MASS GENERALISED LEARNING APPLIED TO THE PROBLEM OF CARRY-OVER IN ARTICULATION THERAPY AVRIL KLAFF, B.A. (Sp. & H. Therapy) (WITWATERSRAND) Department Speech Pathology and Audiology, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg. SUMMARY This study was designed to investigate Morehead and Johnson's hypothesis that mass generalised learning experience in therapy will eliminate carryover problems. Four sub- jects who consistently misarticulated the /S/ phoneme participated in an intensive six week therapy programme. Two experimental subjects were trained in the production of the entire category of sounds of which the target phoneme is a member. Two matched control subjects received traditional articulation therapy. Pre- and post-therapy measures included an articulation test, competence tests and spectrographic analysis. Sound pro- duction tasks and carry-over tasks were administered at regular intervals during therapy to assess generalisation to untrained contexts. Complete carry-over was evidenced by one experimental subject at the end of the sentence phase of therapy. The remaining three subjects demonstrated inconsistent carry-over at the conclusion of the therapy programme. It is suggested that a mass generalised learning procedure may offer an efficient approach to articulation therapy. OPSOMMING Hierdie studie ondersoek Morehead en Johnson se hipotese dat 'n veralgemeende leer- proses in terapie oordrag vergemaklik. Vier proefpersone wat die /S/-foneem konsekwent foutief geartikuleer het, is in 'n intensiewe terapieprogram van ses weke betrek. Twee eksperimentele proefpersone is opgelei om die volledige groep foneme in die kategorie van klanke waarvan die foutiewe foneem 'n deel is, te produseer. Twee proefpersone het as kontrole tradisionele artikulasieterapie ontvang. Artikulasie- en bevoegdheidstoetse sowel as spektrografiese analises is deur die proefpersone voor en na terapie ondergaan. Klankproduksie- en oordragtake is met gereelde tussenposes gedurende terapie onder- neem om oordrag in 'n onopgeleide verband te beoordeeL Volledige oordrag is deur een eksperimentele proefpersoon aan die einde van die sinsfase van terapie behaal. Die ander drie proefpersone het wisselvallige oordrag aan die einde van die terapieprogram getoon. Daar word voorgestel dat 'n veralgemeende leerproses moontlik 'n doeltreffende benade- ring tot artikulasie terapie bied. Articulatory disorders have long been recognised as the most prevalent of all speech disorders.2'1 2'3 0 Thus the need to devise efficient and effective proce- dures for the remediation of these problems is strongly indicated. The ultimate goal of an articulation therapy programme is to bring the individual to a point where he can communicate normally regardless of the speech context and the environment in which he finds himself.1 >2'!'35 However, the problems involved in accomplishing this goal require no introduction to the speech therapist. The terms generalisation, transfer of training and carry-over have been used in- terchangeably in the literature to refer to this aspect of therapy. The approach- Journal of the South African Speech and Hearing Association. Vol. 23. December 1976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Mass Generalized Learning and Articulation Therapy 31 es that have traditionally been advocated for facilitating this phase of.therapy suggest that most speech therapists have viewed carry-over as the extension,of correct articulation from the clinical setting to other situational settings. Inher- ent in these procedures is the assumption that carry-over is an event,which occurs only in the final stages of therapy. Their ineffectiveness is reflected in a statement made by Mowrer.21 'Carry-over remains one of the most perplexing problems speech clinicians have to face in attempting to modify articulation.' Since the problems of carry-over continue to plague speech therapists it is fit- ting that research be directed towards scrutinising the efficacy of traditional therapy procedures and to designing alternative procedures with the sole pur- pose of eliminating carry-over difficulties.4 It is to this aim that the present study was directed. Recently Morehead and Johnson20 made observations that have shed a new light on the carry-over controversy. They attribute the unstable changes that ; occur in therapy to the learning theory framework in which most speech ther- apy programmes take place. Raymore25 notes that most speech therapists use the basic "stimulus method" of therapy t a teach correct production. This technique has its origin in the early work of Travis (1931) but the procedure formulated by Van Riper in 1939 is frequently referred to as the "traditional approach" to articulation therapy. In most cases the therapist presents a model of the correct sound and the child is asked to repeat it. Reinforcement is pro- vided for each successful attempt on approximation to success. Morehead and Johnson20 note that this pairing of a stimulus and response constitutes a speci- fic training experience, the consequences of which are unstable changes in the newly learned behaviour. . In contrast Piagetian-based programmes.'.. assume that mass generalised experience is crucial to learning and these should provide more stable changes in behaviour. Many enthusiasts in different fields have adapted Piaget's viewpoint to suit their specific needs. Since no further references to this method of teaching could be found which is specifically applied to speech therapy, the writer in- terpreted this principle in a way that was thought best suited to articulation learning. Impetus for change in the nature of articulation therapy has also resulted from the application of linguistic theory to normal and defective articulation. Re- cent research has resulted in the proposal that articulation problems be con- sidered in the framework of the phonological s y s t e m . 3 ' 4 ' 1 4 ' 1 9 ' 2 2 ' 2 3 ' 3 1 As a number of phonemes share features in common it has been thought that it is the omission or misuse of a feature that accounts for the misarticulation errors of the child. The implication is that correction of articulation must be directed at locating and correcting the feature which is common to a number of misarticulations.18'19'23 Thus traditional therapy procedures of teaching sounds in isolation are being substituted for by procedures in which an entire category of sounds is worked o n . 1 3 ' 3 1 It is proposed that these recent developments in the remediation of multiple articulation defects can be utilised to translate the principle of mass generalised Tydskrif van die Suid-Afrikaanse Vereniging vir Sprak- en Gehoorheelkunde Vol. 23 Desember 1976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 Avril Klaff learning experience into a practical therapy procedure for dealing with a group of children having difficulty with only one phoneme. It was hypothesized that emphasis on the entire category of sounds of which the target phoneme is a member will provide the child with more generalised learning experience than the teaching of a single, isolated sound. This in turn will result in more stable changes in articulatory ability, hence eliminating carry-over difficulties. METHOD AIM It was the aim of the present study to employ the principle of mass generalised learning experience in articulation therapy and to observe its effects on the entire therapeutic process. SUBJECTS: Four subjects (Ss) were divided into two groups of two Ss each. The experi- mental Ss were trained according to the principle of mass generalised learning. The control Ss received traditional articulation therapy. All Ss were selected according to the following criteria: 1. Chronological age of 8 years or more in order to eliminate developmental articulation errors. 2. Presence of an interdental lisp. 3. Hearing within normal limits. Since great difficulty was experienced in obtaining Ss who conformed to these criteria, those available had to be matched as closely as possible according to the following variables: Subject C.A. SEX Articulation Error Etiological Factors Previous Therapy El 7,6 yrs. F θ Is biz l/r None Observable None CI ' 9,6 yrs. Μ 0/s b/z m Φ None Observable None E2 8,0 yrs. Μ 0/s &/z Tongue thrust Emotional problems. Learning problems. None / C2 9,6 yrs. F 0/s &/z Tongue thrust Emotional problems. Learning problems. A few ses- sions at 4 yrs. TABLE 1. Criteria according to which subjects were matched. Journal of the South African Speech and Hearing Association, Vol. 23. December 976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Mass Generalized Learning and Articulation Therapy 33 Age - since the ages of Ss ranged from 7,6 years to 9,6 years, the experimenter (E) felt that group therapy would be enhanced by placing children of the same age in the same group. Therefore, experimental and control Ss were not matched according to this variable. Sex - one male and one female was placed in each group although this variable is not considered crucial.34 Articulation error - El (Experimental S 1) and CI (Control S 1) had additional errors. (See Table 1) Etiological factors - E2 (Experimental S 2) and C2 (Control S2) were matched for the presence of a tongue thrust swallow pattern and suspected emotional difficulties both of which may be related to articulation disorders.16'34 Intelligence - Ss were not matched according to l.Q. since this is not felt to be causally related to articulation disorders.34 However Ss were matched ac- cording to an informal assessment of learning ability. PROCEDURE TESTS FOR THE EFFECTS OF THERAPY: , Figure 1 shows the sequence of therapy with the relative position of the test- ing sequence indicated. The tests were administrated on a before and after ther- apy basis as well as at specific intervals during therapy. PRE-THERAPY TEST: > A. Goldman Ftistoe B. Competence Tests C. Baseline probes * INTRA-THERAPY PROBES Final position training POST-THERAPY TEST: Goldman Fristoe B. Spectrographic analysis C. 24 hi. retention probe D. 3 week retention probe Figure I. The Sequence of Therapy with the Relative Position of the Testing Sequence Indicated. Tydskrif van die Suid-Afrikaanse Vereniging vir Spraak- en Gehoorheelkunde Vol. 23 Desember 1976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 Avril Klaff PRE-THERAPY TESTS These were administrated to determine the level of performance for each S prior to the commencement of therapy. These included the Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation8 arid two competence tests devised by the Ε to determine if Ss were able to make the phonological and phonetic distinctions between the target and error sound. Thus Ss were required to discriminate between minimal pairs containing [ + strident] sounds and [ - strident] sounds as well as minimal pairs containing slit and grooved phonemes. In addition the acoustic spectrograph was used to analyse the Ss renditions of the /s/ phoneme in an attempt to characterise more accurately the acoustic properties of the pre- therapy and post-therapy articulations. INTRA-THERAPY PROBE TESTS L The probe tests used in the present study were based on those used by Elbert, She It on and Arndt,5 Shelton, Elbert and Arndt2 7 and Wright, Shelton and Arndt.35 The tests can be divided into two categories. A. Sound Production Tasks (SPTs) A 40 item SPT was divided into 20 items for the /s/ phoneme and 20 items for the /z/ phoneme. Both phonemes were sampled in initial, medial final positions and in clusters. B. Carry-Over Tasks Generalisation of correct production to spontaneous speech was measured in four ways: 1. Subjects were asked to read aloud until 20 instances of the /s/ and /z/ phonemes had been sampled. 2. Spontaneous speech was tape recorded in the therapy setting. 3. Spontaneous speech was recorded in an extra-therapy setting, namely the grounds of the University. 4. Parents were given some understanding of how to observe the child's speech in different situations and were asked to report carry-over progress to the E. All the intra-therapy probes were administered prior to therapy in order to establish baseline performance levels for each S. The sequence of ad- ministration of these probes during therapy was not uniform for all Ss since they all progressed at difference rates. Probe tests were adminis- tered when a S obtained a score of 18/20 at the end of each phase of therapy.,If scores indicated that generalisation had taken place to the next level of production then that phase of therapy was omitted. POST-THERAPY TESTS 1. The Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation8 was re-administered 24 hours after the termination of therapy. 2. Both SPTs and carry-over tasks were re-administered 24 hours and again 3 weeks following the termination of therapy. 3. Spectrographic analyses were made of each Ss pre-therapy and post- therapy renditions of 7 /s/ and /z/ words from the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation.8 Journal of the South African Speech and Hearing Association, Vol. 23 December 976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Mass Generalized Learning and Articulation Therapy 35 RELIABILITY Random samples of recordings of probe tests were played to two experienced speech therapists. The Ε rated the probes at the time of recording and on play- back. THERAPY PROCEDURES Each group was seen for approximately 30 minutes, five days a week. Therapy was terminated when complete carry-over had been achieved or when the maxi- mum number of 20 sessions had been reached. Discrimination Training - The initial stages of the experimental therapy pro- cedure were aimed at establishing the distinction between the two groups of contrasting sounds. Thus all the [ + strident] or grooved sounds (/s/, /z/, /s/, /z/, /ts / and /dz/ according to Halle10 were said to belong to "the man with the tall hat". The [ - strident] or slit sounds (/f/, /v/, /0/ and (/£/) according to Halle10 were identified with the "man with the short hat". Discrimination ex- ercises were carried out in which the children were required to associate mini- mal pair words with the relevant "men". In contrast, control therapy was con- cerned with identifying the error and the target sound (/0/"and /s/) and training the children to discriminate between these two sounds.30 The Acquisition Phase - The experimental Ss were taught production of all the [ + strident] sounds. Production of /s/ and /z/ was taught by emphasizing the auditory and visual similarities between these sounds and other f + strident] sounds. The Ss were asked to consciously contrast [ + strident] and [ - strident] sounds in their own speech so that the differences could be emphasized. The correct production of all [ + strident] sounds was stressed at all levels of the acquisition phase. This differs markedly from the traditional procedures in which the child's attention is focused solely on correct production of the phoneme /s/. The Carry-Over Phase - This phase was provided for Ss who failed to demon- strate carry-over following the acquisition phase of therapy. Figure 1 indicates that this phase was divided into two stages. Experimental and control proce- dures differed in that experimental Ss were encouraged to use all [ + strident] sounds while the attention of the control Ss was focused only on the consis- tent use of /s/. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The small number of Ss used in the study did not justify a statistical analysis of the results and data are therefore presented in terms of individual perfor- mances. COMPARISON OF PRE-THERAPY AND POST-THERAPY TEST RESULTS A. Competence Tests - Results indicated that all Ss were able to discrimi- nate on both a phonetic and phonological level, therefore all had com- petence for correct production. B. The Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation — All Ss were found to con- sistently misarticulate the /s/ and /z/ phonemes prior to therapy. All were consistent in correct production of these two phonemes on the Tydskrif van die Suid-Afrikaanse Vereniging vir Spraak- en Gehoorheelkunde, Vol. 23, Desember 1976. R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 Avril Klaff post-therapy test seemingly indicative of the fact that all Ss had achieved the same level of performance. However, results of the carry-over tasks administered at the conclusion of therapy showed that only one S (El) had attained consistent use of /s/ and /z/ in all spontaneous speech situa- tions while the remaining three Ss demonstrated inconsistent carry-over. C. Spectrograph^ Analysis - The spectrograms were analysed with the aid of a phonetician. A systematic variation between pre-therapy and post- therapy renditions was observed. 1. The lowest frequency at which peaks of energy occur shifted from 2000Hz to 3500Hz. 2. Weighting of energy shifted from the mid-frequencies to the higher frequencies. 3. Peaks of energy extended to 16 000Hz. The accoustic patterns of the pre-therapy articulations do not conform to those reported to be characteristic of any other English fricatives. Thus the misarticulation does not appear to be one of substitution of/#/ for /s/ but could be more adequately described as an "articulatory displacement".9 However, the post-therapy patterns correspond closely to the patterns appa- rent in the E's spectrogram confirming that the Ss had achieved acceptable production as a result of therapy. INTRA-THERAPY PROBE TEST R E S U L T S The results presented in this section document the changes that occurred in articulation generalisation as each S progressed through the programme. A. Sound Production Tasks Table II shows the total number of correct responses on the word and sentence task for each S. The following trends can be extracted from Table II. 1. Discrimination training minimally improved production. A number of researchers have questioned the excessive reliance of therapy programmes on discrimination training.6'1 5;1 7' ' ' 2. The scores on the /s/SPT exceeded scores obtained on the /z/SPT for all Ss during word training. Thus the benefits of training the experimental Ss in correct production of /s/ and /z/ simultaneously was not evident. However, while all four Ss obtained maximum scores on the /s/SPT, only the experimental Ss scored maximally on the /z/SPT. (See Table II). The Ε postulates that the effects of mass generalised learning experience may have begun to manifest itself in the ease with which the experimental Ss generalised cor- rect production from words to sentences. It was observed that the control Ss produced the stimulus word correctly but had difficulty generalising correct production to other target phonemes in the sentence. The control Ss were oriented towards a one-to-one rela- tionship between response and reinforcement whereas the experi- mental Ss were rewarded for a diversity of responses. The latter Journal of the South African Speech and Hearing Association, Vol. 23 December 976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Mass Generalized Learning and Articulation Therapy 37 appears to reduce the child's focus on specific stimuli. Rather, the child is made aware of the generality of correct production. SUBJECT PROBE PHASE OF THERAPY BASE- LINE DIS- CRIM. I F Μ SENT. SP.SP. PI P2 EI Is/ SPT 0 1 17 18 / 20 / 20 20 EI /z/SPT 0 0 11 14 / 20 / 20 20 CI IsI SPT 0 0 18 / 20 20 20 CI /z/SPT 0 0 19 / 15 - 20 20' E2 Is/ SPT 0 0 6 16 / 20 - 20 19 E2 /z/ SPT 0 0 0 13 / 20 - 20 20 C2 Isl SPT 0 3 18 / 20 - 20 20 C2 /z/SPT 0 0 10 / 16 - 20 18 Discrim. = discrimination training 1 / 1 = No therapy indicated Sp.Sp. = Spontaneous Speech — = Probe not administered PI = 24 hour retention test | = Termination of therapy P2 = 3 week retention test TABLE II. Total Number of correct articulations for all subjects on sound production tasks. B. Carry-Over Tasks In order to afford a comparative analysis the graphs of matched experi- mental and control Ss will be compared. Comparison between El and CI (See Figure 2) El and CI obtained similar results on the reading tasks and Intra-therapy Talk- ing Task. However, scores on the Extra-therapy Talking Tasks differed for these two Ss. El showed spontaneous generalisation to extra-therapy situations fol- lowing the sentence phase of therapy. CI failed to demonstrate generalisation to these contexts until correct production had been reinforced in extra-therapy settings. Parental reports confirmed that El had attained complete carry-over to all speaking situations and consequently she was dismissed at the end of the sentence phase of therapy. CI retained 100% scores on the retention tests. After having been subjected to carry-over therapy CI remained inconsistent jn his generalisation of correct production to the Extratherapy Talking Task. Further, the results of the three week retention test suggested that CI had re- gressed rather than improved in his carry-over ability. Tydskrif van die Suid-Afrikaanse Vereniging vir Spraak- en Gehoorheelkunde, Vol. 23, Desember 1976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 Avril Klaff 18' 17- in « UJ 0) 15-Ζ Ο Μ- tη SUBJECT E1 - / s / R T R T -ITT -ETT = BASELINE = DISCRIMINATION = I N I T I A L POSITION = F I N A L POSITION = M E D I A L POSITION = S E N T E N C E S = C A R R Y O V E R IN T H E CLINIC COC = C A R R Y OVER O U T S I D E T H E CLINIC PI = 24 HOUR R E T E N T I O N T E S T P2 = 3 WEEK R E T E N T I O N T E S T R T = R E A D I N G T A S K ITT = I N T R A T H E R A P Y T A L K I N G TASK ETT = E X T R A T H E R A P Y T A L K I N G TASK 10 11 ,12 13, 14 15 16 17 1Θ 19 20 ~~' S CIC P1 I F Μ THERAPY PHASE F & M S CIC T H E R A P Y PHASE Figure 2: Performance of El and CI on Reading Tasks and Talking Tasks at each Phase of Therapy. Comparison between E2 and C2 (See Figure 3) E2 did not attain the 50% criteria at the end of the initial position phase of therapy for the administration of carry-over tasks. C2 attained this criteria but failed to demonstrate any carry-over at all. E2's carry-over to spontaneous speech was investigated at the end of initial position training and the data in- dicates a greater degree of generalisation than C2 had shown to the initial carry-over tasks. Journal of the South African Speech and Hearing Association Vol. 23 December 976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Mass Generalized Learning and Articulation Therapy 39 E2's generalisation to the reading tasks and the Intra-therapy Talking Task was similar to that of C2. However, E2 showed some spontaneous generalisa- tion to the Extra-therapy Talking Task at the end of the sentence phase of therapy while C2, like CI, failed to show generalisation to these contexts un- til correct production had been reinforced in spontaneous speech. E2's scores on the Extra-therapy Talking Task exceeded those of C2 at all levels of therapy. Neither of these Ss attained complete carry-over before termination and both showed regression following the three week break. F μ s THERAPY PHASE COC PI P2 = BASELINE = DISCRIMINATION = I N I T I A L POSITION = F I N A L POSITION = M E D I A L POSITION = S E N T E N C E S = C A R R Y O V E R IN T H E CLINIC COC = C A R R Y O V E R OUTSIDE THE CLINIC = 24 HOUR R E T E N T I O N TEST = 3 WEEK R E T E N T I O N TEST = R E A D I N G TASK = I N T R A T H E R A P Y TALKING TASK E T T = E X T R A T H E R A P Y TALKING TASK i t 2 3 4. .5 6 7. θ .9 10 II 12 .13 14 15. .16 17 18 19. 20 Β D I FSM S CIC COC P1 P2 THERAPY PHASE Figure 3: Performance ofE2 and C2 on Reading Tasks and Talking Tasks at each Phase of Therapy. Tydskrif van die Suid-Afrikaanse Vereniging vir Spraak- en Gehoorheelkunde Vol. 23, Desember 1976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 Avril Klaff A general trend emerges from the close comparison of the intra-therapy probe test results of matched experimental and control Ss. In the initial acquisition stages of therapy the scores of the control Ss exceeded that of their matched experimental counterparts. However, this situation reversed itself starting at the sentence phase of therapy. Of significance are the greater scores obtained by the experimental Ss on the Extra-therapy Talking Task. Although a limita- tion of the present study is a lack of accurate assessment of carry-over to non- clinic environments, these scores appeared to demonstrate that both experi- mental Ss were readily able to generalise correct production to situations un- related to speech therapy. Reliability of Data (See Table III) PHASE OF RATING R1 AND R2 Rl, R2 AND E2 El AND E2 DISCRIMINATION 59,4% 46,9% 91,2% INITIAL POS. 90,6% 69,4% 80,0% 24 HR. RETENTION 90,0% 87,8% 96,0% R1 AND R2 - rating between 2 independent judges Rl, R2 AND E2 - Agreement between raters and the experimenter El AND E2 — Agreement between 2 sets of measurements made by the experimenter TABLE III. Mean Percentage of Inter-judge and Intra-judge Reliability. The poor reliability scores found in this study seem to-indicate that listener judgement alone is inadequate due to the acoustic similarity between /0/ and /s/. The use of visual cues as in videotaping and a more objective measure such as spectrographic analysis could eradicate some of the uncertainty. GENERAL DISCUSSION A number of factors have been said to be important in determining "an indivi- dual's ability to modify his behaviour.29 The unique characteristics of each Ss progress suggests that these factors must be taken into account when examin- ing the efficacy of a therapeutic technique. A limitation of this study was the number of inter-subject variables that had to be controlled for. The results of this study suggest that the presence of a tongue thrust did not impede E2 and C2'slability to acquire correct production These results support Mason and Proffit's16 contention that the speech defect can be corrected without prior correction of the tongue thrust. Tongue thrust does not appear to be related to carry-over problems since.CI experienced the Journal of the South African Speech and Hearing Association, Vol. 23. December 976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Mass Generalized Learning and Articulation Therapy 41 same difficulty. However, the influence of motivational factors, differences in chronological age and variations in learning ability may have contributed to the differing rates of progress observed. The effect of certain therapeutic variables was controlled for since all Ss were seen for intensive group therapy. Since the Ε was the therapist for both groups, the role of therapist variables and experimenter bias cannot be excluded. Despite these confounding variables, it is important to ask what factors inher- ent in the principle of mass generalised learning may have accounted for the finding that El showed spontaneous carry-over of correct production. The following tentative explanation is offered on the basis of certain concepts taken from Piagetian and phonological literature. The writer postulates that 'grouping' based on distinctive feature similarity may serve to provide the child with a permanent system of articulatory moni- toring. The grouping together of sounds closely related to one another provides the child with the opportunity of discerning the relationship between sounds already in his repertoire namely /s/, /z/, /ts/ and /dz/ and the target phonemes Is/ and /z/. Studies have shown that the child improves his articulation of pho- nemes that share distinctive features with the phoneme being taught. 1 8 ' 3 3 Similarly, it is possible that the child will generalise the properties of the sounds he is already able to produce to those sounds that are in error. Distinctive feature classes are reported to have corresponding clusters of sen- sory and motor feedback networks.28 Thus, when the child attempts produc- tion of /s/ and /z/ correspondence to the feedback patterns associated with /s/, /z/, /ts/ and /dz/ would be a signal to the child that his attempts were successful. It is hypothesized that if this internal referent system is established for the child in the early stages of therapy, carry-over of correct production to speech contexts unrelated to the speech therapy situation will be facilitated. The efficiency of this procedure may reside in its ability to provide the child with an independent system of articulatory monitoring. Herein lies the major downfall of traditional procedures. The result of their focus on the stimulus- response reinforcement paradigm is that the child is dependent on the thera- pist to monitor and reinforce his attempts at correct production. Mass gene- ralised learning experience, however, seems to provide the child with a strategy for coping with correct production regardless of the presence or ab- sence of stimuli related to correct production in the clinic. CONCLUSION Despite the limitations and difficulties inherent in this type of study, the re- sults do provide some support for Morehead and Johnson's20 hypothesis and also seem to suggest that the application of this principle in the field of articu- lation therapy may offer the speech therapist the advantage of naturally and effectively attaining successful carry-over. Tydskrif van die Suid-Afrikaanse Vereniging vir Spraak- en Gehoorheelkunde Vol. 23 Desember 1976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 Avril Klaff REFERENCES 1. Backus, 0 . and Dunn, H.M. (1947): Intensive Group Therapy in Speech Rehabilitation./. Speech Dis., 12, 2, 135-142. 2. Carrell, J.A. (1968): Disorders of Articulation. Prentice-Hall Inc., Englewood Cliffs, N.J. 3. Compton, A.J. (1970): Generative Studies of Children's Phonological Disorders. 7. Speech Hear. Dis., 35, 4, 315-399. 4. Crocker, J.R. (1969): A Phonological Model of Children's Articulation Competence. / Speech Hear. Dis., 34, 3, 203-213. 5., Elbert, M. Shelton R.L. and Arndt, W.B. (1967): A Task For the Evalua- tion of Articulation Change: I. The Development of Methodology./. Speech Hear. Res. 10,2,281-288. 6. ' Fry, D.B. (1968): The Phonemic System of Children's Speech. British J. Disorders Communication. 3, 1, 13-19. 7. Goda, S. (1970): Articulation Therapy and Consonant Drill Book: Grune and Stratton, N.Y. 8. Goldman, R. and Fristoe, M. (1969): Goldman-Fristoe Test of Aricula- tion. American Guidance Service Inc., Minnesota. 9. Guile, T.C. (1975): Personal Communication.Oept. of Phonetics and Linguistics, Univ. of Witwatersrand. 10. Halle, M. (1964): On the Bases of Phonology. In Fodor, J.A. and Katz, J.J. (Eds.) The Structure of Language: Readings in the Philosophy of Language. Prentice-Hall Inc., Englewood Cliffs, N.Y. 11. Hall-Powers, M. (1971): Functional Disorders of Articulation: Sympto- matology and Etiology. Chapter 33 in Handbook of Speech Pathology and Audiology. Travis, L.E. (Ed.) Appleton-Century Crofts, N.Y. 12. Hall-Powers, M. (1971): Clinical and Educational Procedures in Func- tional Disorders of Articulation. Chapter 34 in Handbook of Speech Pathology and Audiology. Travis, L.E. (Ed.) Appleton-Century Crofts, N.Y. 13. Hannah, E.P. Standel, J. and Gardner, J. (1974): Distinctive Feature Analysis. Chapter 7 in Applied Linguistic Analysis, Hannah, E.P. (Ed.) Joyce Publications, California. 14. Leonard, L.B. (1973): The Nature of Deviant Articulation./. Speech Hear. Dis., 38,2, 156-161. 15. Lewis, F.C. (1974): Distinctive Feature Confusions in Production and Discrimination of Selected Consonants. Language and Speech, 17,1, 60-67. 16. Mason, R.N. and Proffit, W.R.: The Tongue Thrust Controversy: Back- ground and Recommendations. /. Speech Hear. Dis., 39, 2,' 115-132. 17. McClean, J.E. (1970): Extending Stimulus Control of Phoneme Articu- lation by Operant Techniques. A.S.H.A. Monographs, 14, 4, 2447. 18. McReynolds, L.V. and Bennet, S. (1972): Distinctive Feature Generali- sation and Articulation Training. /. Speech Hear. Dis., 37, 4, 462470. 19. McReynolds, L.V.' and Huston, K. (1971): A Distinctive Feature Analysis of Children's Misarticulations./. Speech Hear. Dis., 36, 155-166. 20. Morehead, D.M. and Johnson, M. (1972): Piaget's Theory of Intelligence Applied to the Assessment and Treatment of Linguistically Deviant Journal of the South African Speech and Hearing Association, Vol. 23. December / 976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Mass Generalized Learning and Articulation Therapy 43 Children. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 4, 143-161. 21. Mowrer, D.E. (1971): The Transfer of Training in Articulation Therapy. /. Speech Hear. Dis., 36, 4, 427446. 22. Oiler, D.K. (1970): Regularities in Abnormal Child Phonology. J. Speech Hear. Dis., 38, 3647. 23. Pollack, E. and Rees, N.S. (1972): Disorders of Articulation: Some Clinical Application of a Distinctive Feature Approach./. Speech Hear. Dis., 37,4,451461. 24. Prins, D. (1963): Relations Among Specific Articulatory Deviations and Responses to a Clinical Measure of Sound Discrimination./. Speech Hear. Dis., 28, 4, 382-388. 25. Raymore, S. (1970): The Effects of Systematic Training Programs on the Generalisation of New Phoneme Responses Across Different Positions in Words. Unpublished Master's Thesis, Kansas State University. 26. Shafer, G. (1974): A Spectrographs Investigation of Acoustic Cues in Reduced Consonant Clusters in Normal and Abnormal Child Speech. Unpublished Research Report, Dept. of Speech Pathology and Audiology, University of the Witwatersrand. 27. Shelton, R.L. Elbert, M. and Arndt, W.B. (1967): A Task for the Evalua- tion of Articulation Change. II. Comparison of Task Scores during Base- line and Lesson Series Testing. /. Speech Hear. Res., 10, 3, 578-585. 28. Sommers, R.K. and Kane, A.R. (1974): Nature and Remediation of Functional Articulation Disorders. Chapter 3 in Communication Dis- orders: Remedial Principles and Practices. Dickson, S.E. (Ed.) Scott, Foresman and Co., Illinois. 29. Van Demark, D.R. (1971): Articulatory Changes in the Therapeutic Process. Cleft Pal. J., 8, 159-165. 30. Van Riper, C. (1972): Speech Correction: Principles and Methods. (5th Edition). Prentice-Hall, Inc., Englewood Cliffs, Ν J : 31. Weber, J.L. (1970): Patterning of Deviant Articulation Behaviour. /. Speech Hear. Dis., 35,2, 135-141. 32. Weiner, P.S. (1967): Auditory Discrimination and Articulation. /. Speech Hear. Dis., 32, 1, 19-28. 33. Winitz, H. and Priesler, L. (1967): The Effect of Distinctive Feature Pretraining in Phoneme Discrimination Learning./. Speech Hear. Res., 10,3,515-530. 34. Winitz, H. (1969): Articulatory Acquisition and Behaviour. Appleton- Century Crofts, N.Y. 35. Wright, V. Shelton, R.L. and Arndt, W.B. (1969): A Task for the Evalua- tion of Articulation Change: III, Imitative Task Scores Compared with Scores for more Spontaneous Tasks. /. Speech Hear. Res., 12, 4, 875-884. Tydskrif van die Suid-Afrikaanse Vereniging vir Spraak- en Gehoorheelkunde Vol. 23, Desember 1976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) Acoustics and Signal Analysis NOISE PROBLEMS? General Radio Has the Solution: A Complete Family of Noise Measuring Instruments for Community and Industrial Noise: n GR1933 — 9 Precision Sound-Level Meter GR1944 Noise-Exposure Indicator and Monitor GR1565-B For more information please contact: Associated Electronics (Pty.) Ltd. P.O. Box 31094, Braamfontein, Phone 839-1824 150 Caroline Street, B r i x t o n , Johannesburg. Journal of the South African Speech and Hearing Association. Vol. 23 December 1976 R ep ro du ce d by S ab in et G at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e P ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2)