3 Segmental Speech Timing at the Phoneme and Syllable Levels in English and Afrikaans Speaking White South African Children Carol L Oosthuizen, Ph D (Cape Town) Department of Otorhinolaryngology University of Stellenbosch ABSTRACT Two aspects of segmental timing were acoustically measured in the speech production of30 English and 30 Afrikaans speaking white South African children. These were VOTand medial stop closure duration in VCV nonsense syllables. In addition, medial vowel duration in CVC nonsense syllables was measured in English speakers. Comparisons were made between younger (mean age 4.25 years) andolder (meanage 6.5 years) subjects in each language group. Graphical statistical methods revealed certain trends in the data. English speakers employed short- and long-lag VOTfor voiced and voice- less stops while Afrikaans speakers used two short-lag categories. Contextually determined right-to-left timing effects were identified which were in agreement with the literature, with different rates of acquisition of medial stop closure duration rules being observed between the language groups. OPSOMMING Twee aspekte van segmentele tydreeling in die spraakproduksie van 30Engels- en 30 Afrikaanssprekende blanke Suid-Afrikaansekinders is akoesties gemeet. Dit was stemaanvangstyd en tydsduur van mediale plosiewe sluiting in VKV onsinlettergrepe. Tydsduur van mediale vokale in'KVK onsinlettergrepe is ook in Engelssprekendes gemeet. Vergelykings is ook gemaak tussenjonger (gemiddelde ouderdom 4.25 jaar) en ouer (gemiddelde ouderdom 6.5 jaar) proefpersone in elke taalgroep. Dit is bevind dat Engelssprekendes gebruik maak van kort en lang vertraging in stemaanvangstyd vir stemhebbende en stemlose plosiewe, terwyl Afrikaanssprekendes twee kort vertragingskategoriee gebruik. Regs-tot-links konteksgebonde neigings wat in segmentele tydreeling geidentifiseer is, het met die literatuur ooreengestem terwyl verskillende tempo's van verwerwing van reels vir tydsduur van mediale,plosiewe tussen die taalgroepe waargeneem is. Temporal aspects of speech production have been highlighted by Kent (1976) as possibly "J., the most critical factor in skilled motor performance." (ρ 43δ[) The development of speech tim- Xiflg is thought to reflect neuromaturation of the speech mecha- nism and has important implications for many branches of the study of communication disorders. Kent points out that pho- netic judgments are only gross indicators of motor speech devejppment and that more sensitive parameters of motor skills, such as the acquisition of segmental timing rules, con- tinue to develop after children have acquired the basic phono- logical system. Some developmental patterns in segmental timing have been demonstrated (inter alia, by Kewley-Port & Preston, 1974; DiSimoni, 1974 a,b,c; Tingley & Allen, 1975;Menyuk&Klatt, 1975; Gilbert, 1976; Gilbert & Johnson, 1978; Smith, 1978; Macken & Barton, 1979; Flege, McCutcheon & Smith, 1987) but the still inadequate body of research on the subject pre- cludes a comprehensive description._ Development in the various levels of speech timing has not been fully investigated. Cooper (1977) identified three levels on which children learn control of speech timing: 1. The phonetic-phonemic level, such as voice onset time (VOT), i.e the time taken from the release of a stop plosive closure to the onset of voicing for the post-con- sonantal vowel. Die Suid-Afrikaanse Tydskrif vir Kommunikasieafivykings, Vol. 36, 1989 2. The syllable level, comprising rules for the timing of sounds within syllables. This can be illustrated by a temporal compensation rule which states that a longer- than-average vowel must be followed by a shorter-than- average consonant. 3. The sentence level. Both children and adults seem to preplan the timing of segments within utterances, e.g. the longer the utterances, the shorter the individual segments. A fourth level of speech timing may be added (Dalton & Hardcastle, 1977), viz.: 4. The prosodic level, i.e. transition smoothness between intonation units. It is not yet known whether measures of timing variables in very young children would predict later motor speech perfor- mance. The intriguing question of whether or not there is a relationship between speech timing development and speech disorders has only been addressed in a preliminary way with regard to articulation disorders (Weismer & Elbert, 1982), phonological disorders (Catts & Jensen, 1983; Smit & Bernthall, 1983), cleft palate (Forner, 1983), hearing impair- ment (Stark, 1972), delayed language development (Bord & Wilson, 1980) and stuttering (inter alia, DiSimoni, 1974c; ® SASHA 1989 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 Carol L Oosthuizen Starkweather et al. 1976; Starkweather & Myers, 1979; Oost- huizen, 1986; Adams, 1987; McKnight & Cullinan, 1987). A description of normal development should be the precursor to the study of timing problems in speech disorders. No norma- tive study of speech timing in South African children has yet been published and this report represents a limited attempt to explore a vast subject. Most white South African children speak English or Afrikaans, which have been the two official languages since 1925. Owing to historical and geographical influences, South African English is a dialect which differs from forms of English found in other countries (Lanham, 1967). Afrikaans is a Germanic language which was derived from Dutch. It cannot be assumed that the findings of studies of speech timing using speakers of other English dialects or Ger- manic languages can automatically be applied to speakers of South African English or Afrikaans. Separate investigations are merited which might eventually yield valuable insights into the relationships between the development of speech tim- ing, dialect and language. The three aspects of expressive speech timing which form the topic of this report fall within the phonetic-phonemic and syll- able levels. They are syllable-initial VOT, syllable-medial stop closure duration and syllable-medial vowel duration. These segmental timing elements were studied in the speech of Eng- lish and Afrikaans speaking white South African children and formed part of a larger study directed toward the speech of stutterers (Oosthuizen, 1986). They will be discussed as three separate experimental procedures carried out on the same sub- jects, experiment three using English speaking subjects only. EXPERIMENT ONE. Aim The aim of experiment one was to identify normal trends for VOT production of syllable-initial stop plosives in the experi- mental groups. VOT is an important dimension of segmental timing, since it i s a valid measure to differentiate phonemic voicing categories in many languages. Most studies of VOT production in children have employed very small subject samples, nevertheless some developmental trends have emerged. Kewly-Port and Preston (1974), study- ing American English children (n3), found that, initially, short-lag VOTs predominated with a gradual increase in long- lag VOTs. Those findings were confirmed by Macken and Bar- ton (1979) who noted that their 4 subjects had reached adult contrast values by 2.4 years. However, a small sample of Cana- dian English children failed to demonstrate adult / t / values by three years (Gilbert 1976). In reviewing the literature, Kent (1976) concluded that VOT attains adult stability around eight years of age. ί. Method Subjects 30 English and 30 Afrikaans speaking white South African children from monolingual homesin the Cape Peninsula were divided into younger and older age groups (see table 1). The subjects had no known medical or neurological disorders, nor any history of communication problems and had passed hear- ing screening tests. Table 1: Subject groupings w i t h mean ages in years and months SUBGROUP (N-15) MEAN AGE AGE RANGE 1. Older English speakers 6,10 yrs 6,5 to 7,6 2. Older Afrikaans speakers 6,9 yrs 6,4 to 7,10 3. Younger English speakers 4,4 yrs 4,0 to 5,0 4. Younger Afrikaans speakers 4,1 yrs 3,1 to 4,8 Procedures for data collection Four productions of each of the following CVC nonsense syl- lables were elicited from each subject and recorded on audio tape: /paf/ /baf/ /taf/ /daf/ The subjects "were tested individually in a quiet environment. The syllables were used in a story context by an adult native speaker of each language, following a procedure described by Hawkins (1973), after which deferred imitations were elicited from the children. Data Analysis Storage oscilloscopic displays of the first three clear, isolated productions of each of the syllables for each subject were obtained using an electronic memory (Biomation Model 1015) and Tektronix 510 3N oscilloscope. Judgment of speech seg- mental boundaries in the display followed the guidelines laid down by Shoup and Pfeifer (1976) and segments were measur- ed in milliseconds. The VOT of each production was classified as falling into the short-, long-lag, or voicing lead categories. In their seminal study of adult native speakers of eleven lan- guages, Lisker and Abramson (1964) identified three catego- ries of VOT values: Prevoicing (voicing lead): -125 to zero msecs ι Short-lag: zero to /25 msecs ι Long-lag: /- 60 to /100 j It is n o w generally accepted that a VOT boundary of approx[ imately /35 msecs separates the short- and long-lag categories in English (Cooper, 1977), a figure which corresponds witli the perceptual boundary. j Statistical Treatment 1 Owing to the small number of subjects (15) in each subgroup, and given the exploratory nature of the study, graphical statis- tical methods were applied. Multiple box plots were used to display the central tendency and spread of the mean, the range, maximum and minimum scores for each production of each syllable. Results and discussion ^ Figure 1 shows summary box plots illustrating the relative VOT values for the /paf/-/baf/ cognate pair in the four experi- mental groups. Comment on the distribution refers to the interquartile distance, chosen as a measure of spread, and the median, chosen as a central value. These parameters were / The South African Journal of Communication Disorders, Vol. 36, 1989 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Segmental Speech Timing at the Phoneme and Syllable Levels 5 regarded as preferable to the standard deviation or arithmetic mean, since many of the distributions were asymmetrical; they have the additional advantage of being little affected by outly- ing scores. Outlying scores are represented on the distribution by asterisks in a vertical column. Results for the /taf/-/daf/ cognate pair (not shown) were comparable and are included in the comments. ker (1974). This relative reduction of VOT for voiceless syll- able initial stop plosives reflects the local dialect, since the older English speakers' scores were comparable to adult data obtained in an unpublished pilot study. Speculation regarding the various influences on the local dialect which could affect segmental timing would be premature as ideas in this regard are currently under review (Lass, personal communication 1986). Older Older Younger Younger • 8 0 EnoRsh speakers Afrikaans speakers English speakers Alrikaans speakers I V O T i n m s e c s • 5 0 ' .40 +30 +20 +10 0 -10- -20" - 3 0 " - 4 0 - - 5 0 - 6 0 ψ Figure 1 Summary box plots showing VOT values for the /paf/-/baf/ cognate pair in the four groups of subjects. A few extreme prevoicing scores have been omitted in older Afrikaans speakers. While there was a degree of overlap between VOTs for voiced and voiceless cognate pairs, older English speakers made clearer distinctions between them than did their Afrikaans peers. South African English appears to be a two category dialect employing short- and long-lag VOT contrasts for voiced and voiceless stops respectively. Afrikaans speakers appeared to use two sets of unaspirated stops for 'voiced' and Voiceless' sounds all of which fell into the short-lag category, a finding which provides documentary evidence of a feature which is familiar to Afrikaans phoneticians (de Villiers, /1967). Older English speakers sho lag VOTs on voiceless stops ved a greater trend toward long- than did younger ones. This is in agreement with the reported developmental findings, that children have to learn to lengthen VOT. Afrikaans speakers did not show VOT lengthening for voiceless stops with age, since they did not employ the long-lag category. Analysis of multiple box plots of the range (not shown) indi- cated increased variability of VOT on voiceless stops with age in the case of English speakers. This was attributed to the inherent scope of the long-lag category, which permits a wide variety of acceptable scores. Most Afrikaans speakers showed decreased variability of production with age; which agrees with previous findings (Eguchi and Hirsch, 1969; DiSimoni, 1974a, 1974b; Tingley and Allen, 1975; Zlatin and Koenigs- knecht, 1976). However, some individuals showed increased variability for voiced stops owing to the development of voic- ing lead as a stylistic variant. In the case of the South African English children, the means for long-lag VOT were considerably smaller than those reported in the literature for American English children, yet they fell into the adult long-lag range identified by Abramson and Lis- EXPERIMENT TWO DiSimoni (1974a, 1974b), studying American English child- ren, found that the influence of phonetic context on segmental durations appeared between three and six years of age and approached, but did not yet reach adult levels, by nine years. In VCV syllables, the stop closure durations of both voiced and voiceless stops were found to be greater in / i / t h a n in / a / environments. The present experiment set out to determine whether similar trends characterized the speech of South Afri- can children. Procedures for data collection for experiments 2 and 3. T w o randomized word lists, each containing eight nonsense syllables and yielding three nonconsecutive productions of each syllable by each subject were audio recorded. In experi- ment 2 these were VCV syllables: /apa/ /aba/ /ata/ /ada/ /ipi/ /ibi/ /iti/ /idi/ The task was one of imitation of an adult speaker, recorded in the case of older subjects and live voice presentation for younger subjects, the syllables being preceded by a carrier phrase. The preconsonantal vowels were stressed in experi- ment 2. Data Analysis T w o measures were taken from each production of each syll- able, viz. whole syllable duration and the closure phase of the medial stop plosive. Results and discussion Figures 2 - 5 contain summary multiple box plots illustrating the central tendency and spread of stop closure duration scores for the experimental syllables in the four groups. Nonsense syllables idi ibi ata Slop closure 129 • duration in msecs 109 Figure 2 Summary box plots showing the central ten- dency and spread of stop closure durations i n VCV syllables in older English speakers. Die Suid-Afrikaanse Tydskrif vir Kommunikasieafivykings, Vol. 36, 1989 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 Carol L Oosthuizen Stop 159 - closure duration in msecs 124 Η 532- Nonsense syllables U bl ill Figure 3 Summary box plots showing the central ten- dency and spread of stop closure durations in the VCV syllables in older Afrikaans speakers. 223- Slop closure 157- cbratlon In maeca 124- 90.3- 5 7 2 - 24:0 • aba Id bl ata apa Kl ty 1 Τ τ Figure 4 Summary box plots showing the central ten- dency and spread of stop closure durations in VCV nonsense syllables in younger English speakers. Slop 201 closure duration in msecs 163 Η 48.0 • Nonsense syllables idi ibi ata apa Figure 5 Summary box plots showing the central ten- dency and spread of stop closure durations in VCV syllables in younger Afrikaans speakers. sistent with the results of acoustic studies (Schwartz, 1969; DiSimoni, 1974a; Suen and Beddoes, 1974; Smith, 1978) as well as a physiological study by Butcher and Weiher (1976). Schwartz (1969) attributed the shorter stop closure duration prior to / a / to coarticulation, the earlier consonant release compensating for the greater distance to be covered in order to reach the low vowel position compared with the high one. In contrast to Canadian English adults (Suen & Beddoes, 1974) the South African children showed greater mean'stop closure duration differences between cognate pairs for alveo- lar sounds than for bilabials. Perhaps this place-of-articula- tion effect results from relatively poor motor capabilities of the tongue tip in children. The younger South African English speakers had a mean closure duration difference between voiced and voiceless stops of 16,7 msecs, which is far below the values reported by Smith (1978) for American English children of comparable ages. These discrepancies probably reflect dialectal differen- ces, although methodological factors cannot be discounted. There appeared to be a developmental trend in the direction of lengthening of medial stop closure duration of voiceless stops. This voicing distinction seemed to be acquired earlier in both vowel environments in Afrikaans speakers compared with English speakers. A second age-related trend, which was also interpreted as reflecting a developmental pattern, was toward lengthening of medial stop closure duration in the high vowel environment. Variability of timing production was greater amongst younger speakers compared with older ones, confirming the reported trend in the literature. Whole syllable durations did not de- crease with age, however. Another unexpected finding was that of greater whole syllable durations in English than in Afri- kaans speakers. This cannot simply be attributed to longer VOTs in the English group. VOT lengthens with age, yet whole syllable durations were longer in younger than in older speakers. Since stop closure durations per se, which included transitions, did not differ between the language groups, it seems that the English speakers may have been using longer vowels than did their Afrikaans counterparts. The final vowel is generally thought to exert most influence on medial stop closure duration (Anderson, 1975), but there is conflicting evidence regarding the influence of the preceding vowel (Schwartz, 1969; Butcher & Weiher, 1976). Since both vowels were the same in each syllable, it is not possible to determine which was affecting medial stop closure durations. j The lack of consistent age related trends for whole syllable duration is in agreement with Kubaska and Keating (1981) who observed that, provided that the positions of words in utterances were controlled, and once children had reached the two-word utterance stage of development, word durations did not change much over time in American English children (n3). EXPERIMENT THREE No differences emerged between the older language groups with respect to stop closure duration trends. Both groups showed longer medial stop closure durations for voiceless compared with voiced stops. Stop closure durations were also longer in / i / than in /a/environments. These findings are con- This experiment was performed on English speakers only, since final voiced stops and fricatives do not normally occur in Afrikaans (Combrink & de Stadlef, 1987). Vowels which precede voiced stops have been, shown to be approximately 100 msecs longer than those preceding final The South African Journal of Communication , Disorders, Vol. 36, 1989 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Segmental Speech Timing at the Phoneme and Syllable Levels 7 voiceless stops in adult speech (House & Fairbanks, 1953; Peterson & Lehiste, 1960). Naeser (1970), Smith (1978) and Krause (1982) have demonstrated similar trends in 2-3 year old American English children. The phenomenon has also been identified in older children by DiSimoni (1974b) who indicated that development was in the direction of vowel lengthening for voiced consonants rather than shortening for voiceless consonants. This experiment was aimed at determining the trends charac- terizing the English speaking subgroups in terms of medial vowel duration relative to the consonant environment in CVC nonsense syllables. Data Collection and Analysis The word lists contained these CVC nonsense syllables: /pip / / p a p / /sis/ /sas/ /bib / /bab/ /ziz/ /zaz/ Measures were taken of vowel and whole syllable durations. Results and discussion Figures 6 and 7 show summary box plots indicating vowel durations for the eight syllables in the two subgroups. Results showed that vowels were longer in sibilant than in stop plosive contexts as well as in voiced compared with voiceless consonant environments. 231 - vowel 191 duration in msecs Nonsense syllables zaz ziz pip k a 30.0- Figure β Summary box plots showing the central ten- dency and spread of vowel durations in CVC syllables in Older English speakers. Contrary to other reports (reviewed by Kent, 1976), segment durations did not decrease with age. However, an age-related trend was identified in the increasing differential vowel dura- tion relative to both voicing and manner-of-articulation of the voiced consonant context. Decreasing variability of perfor- mance with age was found on measures of whole syllable duration. GENERAL DISCUSSION The segmental phonetic level of speech timing, which would include VOT, is considered by Dalton and Hardcastle (1977) to be dependent on aerodynamic coordination. The relatively late development of long-lag VOT in speakers of those lan- guages which use it might be attributed to its greater neuro- vowel 214- duration in msecs Nonsense syllables zaz ziz pip pap bib bab Τ Figure 7 Summary box plots showing the central ten- dency and spread of v o w e l durations in CVC syllables in younger English speakers. physiological complexity, compared with short-lag VOT (Cooper, 1977). However, Lisker (1975) suggested that VOT was influenced by an interaction between biological res- traints and phonological rules. The general decrease in mean segment duration which has been reported as a function of age (Smith, 1978; Kent & For- ner, 1980) and the development oflong-lag VOT might largely reflect increasing neuromuscular capabilities. It has been pro- posed (Suen & Beddoes, 1974) that the relatively late deve- lopment of longer stop closure durations for voiceless stops reflects the increased muscular effort required to produce them. This argument is challenged by the cross-language fin- dings in the South African children, the relatively early deve- lopment of longer closure durations in Afrikaans speakers suggesting that phonological considerations may be impor- tant. It seems that certain elements of speech timing are singled out for this kind of development in the age group studied, pro- bably on the basis of their phonological significance. This issue of the relative contribution of biomechanical and phonologically-programmed influences on segmental speech timing is further complicated by the probable lack of a simple one-to-one relationship between the underlying phonological segmental length specification and the observable phonetic duration. Allen (1973) has suggested that sources of variation would include speech tempo, biological restraints (such as j a w mass) and aerodynamic factors. CONCLUSION In the main, the segmental timing trends identified in the South African subjects agreed with those reported in the litera- ture, i.e.: 1. Increasing VOT for voiceless stops as a function of age in English speakers. 2. Longer medial stop closure durations in VCV syllables when the stop was voiceless and in high vowel environ- ments. A developmental trend toward the acquisition of differential stop closure durations were observed. 3. Longer medial vowel durations in CVC syllables in voic- ed and voiceless sibilant consonant environments and a developmental trend in this regard in English speak- Die Suid-Aftikaanse Tydskrif vir Kommunikasieafivykings, Vol. So, 1989 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 Carol L Oosthuizen South African English was identified as a two-category lan- guage employing short- and long-lag VOTs for voiced and voiceless stops respectively. Afrikaans speakers appeared to use two short-lag categories with random voicing lead on voiced stops in some subjects. The fact that there were mixed findings regarding decreasing variability of production as a function of age may be attributed to the relatively small age gap between the older and younger groups of subjects. Differences between speakers of the two languages with res- pect to rate and, to a lesser extent, degree of acquisition of anticipatory contextual timing effects highlighted the impor- tance of comparing cross-language data when interpreting research results in this area of study. The research findings on segmental timing effects in the con- nected speech of adult speakers (Chrystal & House, 1988) sug- gest that due caution should be observed when attempting to generalize from data derived from isolated words or syllables. Such restricted stimuli may bear a close relation only to utte- rancerinitial and prepausal words in connected speech, but this remains to be determined. REFERENCES Abramson, A.S. and Lisker, L. Voice-timing perception in Spanish word-initial stops ./. Phonetics, 5, 1-7, 1974. Adams, M.R. Voice onsets and segment durations of normal speakers and beginning stutterers. J. Fluency Dis., 12, 133-139, 1987. Allen, G.D. Segmental timing control in speech production./. Phone- tics, 1, 219-237, 1973. Anderson, B.W. Ballistic control of rhythmic articulatory movements in normal speech. N.Y. Acad, of Sciences, 263, 236-243, 1975. Bord, Z. and Wilson, H. Acquisition of the voicing contrast by lan- guage-delayed and normal-speaking children. J. Speech Hear. Res., 23, 152-161, 1980. Butcher, A. and Weiher, E. An electropalatographic investigation of coarticulation in VCV sequences./. Phonetics, 4, 59-74, 1976. Catts, H.W. and Jensen, P.J. Speech timing of phonologically disor- dered children: voicing contrast of initial and final stop con- sonants. J. Speech Hear. Res. 26, 501-510, 1983. CombrinkJ.G.H. and de Stadler, L.G. Afrikaanse Fonologie. Johannes- burg: MacMillan, 1987. Cooper, W.E. The development of speech timing. In S.J. Segalowitz and F.A. Gruber (Eds.) Language Development and Neurological Theory. New York : Academic, 1977. Crystal, T.H. and House, A.S. Segmental durations in connected- speech signals: current results. /. Acoust. Soc. Am., 83, 1553- 1573, 1988. Dalton, P. and Hardcastle, W.J. Disorders of Fluency. London: Ar- nold, 1977. De Villiers, M. Afrikaanse Klankleer. Cape Town : Balkema, 1967. DiSimoni, F.G. Influence of vowel environment on the duration of consonants in the speech of three-, six-, and nine-year old children./. Acoust. Soc. Am., 55, 360-361, 1974a. DiSimoni, F.G. Influence of consonant environment on duration of vowels in the speech of three-, six-, and nine-year old children./ Acoust. Soc. Am, 55, 362-363, 1974b. DiSimoni, F.G. Preliminary study of certain timing relations in the speech of stutterers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 56, 695-696, 1974c. DiSimoni, F.G. Some preliminary observations on temporal compen- sation in the speech of children./. Acoust. Soc. Am., 56, 697- 699, 1974d. Eguchi, S. and Hirsh, I.J. Development of speech sounds in children. ·' Acta Oto-laryngologica, Suppl. 257, 1969. Flege J.E., McCutcheon, M.J. and Smith, S.C. The development of skill in producing word-final English stops./. Acoust. Soc. Am., 82, 433-447, 1987. Forner, L.L. Speech segment durations produced by five and six year old speakers with and without cleft palate. Cleft Palate J., 20, 185-198, 1983. Gilbert, J.H.V. A voice onset time analysis of apical stop production in three-year-olds./. Child Lang., 4, 103-110, 1976. Gilbert, J.H.V. and Johnson, C.E. Temporal and sequential con- straints on six-year-old's phonological productions: some ob- servations on the 'ambliance' phenomenon./. Child Lang., 5, 101-112, 1978. Hawkins, S. Temporal coordination of consonants in the speech of children : preliminary data./. Phonetics, 1, 181-217, 1973. House, A.S. and Fairbanks, G. The influence of consonant environ- ment upon the secondary acoustical characteristics of vowels./. Acoust. Soc. Am., 25, 105-113, 1953. Kent, R.D. Anatomical and neuromuscular maturation of the speech mechanism: evidence from acoustic studies/. Speech Hear. Res., 19, 421-447, 1976. Kent, R.D. and Forner, L.L. Speech segment durations in sentence recitations by children and adults./. Phonetics, 8, 157-168, 1980. Kewly-Port, D. and Preston, M.S. Early apical stop production: a voice onset time analysis. /. Phonetics, 2, 195 -210, 1974. Krause, S.E. Developmental use of vowel duration as a cue to post- vocalic consonant voicing./. Speech Hear. Res. 25, 388-398, 1982. Kubaska, C. A. and Keating, P.A. Word duration in early child speech./. Speech Hear. Res., 24, 615-621, 1981. Lanham, L.W. The Pronunciation of South African English. Cape Town: Balkema, 1967. Lass, R. Personal communication - Department of Linguistics, Univer- sity of Cape Town, December 1986. Lisker, L. Is it VOT or a first-formant transition detector?/. Acoust. Soc. Am., 57, 1547-1551, 1975. Lisker, L. and Abrahamson, A.S. A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops : acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384-422, 1964. Mack, M. Voicing-dependent vowel duration in English and French : monolingual and bilingual production/. Acoust. Soc. Am., 71, 173-178, 1982. Macken, M.A. and Barton, D. The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English : A study of voice onset time in word-initial stop con- sonants./. Child Lang., 7, 41-74, 1979. McKnight, R. and Cullinan, W. Subgroups of stuttering children: speech and voice reaction times, segmental durations, and naming latencies./. Fluency Dis., 12, 217-233, 1987. Menyuk, P. and Klatt, M. Voice onset time in consonant cluster pro- duction by children and adults. /. Child Lang., 2, 223-231, 1975. Naeser, M.A. The influence of initial and final consonants on vowel duration in CVC syllables. Technical report No. 130, Wiscon- sin Research and Development Center for Cognitive Learning, University of Wisconsin, 1970. Oosthuizen, C.L. A Cross-linguistic study of certain temporal features of speech in stuttering and nonstuttering children. Unpublish- ed Doctoral thesis, University of Cape Town, 1986. Peterson, G.E. and Lehiste, I. Duration of syllable nuclei in English./. Acoust. Soc. Am., 32, 693-703, 1960. Schwartz, M. Influence of vowel environment on the duration of /s/ and If I. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 46, 480-483, 1969. i Shoup, J.E. and Pfeifer, L.L. Acoustic characteristics of speech sounds. In N.J. Lass (Ed.) Contemporary Issues in Experimental Phonetics. New York : Academic, 1976. Smit, A.B. and Bernthall, J.E. Voicing contrasts and their phonological implications in the speech of articulation-disordered children1. /. Speech Hear. Res, 26, 486-500, 1983. I Smith, B.L. Temporal aspects of English speech production: a develop- mental perspective./. Phonetics, 6, 37-68, 1978. | Stark, R. Some features of the vocalizations of young deaf children. In| J.F. Bosma (Ed.) Third Symposium on Oral Sensation and Per- ception: The mouth of the Infant Springfield, 111.: Thomas, 1972. Starkweather, C.W. and Myers, M. Duration of subsegments within the intervocalic interval in stutterers and nonstutterers. / . Fluency Dis., 4, 205-214, 1979. Starkweather, C. W., Hirschman, P. and Tannenbaum, R.S. Latency of vocalization onset: stutterers versus nonstutterers. /. Speech Hear. Res., 19, 481-492, 1976. Suen, C.Y. and Beddoes, M.P. The silent interval of stop consonants. Lang. Speech, 17, 126-134, 1974. Tingley, B.M. and Allen, G.D. Development of speech timing'control in children. Child Dev., 46, 186-194, 1975. ' Weismer, G. and Elbert, M. Temporal characteristics of the "functio- nally" misarticulated /s/in 4 - 6-year-old children./. Speech Hear. Res., 25, 275-287, 1982. Zimmermann, G.N. and Sapon, S.M. Note on vowel duration seen cross-linguistically. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 30, 152-153, 1958. Zlatin, M.A. and Koenigsknecht, R.A. Development of the voicing con- trast : a comparison of voice onset time in stop perception and production./. Speech Hear. Res., 19, 93-111, 1976. The South African Journal of Communication Disorders, Vol. 36, 1989 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2)