemical and spatial factors affect intercolony fights in a dominant species, the mediterranean acrobat ant Crematogaster scutellaris. PLoS One 10: e0137919. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137919 Gilbert, B., Tunney, T.D., McCann, K.S., DeLong, J.P., Vasseur, D.A., Savage, V., Shurin, J.B., Dell, A.I., Barton, B.T., Harley, C.D.G., Kharouba, H.M., Kratina, P., Blanchard, J.L., Clements, C., Winder, M., Greig, H.S. & O’Connor, M.I. (2014). A bioenergetic framework for the temperature dependence of trophic interactions. Ecology Letters, 17: 902- 914. doi: 10.1111/ele.12307 Gordon, D.M. (1989). Ants distinguish neighbor from strangers. Oecologia, 81: 198-200. doi: 10.1007/BF00379806 Helanterä, H., Lee, Y.R., Drijfhout, F.P. & Martin, S.J. (2011). Genetic diversity, colony chemical phenotype, and nest mate recognition in the ant Formica fusca. Behavioral Ecology, 22: 710-716. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arr037 Huey, R.B., Kearney, M.R., Krockenberger, A., Holtum, J.A., Jess, M. & Williams, S.E. (2012). Predicting organismal vulnerability to climate warming: roles of behaviour, physiology and adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367: 1665-1679. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0005 Johnsen, S. & Lohmann, K.J. (2005). The physics and neurobiology of magnetoreception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6: 703-712. doi: 10.1038/nrn1745 Jutsum, A.R, Saunders, T.S. & Cherrett, J.M. (1979). Intraspecific aggression in the leaf-cutting ant Acromyrmex octospinosus. Animal Behaviour, 27: 839-844. doi: 10.1016/ 0003-3472(79)90021-6 Kermarrec, A. (1981). Sensibilite a un champ magnetique artificial et reaction d’evitement chez Acromyrmex octospinosus (Reich) (Formicidae, Attini). Insectes Sociaux, 28: 40-46. doi: 10.1007/BF02223621 Liang, D. & Silverman, J. (2000). “You are what you eat”: Diet modifies cuticular hydrocarbons and nestmate recognition in the Argentine ant, Linepithema humile. Naturwissenschaften, 897: 412-416. doi: 10.1007/s001140050752 Lucano, M.J., Cernicchiaro, G., Wajnberg, E & Esquivel, D.M.S. (2006). Stingless bee antennae: a magnetic sensory organ? Biometals, 19: 295-300. doi: 10.1007/s10534-005-0520-4 Matthews, R.W. & Matthews, J.R. (2010). Insect Behaviour. London, UK: Springer, 513 p. Monnin, T. & Peeters, C. (1999). Dominance hierarchy and reproductive conflicts among subordinates in a monogynous queenless ant. Behavioral Ecology, 10: 323-332. doi: 10.1093/ beheco/10.3.323 Newey, P.S., Robson, K.S.K.A. & Crozier, R.H. (2010). Weaver ants Oecoplylla smaragdina encounter nasty neighbors rather than dear enemies. Ecology, 9: 2366-2372. doi: 10.1890/09-0561.1 Oliveira, J.F., Wajnberg, E., Esquivel, D.M.S., Weinkauf, S., Winklhofer, M. & Hanzlik, M. (2010). Ant antennae: are they sites for magnetoreception? Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 7: 143-152. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2009.0102 Peck, M.A., Huh, Y., Skomski, R., Zhang, R., Kharel, P., Allison, M.D., Sellmyer, D.J. & Langell, M.A. (2011). Magnetic properties of NiO and (Ni,Zn)O nanoclusters. Journal of Applied Physics, 109: 07B518. doi: 10.1063/1.3556953 Pereira-Bomfim, M.G.C., Antonialli-Junior, W.F. & Acosta-Avalos, D. (2015). Effect of magnetic field on the foraging rhythm and behavior of the swarm-founding paper wasp Polybia paulista Ihering (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). Sociobiology, 62: 99-104. doi: 10.13102/sociobiology.v62i1. 99-104 Pereira, M.C., Firmino, E.L.B., Bernardi, R.C., Lima, L.D., Guimarães, I.C., Cardoso, C.A.L. & Antonialli-Junior, W.F. (2019). Dear enemy phenomenon in the ant Ectatomma brunneum (Formicidae: Ectatomminae): Chemical signals mediate intraspecifc aggressive interactons. Sociobiology, 66: 218-226. doi: 10.13102/sociobiology.v66i2.3554 Ratnieks, F.L.W., Foster, K.R. & Wenseleers, T. (2006). Conflict resolution in insect societies. Annual Review of Entomology, 51: 581-608. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.51.110104.151003 Sanada-Morimura, S., Minai, M., Yokoyama, M., Hirota, T., Satoh, T. & Obara, Y. (2003). Encounter-induced hostility to neighbors in the ant Pristomyrmex pungens. Behavioral Ecology, 14: 713-718. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arg057 Sorvari, J., Theodora, P., Turillazzi, S., Hakkarainen, H. & Sundsteöm, L. (2008). Food resources, chemical signaling, and nestmate recognition in the ant Formica aquilonia. Behavioral Ecology, 19: 441-447. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arm160 Sturgis, S.J. & Gordon, M.D. (2012). Nestmate recognition in ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): a review. Myrmecological News, 16: 101-110. Sociobiology 68(1): e5188 (March, 2021) 9 Suarez, A.V., Tsuitsui, N.D., Holway, D.A. & Case, T.J. (1999). Behavioral and genetic differentiation between native and introduced populations of the Argentine ant. Biological Invasions, 1: 43-53. doi: 10.1023/A:1010038413690 Sunday, J.M., Bates, A.E., Kearney, M.R, Colwell, R.K., Dulvy, N.K., Longino, J.T. & Huey, R.B. (2014). Thermal- safety margins and the necessity of thermoregulatory behavior across latitude and elevation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 111: 5610-5615. doi: 10.1073/ pnas.1316145111 Temeless, E.J. (1994). The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they “dear enemies?” Animal Behaviour, 47: 339-350. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1047 Thomas, M.L., Tsutsui, N.D. & Holway, D.A. (2004). Intraspecific competition influences the symmetry and intensity of aggression in the Argentine ant. Behavioral Ecology, 16: 472-481. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ari014 van Zweden, J.S, Dreier, S. & d’Ettorre, P. (2009). Disentangling environmental and heritable nestmate recognition cues in a carpenter ant. Journal of Insect Physiology, 55: 158-163. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2008.11.001 van Zweden, J.S. & d’Ettorre, P. (2010). Nestmate recognition in social insects and the role of hydrocarbons. In: Blomquist G.J., Bagnères A.G., editors. Insect hydrocarbons biology, biochemistry, and chemical ecology. 1st ed. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. p. 222 - 243. Vasseur, D.A., DeLong, J.P., Gilbert, B., Greig, H.S., Harley, C.D.G., McCann, K.S., Savage, V., Tunney, T.D. & O’Connor, M.I. (2014). Increased temperature variation poses a greater risk to species than climate warming. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281: 20132612. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2612 Vowles, D.M. (1954). The orientation of ants. II. Orientation to light, gravity and polarized light. Journal of Experimental Biology, 31: 356-375. Wajnberg, E., Acosta-Avalos, D., El-Jaick, L.J., Abraçado, L., Coelho, J.L.A., Bakuzis, A.F., Morais, P.C. & Esquivel, D.M.S. (2000). Electron paramagnetic resonance study of the migratory ant Pachycondyla marginata abdomens. Biophysical Journal, 78: 1018-1023. doi: 10.1016/S0006- 3495(00)76660-4 Wajnberg, E., Cernicchiaro, G. & Esquivel, D.M.S. (2004). Antennae: the strongest magnetic part of the migratory ant. Biometals, 17: 467-470. doi: 10.1023/B:BI OM.0000029443.93732.62 Wajnberg, E., Acosta-Avalos, D., Alves, O.C., de Oliveira, J.F., Srygley, R.B. & Esquivel, D.M.S. (2010). Magnetoreception in eusocial insects: an update. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 7: S207-S225. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2009.0526.focus Walther, G.R., Post, E., Convey, P., Menzel, A., Parmesank, C., Beebee, T.J.C., Fromentin, J.M., Hoegh-Guldberg, O. & Bairlein, F. (2002). Ecological responses to recent climate change. Nature 416: 389-395. doi: 10.1038/416389a Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. (2005). Magnetic orientation and magnetoreception in birds and other animals. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 191: 675-693. doi: 10.1007/ s00359-005-0627-7 Woods, H.A., Dillon, M.E. & Pincebourde, S. (2015). The roles of microclimatic diversity and of behavior in mediating the responses of ectotherms to climate change. Journal of Thermal Biology, 54: 86-97. doi: 10.1016/j. jtherbio.2014.10.002 Youk H (2005) Numerical study of quadrupole magnetic traps for neutral atoms: anti-Helmholtz cois and a U-chip. Canadian Undergraduate Physics Journal, 3: 13-18. Zinck, L., Hora, R.R., Chaline, N. & Jaisson, P. (2008). Low intraspecific aggression level in the polydomous and facultative polygynous ant Ectatomma tuberculatum. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata, 126: 211-216. doi: 10.1111/j.1570-7458.2007.00654.x