Open access journal: http://periodicos.uefs.br/ojs/index.php/sociobiology ISSN: 0361-6525 DOI: 10.13102/sociobiology.v68i1.5227Sociobiology 68(1): e-5227 (March, 2021) Introduction Between 15,000 and 20,000 plant species, including agricultural species, have flowers with poricidal dehiscence anthers (e.g. De Luca & Vallejo-Marín, 2013) and foraging insects usually release the plant pollen through thorax vibration in a behavior known as buzzing (Buchmann, 1983; Thorp, 2000; Portman et al., 2019). Alternatively, some bee species “scrape” pollen (Wille, 1963; Thorp, 2000), using their mouthpieces in a “milking” movement to collect pollen from the poricidal flowers (Portman et al., 2019). These behaviors Under standard greenhouse conditions, the tomato fruits resulting from spontaneous self-pollination are expected to be of lower quality than those from bee pollination. In addition, the use of species with different behaviors is expected to change productivity differently. To test these hypotheses, we evaluated the pollination services from the use of three native stingless bee species with distinct foraging behaviors, Melipona bicolor Lepeletier, 1836, Nannotrigona testaceicornis (Lepeletier, 1836), and Partamona helleri (Friese, 1900) during the blooming period of cherry tomatoes in greenhouses. Fruit quality parameters resulting from pollination experiments were measured and the acclimatization of the analyzed bee species was evaluated. Visits of M. bicolor and N. testaceicornis to the tomato flowers contributed significantly to increases in the average weight, seed number, and thickness of the pericarp (only for N. testaceicornis) of the fruits, compared to the spontaneous self- pollination treatment. Partamona helleri, however, did not show any pollen collection behavior in the experimental conditions. Although N. testaceicornis did not perform the buzzing behavior, fruits from its pollination were equivalent to fruits from pollination by M. bicolor. The use of bee species with different flower-visiting behaviors can optimize tomato pollination in greenhouses and help to standardize fruit weights, contributing significantly to the quality of the fruits and increasing productivity, with consequent increases in commercial value. Sociobiology An international journal on social insects Maira Coelho Moura-Moraes1,2, Wilson Frantine-Silva1, Maria Cristina Gaglianone1, Lucio Antonio O. Campos2 Article History Edited by Solange Augusto, UFU, Brazil Received 27 April 2020 Initial acceptance 05 October 2020 Final acceptance 31 October 2020 Publication date 22 February 2021 Keywords Buzzing, scraping, Melipona, Nannotrigona, protected cultivation, Solanum lycopersicum. Corresponding author Maira C. Moura-Moraes https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0518-5193 Universidade Estadual do Norte Fluminense Darcy Ribeiro – UENF Centro de Biociências e Biotecnologia, Laboratório de Ciências Ambientais Av. Alberto Lamego, 2000, Parque Califórnia CEP 28013-602, Campos dos Goytacazes, Rio de Janeiro, Brasil. E-Mail: mcoelhobio@gmail.com performed by a high diversity of pollinators is a key factor for quality enhancement in crop production (Westerkamp & Gottsberger, 2000), including tomatoes (Depra et al., 2014; Gaglianone et al., 2018). The tomato (Solanum lycopersicum L.) is an autogamous Solanaceae with functionally poricidal anthers that must be vibrated to release the pollen grains through the apical pores (McGregor, 1976). In open-field tomato crops, the wind action and bee visitation can promote the necessary vibration and pollen deposition on the flower stigma (Free, 1993; Depra et al., 2014; Gaglianone et al., 2018). On the 1 - Laboratório de Ciências Ambientais – LCA, Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia e Recursos Naturais, Universidade Estadual do Norte Fluminense Darcy Ribeiro – UENF, Campos dos Goytacazes, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil 2 - Departamento de Entomologia, Universidade Federal de Viçosa, Viçosa, Minas Gerais, Brazil RESEARCH ARTICLE - BEES The use of different stingless bee species to pollinate cherry tomatoes under protected cultivation Abstract MC Moura-Moraes, W Frantine-Silva, MC Gaglianone, LAO Campos – Stingless bee pollination in cherry tomatoes2 other hand, greenhouse crops have a scarcity of wind and animal pollination conditions that are usually counterbalanced by mechanically induced vibration and airflow (Bell et al., 2006). These pollination techniques increase production costs and represent losses to the commercial value (Picken, 1984; Westerkamp & Gottsberger, 2000; Cruz & Campos, 2009). Therefore, in recent years, the management of tomato pollinators in greenhouse crops has been tested through the introduction of bees including Amegilla (Notomegilla) chlorocyanea Cockerell (Hogendoorn et al., 2006); Apis mellifera L. (Bispo dos Santos et al., 2009); Xylocopa species (reviewed in Velthuis & van Doorn, 2006); and the stingless bees Nannotrigona perilampoides (Cresson) (Cauich et al., 2004; Palma et al., 2008) and Melipona quadrifasciata Lepeletier (Del Sarto et al., 2005; Bispo dos Santos et al., 2009; Hikawa & Miyanaga, 2009; Bartelli & Nogueira-Ferreira, 2014; Silva-Neto et al., 2018). The stingless bees Partamona helleri (Friese), Melipona bicolor Lepeletier, and Nannotrigona testaceicornis Lepeletier display different behavior during visits to poricidal anthers, buzzing (P. helleri and M. bicolor) and scraping (N. testaceicornis), and there is no information about these species on greenhouse crops. Although N. testaceicornis has been surveyed in some open- field crops (Dos Santos et al., 2008; Cruz & Campos, 2009) and P. helleri in wild plants in Brazil (Lopes De Carvalho et al., 1999, Ramalho, 2004, Taura & Laroca, 2004, Azevedo et al., 2007, Carvalho, 2007), we did not find any data about their interaction with plants in greenhouses. Interest in managing stingless bees for pollination of agricultural crops has increased considerably in recent years. This is due to the high richness of species, their little aggressive behavior with easy and known handling and multiplication of colonies (Nogueira-Neto, 1997). In the current study, we evaluated the viability of the stingless bees Melipona bicolor, Nannotrigona testaceicornis, and Partamona helleri for pollination of cherry tomatoes (Solanum lycopersicum) in greenhouses, as well as their effectiveness in simultaneous use. Our expectation was that pollination by more than one bee species would enhance the fruit set and quality of the fruits produced. Material and methods Crop implementation The study was conducted in two greenhouses at the Federal University of Viçosa (20°45’14”S e 42°52’53”W), municipality of Viçosa, MG (Brazil), implementing crops between April and July 2014. Cherry tomato seedlings (hybrid “Chipano®” - red) were obtained by sowing and after 30 days, 200 seedlings were transplanted to 8 kg pots filled with commercial vegetable substrate, individually fertilized, and equally distributed in two vegetation greenhouses (100 in each). The tomato plants were carried with only one stem, eliminating all lateral shoots. The anthesis of the flowers started 30 days after transplanting the seedlings. We used arch-shaped greenhouses 3.2 m high at the sides and 5.2 m at the top (total area of 108 m2), closed at the sides with 50 mesh antivirus screen, covered with 150 micron light diffuser anti- UV agricultural film. An automated irrigation system was programmed to irrigate plants twice a day with about 600 ml of water to each pot. The cover fertilization was applied to each pot every 20 days with 40 g of ammonium sulfate. Temperature and relative humidity data were obtained over 24 hours using Data Logger (HOBO® U10). The experiment lasted 120 days. Beehives placement We selected strong nests (with large numbers of individuals) of Melipona bicolor, Nannotrigona testaceicornis, and Partamona helleri, from the Central Apiary of the Federal University of Viçosa. The number of individuals was estimated indirectly from the observation of a large number of brood cells and food storage pots. We opted not to use colonies with reduced food reserves and few brood cells due to the risk of loss during the experiment, as it was conducted during a period considered as low flowering when colonies generally decrease the production and storage of resources. The nests were transported from the Central Apiary and installed in the greenhouses seven days before the anthesis of the first flowers. In greenhouse A, we installed one nest of M. bicolor and two nests of N. testaceicornis, and in greenhouse B we installed two nests of N. testaceicornis and one of P. helleri. The nests were kept closed for 07 days before being opened to provide access to the interior of the greenhouses. The access was opened at night. In order to minimize the effects of temperature on nest development and bee behavior, the nests were installed on 1.5 m high supports positioned outside the greenhouse and their entrances were connected to the insides through a PVC pipe. Only P. helleri was kept inside the greenhouse in order to preserve the nest entrance architecture and facilitate the return of these bees to the nest. Throughout the study, artificial feeders containing aqueous honey solution (70%) and water were placed inside the greenhouse to supply the nests with energetic food (tomato flowers do not produce nectar). We randomly distributed geometric shapes (square, triangle, and round) of different colors (yellow, green, and black) throughout the greenhouse in order to assist the flight orientation of the bees. Bees learn and memorize visual features such as shapes, patterns, direction, depth, contrast, intensity, and movement of light that are important in flight control and use visual coordinates to explore places of interest such as nest and flower locations (Srinivasan, 1994; Giurfa & Lehrer, 2001). Pollination tests and behavior of the bees During the pollination experiments, we also recorded the pollination behavior and average time spent on each single visit by M. bicolor and N. testaceicornis workers between 08:00 and 11:00 on two non-consecutive days. Sociobiology 68(1): e-5227 (March, 2021) 3 The flowers of the fourth branch of both plantations were subjected to the following treatments: (i) spontaneous self-pollination (SP): 20 branches with flowers in pre-anthesis phase were marked and protected with organza bags (16x30 cm) to avoid any eventual visit; (ii) induced self-pollination (ISP): 20 branches were protected with organza bags and manually vibrated using cuttings during anthesis phase (Higuti et al., 2010); (iii) hand cross-pollination (HCP): the pre-anthesis flowers in 20 branches were covered and then manually pollinated 24 hours after anthesis, touching the stigmatic surface with a pollen mixture extracted from anthers of 20 flowers from different plants; (iv) pollination by M. bicolor and N. testaceicornis (MBNT): open flowers from 20 branches were available for visits of the two bee species during three consecutive days; (v) pollination by M. bicolor (MB): same as in the MBNT treatment, but only visited by M. bicolor; (vi) pollination by N. testaceicornis (NT): performed as MB treatment, but preventing foraging of M. bicolor by closing its nest; (vii) pollination by any bee (BP): visits inferred through necrotic marks on the anthers, regardless of the visitor species; (viii) single visit by M. bicolor (SMB): 60 flowers monitored and bagged after the single visit; (ix) single visit by N. testaceicornis (SNT): 60 flowers monitored and bagged after the single visit; and (x) single visit by P. helleri (SPH): 60 flowers monitored and bagged after the single visit. The flowers or inflorescences were protected with an organza bag immediately after the completion of each of the treatments mentioned above. To perform the hand cross-pollination test (HCP), stigma receptivity was tested on 10 pre-harvest flowers and 10 open flowers after 24 h using 3% hydrogen peroxide. The receptivity was confirmed by detecting the air bubble formation on the surface of the stigma due to the activity of the peroxidase enzyme, indicating that the stigma was receptive (Kearns & Inouye, 1993). In all treatments, the bags were removed after the beginning of fruit formation. The complementarity of the pollination services of the three bee species was analyzed through comparisons of the fruit quality parameters resulting from the experiments. To determine the influence of pollination tests on tomato production and fruit quality, the first three fruits of each branch were harvested, and five attributes were verified: (1) weight, (2) number of seeds, (3) transverse diameter, (4) longitudinal diameter, and (5) pericarp thickness. Fruit weight was determined with the aid of a precision scale. Transverse diameter, longitudinal diameter, and pericarp thickness were measured using a digital caliper. The number of seeds was manually counted by opening the fruit and removing its pulp to access the seeds. For the data analysis, we tested data normality using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. To evaluate the independence of the tested variables we used the Pearson’s correlation coefficient. We assumed that width and height would be previously correlated, then we expressed this relationship resuming the values as volume in cm3 considering an irregular sphere: , where r is the radius for each sphere axis. We used the Kruskal-Wallis and the pairwise Dunn post hoc tests with the sequential Bonferroni correction (p < 0.05) to assess the differences among fruit parameters from the 4th branch in the different pollination treatments (SP, ISP, HCP, MBNT, MB, NT, BP, SMB, and SNT) (Zar, 1999). In addition, we evaluated the median differences from the SP treatment through the box-plot method, since sensitive median differences may be important when evaluating production increases. All tests were performed at a significance level of 5%, using PAST v 3.26 (Hammer et al., 2001). Results Stingless bee behavior Regarding the species behavior, after opening the entrances of the M. bicolor, N. testaceicornis, and P. helleri nests to access the greenhouse, foragers of the three species displayed the behavior of flying towards sunlight and colliding with the screen, where they remained and consequently died of exhaustion. Flight behavior toward sunlight ceased after three days of confinement. After this period, outside activities were restricted to short flights to remove trash from the nests. Sixteen days after the opening of the P. helleri nest entrance, some foragers were observed to be under the petals, branches, and leaves. However, the pollination tests for P. helleri (SPH) were not possible, as this species did not show any pollen collection behavior under the experimental conditions. During the experiment period in which P. helleri was confined, the average temperature in the greenhouse was 19.28ºC (max 32.09°C and min. 10.05°C) and average relative air humidity was 85% (max. 98.53% and min 62.44%). Twenty-four days after the opening of the nest entrances to the interior of greenhouse A, foragers of the species M. bicolor and N. testaceicornis began the visits to the flowers (Fig 1). The average temperature during the period in which the bees were confined in this greenhouse was 19.83°C (max 35.04°C and min. 9.68°C) and average relative air humidity was 82.21% (max. 97.14% and min 47.86%). In greenhouse B, the foragers of N. testaceicornis began foraging in tomato flowers 27 days after the opening of the nest entrance to the greenhouse. Foragers of N. testaceicornis and M. bicolor started foraging activities in both greenhouses at around 8:00 am and ceased at 11:00 am, with the highest activity recorded at 09:30 am. During the period in which both species had free access, foragers of N. testaceicornis always drove away M. bicolor foragers from the flowers and then remained collecting the pollen grains exposed after the M. bicolor visit, as well as the pollen grains inside the anthers. No more interactions were observed between the two species on flowers. It was very common to observe two foragers of N. testaceicornis collecting pollen from the same flower. MC Moura-Moraes, W Frantine-Silva, MC Gaglianone, LAO Campos – Stingless bee pollination in cherry tomatoes4 Bees of N. testaceicornis showed a high floral constancy, randomly foraging flowers in either the same or distinct plants. When landing on the anthers, the foragers of N. testaceicornis went to the apical portion of the anthers and circled around it, introducing their glossa into the anther’s pore in a behavior of scraping the extremities. Pollen grains accumulated in the ventral portion of the thorax during visits were transferred to the corbiculae. When foraging the flowers, M. bicolor vibrated the flowers of the tomatoes by buzzing. The individuals of M. bicolor explored several flowers of the same plant before moving to the next plant, almost always in the same line. Flowers on the first day of anthesis were not visited by M. bicolor or N. testaceicornis foragers. On the 2nd day of anthesis, M. bicolor and N. testaceicornis performed longer visits on flowers not yet visited, compared to flowers that had already been visited by any bee. The visits of M. bicolor lasted on average 12 s (22 ± 7 s) on unvisited flowers and 3.8 s (6 ± 2) on flowers already visited on the same day. Similarly, N. testaceicornis spent an average of 91 s (140 ± 50) on unvisited flowers and 29.5 s (42 ± 11) on already visited flowers. Visited flowers could be recognized by necrosis marks on the anthers (Fig 1). Pollination experiments The results from the greenhouse A experiment showed significant differences in all pollination tests and attributes evaluated, except for pericarp thickness (Fig 2a), when compared to fruits from spontaneous self-pollination treatment (SP) (Table 1). Fruits from bee pollination treatments were an average of 4.69 ± 0.71 g heavier and had 52.44 ± 5.3 more seeds than those from SP treatment. In addition, pericarp thickness was larger in the treatment with pollination by N. testaceicornis (NT) Fig 1. Visitation and necrotic lesions in flowers of cherry tomatoes (Solanum lycopersicum L). (a) visitation by N. testaceicornis buzzing behaviour; (b) visitation by M. bicolor pollination scraping behavior; (c) necrotic lesions left by N. testaceicornis; (d) necrotic lesions left by M. bicolor visitation. Black arrows indicate the exact point of the lesions in both flowers. Sociobiology 68(1): e-5227 (March, 2021) 5 (3.41 ± 0.16 cm) compared to SP (2.93 ± 0.65) in greenhouse B. On the other hand, we did not find consistent significant differences between treatments involving induced self- pollination (ISP) or hand cross-pollination (HCP) compared to the bee treatments. Fruits from SNT, MB, and MBNT showed significant differences (p < 0.05) only for weight (Fig 2b) and number of seeds (Fig 2c), compared to HCP in greenhouse A, and NT compared to HCP in greenhouse B. Transverse and longitudinal diameters, and therefore the volume were highly correlated with the weight (Person’s r > 0.96, p < 0.001), and the statistical tests followed the same pattern throughout the different comparisons. Fig 2. Box-plot and statistic differences among four parameters of cherry tomatoes measured in the present study in greenhouses A (blue) and B (yellow). (a) Pericarp thickness; (b) Weight; (c) number of seeds; (d) volume derived from transverse and longitudinal diameters. Boxplot bars depicts quartile intervals, inner line the median position, out lines the boundaries of the distribution and circle the outliers. Letters over the box-plots indicate the statistical differences (p < 0.05) according to the Kruskal-Wallis and Dunn’s post hoc tests. Boxplot bars depicts quartile intervals, Spontaneous self-pollination (SP); induced self-pollination (ISP); hand cross pollination (HCP); pollination by M. bicolor and N. testaceicornis (MBNT); pollination by M. bicolor (MB); pollination by N. testaceicornis (NT); pollination by bees (BP); single visit pollination by M. bicolor (SMB); and single visit pollination by N. testaceicornis (SNT). Greenhouse Experiment N Weight (g) Number of seeds Transverse diameter (mm) Longitudinal diameter (mm) Pericarp thickness (mm) A SP 60 9.00 ± 0.53 a 18.41 ± 2.95 a 23.11 ± 0.50 a 24.83 ± 0.58 a 4.00 ± 0.56 a ISP 64 12.70 ± 0.50 b,c 52.98 ± 3.43 b 26.62 ± 0.35 b,c 28.28 ± 0.41 b.d 3.71 ± 0.08 a HCP 39 11.07 ± 0.56 a,c 56.74 ± 2.71 b 25.32 ± 0.45 a.b 26.76 ± 0.50 a,b,c 3.50 ± 0.12 a BP 56 13.34 ± 0.57 b,c 66.71 ± 2.57 b,c 26.95 ± 0.43 b,c 29.11 ± 0.47 d 3.72 ± 0.07 a MB 122 13.39 ± 0.27 b 70.11 ± 1.37 c 27.27 ± 0.20 c 29.13 ± 0.23 d 3.57 ± 0.04 a MBNT 20 14.64 ± 0.79 b 73.65 ± 4.97 c 27.96 ± 0.62 c 29.98 ± 0.64 d 3.70 ± 0.12 a SNT 17 14.37 ± 0.89 b 72.58 ± 3.87 b,c 27.70 ± 0.84 c 30.13 ± 0.64 d 3.84 ± 0.15 a SMB 30 12.71 ± 0.62 b,c 71.20 ± 4.00 b,c 26.76 ± 0.44 b,c 28.46 ± 0.52 c,d 3.63 ± 0.10 a B SP 60 7.03 ± 0.45 a 20.45 ± 3.48 a 21.52 ± 0.08 a 22.43 ± 0.52 a 2.93 ± 3.48 a ISP 63 11.21 ± 0.38 b 45.86 ± 3.21 b 25.68 ± 0.31 b 26.94 ± 0.34 b 3.48 ± 0.07 b HCP 31 8.74 ± 0.47 a,c 39.06 ± 3.56 b,c 23.92 ± 0.53 a,b 24.54 ± 0.48 a 3.00 ± 0.12 a,c NT 102 10.42 ± 0.34 b,c 52.48 ± 2.38 c 24.96 ± 0.31 b 26.43 ± 0.35 b 3.41 ± 0.16 b,c Table 1. Means followed by different letters (a-d) represent statistical differences (p <0.05) for the same attribute by the Kruskal-Wallis test and Dunn’s posterior test. MC Moura-Moraes, W Frantine-Silva, MC Gaglianone, LAO Campos – Stingless bee pollination in cherry tomatoes6 Discussion The behavior on the first days after the transfer of M. bicolor, N. testaceicornis, and P. helleri nests into the greenhouse was similar to that reported in studies with other stingless bee species (Cauich et al., 2004; Cruz et al., 2004; Bomfim et al., 2014). The limited size of an artificial environment such as a greenhouse imposes physical barriers to flying activities, with collisions against the greenhouse cover being very common in the first days (Slaa, 2006). Older foragers already have certain established habits and experiences from the environment in which they were previously maintained, so they have greater difficulty acclimating to the protected environment (Free, 1993). This explains the high mortality of foragers of the three species studied in the first days of introduction of the nests in the greenhouse. Acclimatization of stingless bees to greenhouse conditions can vary greatly between species and between colonies of the same species (Cauich et al., 2004; Cruz et al., 2004; Bartelli et al., 2014; Bartelli & Nogueira-Ferreira, 2014; Bomfim et al., 2014; Da Silva et al., 2016; Silva-Neto et al., 2018). For instance, foragers of Nannotrigona perilampoides (Cresson, 1878) may take from five to nine days to consistently start their activities in tomato greenhouse crops (Cauich et al., 2004). In different crops, such as small watermelons, colonies of Scaptotrigona sp. started foraging after the second day and M. subnitida started to explore resources on bell pepper flowers after seven days in the greenhouse (Cruz et al., 2004; Bomfim et al. 2014). The acclimatization time of Melipona species seems to be variable; Bartelli et al. (2014) observed that it took 22 days for M. quadrifasciata workers to start foraging tomato flowers. Bumblebee species on the other hand might expend 9 to 10 days for acclimatation (Morandin et al., 2001a). Partamona helleri was not considered a pollinator of cherry tomatoes under the conditions of this study. Several factors may be involved in this outcome, such as lack of interest in the floral resources or inability to acclimate to greenhouse conditions (Bomfim et al., 2014). A determining factor for bees to use their functions is a temperature not exceeding 30 °C (Brand, 2005; Kiss, 2006). In the greenhouses, the maximum temperature recorded during the day was 32.09 °C and the relative maximum humidity was 98.53%. The temperature is the factor that most influences the internal and external activities of the colony; when very high, foraging decreases and ventilatory behavior increases in order to stabilize the internal temperature of the colony (Michener, 2000; Roubik, 1989; Teixeira & Campos, 2005). According to Kleinert et al. (2009) stingless bees have excellent foraging activity when the temperature remains between 20 and 30°C, and the relative humidity values are between 30% and 70%. Foraging activities were concentrated in the early hours of the day, when the temperature in the greenhouse was mild, which agrees with observations from other studies for different stingless bee species (Cruz et al., 2004; Teixeira & Campos, 2005; Nunes-Silva et al., 2013; Bomfim et al., 2014; Silva-Neto et al., 2018). However, one factor that may have caused the absence of foraging activity observed in the afternoon is the high number of visits per flower in the morning, leading to pollen depletion. Some bee species are able to evaluate the amount of pollen available during the visit (Buchmann & Cane, 1989; Shelly et al., 2000; Nunes-Silva et al., 2013), and the perception that flowers would have a low amount of pollen may lead to the bees no longer visiting these flowers. Some studies reported that two visits per flower are enough for pollination of tomatoes in greenhouses and that higher levels of necrotic markings on the anthers lead to a decrease in return visits (Morandin et al., 2001b). The foraging behavior of M. bicolor exploring several flowers on one plant is expected and often described for species from the genus Melipona (Camargo, 1972; Cruz et al., 2004). Fruits from flowers visited only by N. testaceicornis in both single and multiple visits, showed higher average values in all analyzed parameters when compared to fruits from flowers visited by M. bicolor. Previous studies reported that bee species from the genus Melipona were efficient pollinators of tomatoes (Del Sarto et al., 2005; Bispo dos Santos et al., 2009; Hikawa & Miyanaga, 2009; Bartelli & Nogueira-Ferreira, 2014; Silva-Neto et al., 2018). Although we did not measure the pollen grain removal rate, the fact that all evaluated attributes present values similar to the fruits of the Induced self-pollination tests (ISP) and higher than those found in self-pollinated fruits (SP) corroborates the effective pollination in the different types of foraging, scraping (N. testaceicornis) and buzzing (M. bicolor) (Cauich et al., 2004; Del Sarto et al., 2005; Dos Santos et al., 2008; Palma et al., 2008; Bispo dos Santos et al., 2009; Hikawa & Miyanaga, 2009; Roselino et al., 2009; Bartelli & Nogueira- Ferreira, 2014; Silva-Neto et al., 2018). In addition to foraging behavior, the time spent visiting flowers seems to maximize the distribution of pollen grains on stigma, essential for fruit development (Mann, 1943). The differences in visit time between M. bicolor and N. testaceicornis species may be related to different visitation behaviors. The buzzing behavior of M. bicolor can be considered more efficient for pollen extraction and lower energy expenditure, when compared to the time spent in each visit of N. testaceicornis. The time spent by N. testaceicornis may be the main factor in maintaining a greater floral constancy in flowers of the same and nearby plants, which could contribute to higher mean values of the analyzed tomato parameters. Even without statistical significance, we can highlight the higher medians and quartiles distribution found in the weight and number of seeds of the fruits produced from the visit of the two bees, corroborating the expectations of the simultaneous use of these species for the pollination of cherry tomatoes. Based on the observed results, we conclude that stingless bees M. bicolor and N. testaceicornis can be used simultaneously to pollinate cherry tomatoes grown in greenhouses. The fruit set resulting from non-buzzing N. Sociobiology 68(1): e-5227 (March, 2021) 7 testaceicornis visits is similar to the fruit set after visits from M. bicolor, a buzzing species. In future work perspectives, we suggest testing the effectivity of simultaneous use of stingless bees in other tomato varieties. Acknowledgements We thank GEF/FAO/PNUMA/MMA/FUNBIO/CNPq (Project on the Conservation and Management of Pollinators for a Sustainable Agriculture through an Ecossystem Approach) and the Federal University of Viçosa, for the facility structure, and the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES) - Financial Code 001 and Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e tecnológico (CNPq) for the financial support provided. WFS thanks to CAPES and PPGERN for the PNPD grant (id: 88882.314552/2019-01) and CNPq (303894/2018-0) and Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio de Janeiro (FAPERJ) (203.321/2017) for grants to MCG. The authors are also grateful to Geraldo Paiva, Iris Stanciole, Geraldo Meri, Camila Folly, Cristiane Marques, Paula Netto and Riudo Paiva for their help in the fieldwork. We also thank Msc. Bianca Gonçalo Meyrelles and Dr. Weyder Cristiano Santana for suggestions during the experiments. References Azevedo, R.L., Carvalho, C.A.L., Pereira, L.L. & Nascimento, A.S. (2007). Abelhas (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) visitantes das flores do feijão guandu no Recôncavo Baiano, Brasil. Ciência Rural, 35: 1453-1457. doi: 10.1590/S0103-84782007000500038 Bartelli, B.F. & Nogueira-Ferreira, F.H. (2014). Pollination Services Provided by Melipona quadrifasciata Lepeletier (Hymenoptera: Meliponini) in Greenhouses with Solanum lycopersicum L. (Solanaceae). Sociobiology, 61: 510-516. doi: 10.13102/sociobiology.v61i4.510-516 Bartelli, B.F., Santos, A.O.R. & Nogueira-Ferreira, F.H. (2014). Colony performance of Melipona quadrifasciata (Hymenoptera, Meliponina) in a Greenhouse of Lycopersicon esculentum (Solanaceae). Sociobiology, 61: 60-67. doi: 10.13 102/sociobiology.v61i1.60-67 Bell, M.C., Spooner-Hart, R.N. & Haigh, A.M. (2006). Pollination of greenhouse tomatoes by the Australian bluebanded bee Amegilla (Zonamegilla) holmesi (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Journal of Economic Entomology, 99: 437-442. doi: 10.1603/0022-0493-99.2.437 Bispo dos Santos, A.S., Roselino, A.C., Hrncir, M. & Bego, L.R. (2009). Pollination of tomatoes by the stingless bee Melipona quadrifasciata and the honey bee Apis mellifera (Hymenoptera, Apidae). Genetics and Molecular Research, 8: 751-757. doi: 10.4238/vol8-2kerr015 Bomfim, I.G.A., Bezerra, A.D.M., Nunes, A.C., Aragão, F.A.S. & Freitas, B.M. (2014). Adaptive and Foraging Behavior of Two Stingless Bee Species (Apidae: Meliponini) in Greenhouse Mini Watermelon Pollination. Sociobiology, 61: 502-509. doi: 10.13102/sociobiology.v61i4.502-509 Brand, H. (2005). Geoprópolis em colméias de Mandaçaia (Melipona quadrifasciata Lepeletier): um modelo de herança poligênica. Acta Biológica Paranaense, 4: 139-141. doi: 10.53 80/abpr.v34i0.960 Buchmann, S.L. & Cane, J.H. (1989). Bees assess pollen returns while sonicating Solanum flowers. Oecologia, 81: 289-294. doi: 10.1007/BF00377073 Buchmann, S.L. (1983). Buzz pollination in angiosperms. Handbook of Experimental Pollination Biology, New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Company, 73-113 p. Camargo, J.M.F. (1972). Manual de apicultura. Ed. Agronômica Ceres, São Paulo. 252 p. Carvalho, P.E.R. (2007). Cafezeiro do mato. Circular Técnica EMBRAPA, 138: 1-16. Cauich, O., Quezada-Euán, J.J.G., Macias-Macias, J.O., Reyes-Oregel, V., Medina-Peralta, S. & Parra-Tabla, V. (2004). Behavior and pollination efficiency of Nannotrigona perilampoides (Hymenoptera: Meliponini) on greenhouse tomatoes (Lycopersicon esculentum) in subtropical México. Journal of Economic Entomology, 97: 172-179. Cruz, D.O. & Campos, L.A.O. (2009). Polinização por abelhas em cultivos protegidos. Revista Brasileira de Agrociência, 15: 5-10. doi: 10.18539/CAST.V15I1-4.1979 Cruz, D.O., Freitas, B.M., Silva, L.A., Silva, E.M.S. & Bomfim, G.A. (2004). Adaptação e comportamento de pastejo da abelha jandaíra (Melipona subnitida Ducke) em ambiente protegido. Acta Scientiarum Animal Sciences, 26: 293-298. doi: 10.4025/actascianimsci.v26i3.1777 Da Silva, M.A., Oliveira, F.A. & Hrncir, M. (2016). Efeito de diferentes tratamentos de polinização em berinjela em casa de vegetação. Revista Verde de Agroecologia e Desenvolvimento Sustentável, 11: 30-36. doi: 10.18378/rvads.v11i1.3771 De Luca, P.A. & Vallejo-Marín, M. (2013). What’s the “buzz” about? The ecology and evolutionary significance of buzz-pollination. Current Opinion in Plant Biology, 16: 429- 435. doi: 10.1016/j.pbi.2013.05.002 Del Sarto, M.C.L., Peruquetti, R.C. & Campos, L.A.O. (2005). Evaluation of the Neotropical stingless bee Melipona quadrifasciata (Hymenoptera: Apidae) as pollinator of greenhouse tomatoes. Journal of Economic Entomology, 98: 260-266. doi: 10.1093/jee/98.2.260 Depra, M.S., Delaqua, G.C.G., Freitas, L. & Gaglianone, M.C. (2014). Pollination deficit in open-field tomato crops (Solanum lycopersicum L., Solanaceae) in Rio de Janeiro state, southeast Brazil. Journal of Pollination Ecology, 12: 1-8. doi: 10.26786/1920-7603 (2014) 7 MC Moura-Moraes, W Frantine-Silva, MC Gaglianone, LAO Campos – Stingless bee pollination in cherry tomatoes8 Dos Santos, S.A.B., Roselino, A.C. & Bego, L. (2008). Pollination of Cucumber, Cucumis sativus L. (Cucurbitales: Cucurbitaceae), by the Stingless Bees Scaptotrigona aff. depilis Moure and Nannotrigona testaceicornis Lepeletier (Hym.: Meliponini) in Greenhouses. Neotropical Entomology, 37: 506-512. doi: 10.1590/S1519-566X2008000500002 Free, J.B. (1993). Insect pollination of crops. University of Wales, Cardiff, UK. 684 p. Gaglianone, M.C., Franceschinelli, E.V., Campos, M.J.O., Freitas L., Silva Neto, C.M., Deprá, M.S., Elias, M.A.S., Bergamini, L., Netto, P., Meyrelles, B.G., Montagnana, P.C., Patricio, G.P. & Campos L.A.O. (2018). Tomato pollination in Brazil. In Roubik (ed) The Pollination of cultivated plants: a compendium for practitioners, 1: 238-247. Giurfa, M. & Lehrer, M. (2001). Honeybee vision and floral displays: from detection to close-up recognition, pp 61–82. In: Cognitive ecology of pollination (Chittka L, Thomson J Eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. doi: 10.1017/ CBO9780511542268.005 Hammer, Ø., Harper, D.A.T. & Ryan, P.D. (2001). PAST: Paleontological statistics software package for education and data analysis. Palaeontologia Electronica, 4: 1-9. Higuti, A.R.O., Godoy, A.R., Salata, A.C. & Cardoso, A.I.I. (2010). Produção de tomate em função da “vibração” das plantas. Biota Neotropica, 69: 87-92. doi: 10.1590/S0006- 87052010000100012 Hikawa, M. & Miyanaga, R. (2009). Effects of pollination by Melipona quadrifasciata (Hymenoptera: Apidae) on tomatoes in protected culture. Applied Entomology and Zoology, 44: 301- 307. doi: 10.1303 / aez.2009.301 Hogendoorn K., Gross C.L., Sedgley M. & Keller M. (2006). Increased tomato yield through pollination by native Australian Amegilla chlorocyanea (Hymenoptera: Anthophoridae). Journal of Economic Entomology, 99: 829- 833. doi: 10.1093/jee/99.3.828 Kearns, C.A. & Inouye, D.W. (1993). Techniques for pollinations biologists. Niwot, Colorado, University press of Colorado, 583 p. Kiss, J. (2006). Casa aquecida: Um sistema de colméias com calefação mantém a temperature em 28 graus e permite a criação de abelhas nordestinas no Sul do país. Revista Globo Rural, 245: 32-37. Kleinert, A.M.P.; Ramalho, M., Cortopassi-Laurino, M. & Imperatriz-Fonseca, V.L. (2009). Abelhas sociais (Bombbini, Apini, Meliponini). In. Panizzi, A. R. & Parra, J. R. P. (Eds). Bioecologia e nutrição de insetos. Brasilia: EMBRAPA Informação Tecnológica, 371-424. Lopes de Carvalho, C.A., Marchini, L.C. & Ros, P.B. (1999). Fontes de pólen utilizadas por Apis mellifera L. e algumas espécies de Trigonini (Apidae) em Piracicaba (SP). Bragantia, 58: 49-56. doi: 10.1590/S0006-87051999000100007 Mann, L.K. (1943). Fruit shape of watermelons as affected by placement of pollen on the stigma. Botanical Gazette, 105: 257-262. McGregor, S.E. (1976). Insect pollination of cultivated plants. Agriculture research Service (ARS) Publication 774. Washington: USDA. Michener, C.D. (2000). The bees of the world. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press, 913 p. Morandin, L.A., Laverty, T.M. & Kevan, P.G. (2001a). Bumble Bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) Activity and Pollination Levels in Commercial Tomato Greenhouses, 94: 462-467. doi: 10.1603/ 0022-0493-94.2.462 Morandin, L.A., Laverty, T.M. & Kevan, P.G. (2001b). Effect of bumble bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) pollination intensity on the quality of greenhouse tomatoes. Journal of Economic Entomology, 94: 172-179. doi: 10.1603/0022-0493-94.1.172 Nogueira-Neto, P. (1997). Vida e criação de abelhas indígenas sem ferrão. São Paulo: Editora Nogueirapis, 445 p. Nunes-Silva, P., Hrncir, M., Silva, C.I., Roldão, Y.S. & Imperatriz-Fonseca, V.L. (2013). Stingless bees, Melipona fasciculata, as eficiente pollinators of eggplant (Solanum melongena) in greenhouses. Apidologie, 44: 537-546. doi: 10.1007/s13592-013-0204-y Palma, G., Quezada-Euán, J.J.G., Reyes-Oregel, V., Meléndez, V. & Moo-Valle, H. (2008). Production of greenhouse tomatoes (Lycopersicon esculentum) using Nannotrigona perilampoides, Bombus impatiens and mechanical vibration (Hymenoptera: Apoidea). Journal of Applied Entomology, 132: 79-85. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0418.2007.01246.x Picken, A.J.F. (1984). A review of pollination and fruit set in the tomato (Lycopersicon esculentum Mill.). Journal of Horticultural Science, 59: 1-13. doi: 10.1080/00221589.1984.11515163 Portman, Z.M., Orr M.C. & Griswold, T. (2019). A review and updated classification of pollen gathering behavior in bees (Hymenoptera, Apoidea). Journal of Hymenoptera Research, 71: 171-208. doi: 10.3897/jhr.71.32671 Ramalho, M. (2004). Stingless bees and mass flowering trees in the canopy of Atlantic Forest: a tight relationship. Acta Botanica Brasilica, 18: 37-47. doi: 10.1590/S0102-330 62004000100005 Roselino, A.C., Santos, S.B., Hrncir, M. & Bego, L.R. (2009). Differences between the quality of strawberries (Fragaria x ananassa) pollinated by the stingless bees Scaptotrigona aff. depilis and Nannotrigona testaceicornis. Genetics and Molecular Research, 8: 539-545. doi: 10.4238/vol8-2kerr005 Roubik, D.W. (1989). Ecology and natural history of tropical bees. Cambridge Tropical Biology Series, 514 p. Shelly, T.E., Villalobos, E. & OTS-USAP. (2000). Buzzing bees (Hym.: Apidae, Halictidae) on Solanum (Solanaceae): Sociobiology 68(1): e-5227 (March, 2021) 9 floral choice and handling time track pollen availability. Florida Entomologist, 83: 180-187. doi: 10.2307/ 3496153 Silva-Neto, C.M., Ribeiro, A.C.C., Gomes, F.L., Melo, A.P.C., Oliveira, G.M., Faquinello, P., Franceschinelli, E.V. & Nascimento, A.R. (2018). The stingless bee mandaçaia (Melipona quadrifasciata Lepeletier) increases the quality of greenhouse tomatoes. Journal of Apicultural Research, 58. doi: 10.1080/ 00218839.2018.1494913 Slaa, E.J.; Chaves, L.A.S.; Malagodi-Braga, K.S. & Hofstede, F.E. (2006). Stingless Bees in Applied Pollination: Practice and Perspectives. Apidologie, 37: 293-315. doi: 10.1051/ apido:2006022 Srinivasan, M.V., Zhang, S.W. & Witney, K. (1994). Visual discrimination of pattern orientation in honeybees: Performance and implications for “cortical” processing. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 343: 199- 210. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1994.0021 Taura, H. M. & Laroca, S. (2004). Biologia da polinização: interações entre as abelhas (Hym. Apoidea) e as flores de Vassobia breviflora (Solanaceae). Acta Biologica Paranaense, 33: 143-162. doi: 10.5380/abpr.v33i0.630 Teixeira, L. & Campos, F.D.N.M. (2005). Início da atividade de vôo em abelhas sem ferrão (Hymenoptera, Apidae): influência do tamanho da abelha e da temperatura ambiente. Revista Brasileira de Zoociências, 7: 195-202. Thorp, R.W. (2000). The collection of pollen by bees. Plant Systematics and Evolution, 222: 211-223. Velthuis, H.W. & van Doorn, A. (2006). A century of advances in bumblebee domestication and the economic and environmental aspect of its commercialization for pollination. Apidologie, 37: 421-451. doi: 10.1051/apido:2006019 Westerkamp, C. & Gottsberger. G. (2000). Diversity pays in crop pollination. Crop Science, 40: 1209-1222. doi: 10.2135/ cropsci2000.4051209x Wille, A. (1963). Behavioral adaptions of bees for pollen collecting from Cassia flowers. Revista de Biologia Tropical, 11: 205-210. Zar, J.H. (1999). Biostatistical analysis. Fourth edition. USA: Prentice Hall, 208-214 p.