(2 rânduri libere, 11p)


Studies and Scientific Researches. Economics Edition, No 20, 2014  http://sceco.ub.ro 

 
BRAND NAMING: SOUND SYMBOLISM, BRAND 

PREFERENCE AND BRAND PERFORMANCE  
 

Alina Duduciuc  
National University of Political Studies and Public Administration 

alina.duduciuc@comunicare.ro 
Loredana Ivan 

National University of Political Studies and Public Administration 
loredana.ivan@comunicare.ro 

 
 
Abstract  
The aim of this study is to highlight the importance of sound symbolism for Romanian 
marketing and advertising applied research. Previous research showed that the phonetic 
structure of brand name communicates its characteristics, i.e. it drives consumers to assess 
certain features and performance of the product. We assumed that when consumers encounter 
an unknown brand name, they automatically infer characteristics from the meaning conveyed 
by the sounds (e.g. phonemes). Therefore, we supposed that a brand name for a shampoo 
(artificially created on experimental purpose) containing back vowels is evaluated better by 
consumers when they compare it to another brand name with front vowels. Furthermore, we 
tested the influence of the stops and fricatives consonants in inferring certain attributes of 
product. To this end, fifty nine students (N=59) participated in a research based on 
questionnaire. The results revealed that subjects evaluated better the brand names containing 
back vowels than brand names with front vowel. No effect was obtained regarding the presence 
of stops and fricatives consonants in assessing the brand performance. 
 
Keywords  
brand; sound symbolism; advertising; social psychology; brand names 
 
JEL Classification  
M31  
 
 
 
Introduction  
Brand naming is considered one of the major determinants in the process of 
positioning and advertising of different goods and services. Releasing new brands on 
the market means naming them for triggering implicit positive associations in the 
mind of the customers. Therefore, a new product should not only be branded, but it 
should determine a positive evaluation towards its features in order to overtake 
competing brands on the market. Although marketers and advertisers have been aware 
of the quality of the product names, it is only recently that they prize the utility of 
sound symbolism research for fitting the creative concept of the brands. 
 
 
Factors affecting perception of brand names: previous studies 
Analyzing what kind of processes and factors could affect brand names evaluation, 
marketers and social psychologists have been focused, particularly, on five factors as 
follows: personalizing the brand (Wansink, Painter, & van Ittersum, 2001), fluency of 
words/brand names (Alter & Daniel Oppenheimer, 2006); name-letter effect (Brendl, 
Chattopadhyay, Pelham, & Carvallo, 2005; Nuttin, 1985); alpha-numeric brand names 
(Gunasti and Ross, 2010) and sound symbolism (Wu, Klink & Guo, 2013; Lowrey & 

169 

 



Duduciuc, Ivan 

Shrum, 2007; Yorkston & Menon, 2004; Klink, 2001). We will review below the 
studies regarding this topics.  
 

Fluency  
One of the main research in brand naming belongs to American psychologists Adam 
Alter and Daniel Oppenheimer (2006), who investigated the impact of the 
psychological principle of fluency (that people tend to prefer easily processed 
information) on short-term share price movements. After they did three experiments – 
one in lab and the other in real-world stock market – they had found that fluently 
named stocks outperformed stocks with diffluent names in the short term. These 
results imply that easily processing of a stimulus (conceptual fluency) mediate human 
behavior and major decisions, in this case where to invest money or who to be trust. 
An important factor affecting the fluency is familiarity: stimuli that have been 
previously encountered and stored in memory are processed more easily than their 
unknown counterparts (Fennis & Stroebe, 2010). Consequently, the ease and 
effortless a piece of information is processed the better is evaluated by consumers. 
Thus, the more fluently a brand name is perceived, the more familiar is considered.  
 

Personalization of brands  
Descriptive brand names such as Jack Daniels, Psychedelic Sorbet or the Booming 
Onion could impact sales or make a customer believe the food tastes better. The 
straightforward names have a positive impact on sales, attitudes and purchase 
intentions, but they did not influence how much money customers would pay for the 
descriptively labelled (Wansink, Brian, James Painter, and Koert van Ittersum, 2001). 
In a field experiment at the cafeteria of Midwestern University, Braian Wansink and 
his collaborators showed that descriptive labels, including geographic labels (Cajun 
and Italian), nostalgia labels (Homestyle or Grandma’s) or sensory labels (tender or 
satin) trigger more favorable associations for foods as compared to the others names. 
The authors manipulated few of the menu’s items so as to be as basic label (e.g., 
grilled chicken) or as descriptive label. Based on data collected with a questionnaire, 
authors revealed an increase of sales for food with descriptive labels, with around 
56% of consumers saying that they selected them as compared with 44% of sample 
who preferred food with regular labels.  
 

Alpha-numeric brand names 
Alpha-numeric brand names are used mostly for most technical products and cars so 
as consumer to make easily associations between numbers and different qualities of 
the brands. After developing a taxonomy of alphanumeric brand names (such as ABs 
type), Gunasti and Ross (2010) found that higher versus lower numeric portion of 
brand names (e.g., X-200 versus X-100) is preferred more even when it is objectively 
inferior to other choice alternatives. Consumers with low need for cognition apply 
“the higher, the better” heuristic to select brands labeled with ABs and choose 
products with higher numeric portions. At the same time, consumers with high need 
for cognition involved more cognitive process for making inferences based on brand 
name–attribute correlations (Gunasti, 2010).  
 

Name-letter effect and implicit egoism  
Name Letter Effect (NLE) – that is people like letters contained in their name more 
than other letters (Nuttin, 1985) – has been amply replicated in the field of consumer 
psychology (see e.g. Brendl, Chattopadhyay, Pelham, & Carvallo, 2005) and was 
largely applied in promoting goods in certain countries (see Share a Coke in 150 
countries including Romania). The research of Pelham et al. (2002) showed that an 

170 

 



Brand naming: sound symbolism, brand preference and brand performance 

important percentage of people choose states, cities and occupations with names 
resembling their own (e.g., that Lauras are lawyers and Florences live in Florida). 
This phenomenon is called implicit egotism and has been observed in many cultures 
around the world. In the field of advertising, Brendel et al. (2005) reported that the 
causal effect of implicit egotism works outside the lab, in a real market situation. Not 
only that people’s names influence their choices, but also they prefer a brand name 
that resemble (i.e. Tonya) with their name rather than a real well-known brand (i.e. 
Twix). 
 

Sound Symbolism  
Sound Symbolism – the nonarbitrary relation between a sound of a word and its 
meaning – has been widely recognized as an important factor in how individuals infer 
specific meaning from unfamiliar brand names (Wu, Klink & Guo, 2013; Lowrey & 
Shrum, 2007; Klink, 2001). A brand name is composed of sounds called phonemes. 
These phonemes have two functions. First, they are the basic components of language 
and are used to form syllables and words. In fact, a brand name is also a computed 
series of syllables that turn out in a word with or without meaning. Second, phonemes 
can provide meaning by means of sound. Consequently, consumers infer meaning and 
evaluate the performance of products based on the cues provide by sounds of brand 
names (Yorkston & Menon, 2004).  
In a set of studies conducted by Richard Klink (2000, 2001, 2003), he showed that the 
presence of back-low (a, o, u) and high-front vowels (e, i), in brand names could 
conveys attribute qualities of smallness, lightness, mildness, thinness, fastness, 
coldness, bitterness, femininity, weakness, lightness, and prettiness. Furthermore, 
Klink (2001) brought empirical data about the effects of sound symbolism in affecting 
consumers’ evaluations of various brands. In addition to vowel sounds, research 
documents that consonant sounds also convey inherent meaning based on the length 
of the sound they produce (Klink, 2000; Lowrey, Shrum & Dubitsky, 2003). Stops 
and fricatives differ in their manner of articulation. The letters p, t, b, g, d, and k (or 
hard c) are considered stops. Fricatives (s, v, z), on the other hand, have less closure 
of articulators than stops. Approaching the sound symbolism, scholars demonstrated 
through laboratory experiments that frond vowels and fricatives were more associated 
with smaller and more angular shapes than were back vowels and stops (Klink, 2004). 
In another allied work, E. A. Yorkston and G. Menon (2002) found that when 
consumer chose between two brands of ice cream, Frish and Frosh, they preferred the 
latter (Frosh) instead of the former (Frish) as a result of associating the sound [ä from 
Frosh] with attributes such as bigger, heavier and slower. More recently, T.M. 
Lowery și L.J. Shrum (2007) establish evidence that people prefer particular words as 
brand names when the attributes connoted by the vowel sounds of the word are 
congruent with the attributes of the products. Specifically, when the back vowel 
sound [ä] was juxtaposed with the front vowel sound [i], the vowel [ä] sound word 
was preferred only when the attributes implied by the sound (large, slow, dull) had 
positive implication for the product (i.e. SUV, hammer).  
When brand names is meaningless, sound symbolism – images, qualities and 
emotions that people unconsciously associate with vowel sounds and consonants – 
has a certain weight in consumer choices. The phonetic principles have been recently 
included as one of the main direction in marketing research as well as within branding 
companies. As the member of the Lexicon Branding1 declared “in an economy where 

1 Lexicon Branding, Inc is an U.S. marketing company specializing in brand naming. They created the 
famous brands such as Toyota, Pentium, Blackberry and conducted a global research program called 
Sounder to determine whether certain phonemes are more effective than others when communicate 
attributes of the brand. Source: lexiconbranding.com 

171 

 

                                                           



Duduciuc, Ivan 

global brands are the norm rather than the exception, a greater understanding of sound 
symbolism has put Lexicon that much closer to our ultimate goal of deciphering and 
implementing what we call the universal language of branding” 
(lexicombranding.com). 
As far as we know, no research has been conducted to test the effect of sound 
symbolism of the Romanian language. Therefore, the present study represents an 
exploratory research on the influence of sound symbolism in communicating brand 
characteristics. Specifically, we assumed that when consumers encounter new and 
unfamiliar brand names, they often infer brand features from the meaning conveyed 
by the intensity and frequency of the sounds as they roll on the tongue via the 
airstream (i.e. the vowels and consonants).  
 
 

Methodology  
Based on the previous research, we framed and tested the following four hypotheses: 
Hypothesis 1. Consumers evaluate the intrinsic proprieties of a brand name containing 
fricatives (i.e. Simeli), as better than a brand name containing stops (i.e. Pimeli). 
Hypothesis 2. Consumers evaluate the intrinsic proprieties of a brand name containing 
front vowels (i.e. Simeli), as better than a brand name containing back vowels (i.e. 
Sameli). 
Hypothesis 3. Brand names with front vowels (i.e. Simeli) are associated more with 
feminine brand characteristics than brand names containing back vowels (i.e. Sameli). 
Hypothesis 4. Brand names with stops (i.e. Pimeli) are associated more with 
masculine characteristics than brand names containing fricatives (i.e. Simeli). 
 

Procedure, sample and developing of questionnaire  
Fifty nine (N=59) students enrolled in a Social Psychology Master level course 

participated in a study for an extra credits in April 2014. Participants were aged 
between 23 (N=26) and 24 (N=23). They had to fill in a questionnaire regarding the 
evaluation of for fictitious brand names for a shampoo (Simeli/Sameli; Simeli/Pimeli). 
The brand names have been created according to the principles of phonology 
mentioned earlier. Consequently, I manipulated only four phonemes: front vowel 
(Simeli) and back vowel (Sameli), stop (Pimeli) and fricative (Simeli). I asked 
subjects to choose which of the four names (Simeli/Sameli/Pimeli/Simeli) would be 
appropriated for designating a hair-loss treatment shampoo.  Secondly, they had to 
mention which brand name fits the category “products for women” versus “products 
for men”. The questionnaire also assessed the preferences of the respondents 
concerning Romanian letters, the evaluation of each brand name on a 5-point scale (1 
= strongly agree; 5 =strongly disagree) and socio-demographics (gender, age, 
income, education, residence). 

 
Results 
Preference for Romanian letters. As can be seen from the Table no.1, the 

most attractive letters in the Romanian alphabet are A (16,9 %) , L (13,6%), P (11,9) 
and C (10,2 %) as opposed to the letters M, O, D and Y, with around 2% of subjects 
saying that they prefer it. The research did not reveal any gender effects because in 
the sample, females (N=46) outnumbered males (N=10).  

 
 

 
 
 

172 

 



Brand naming: sound symbolism, brand preference and brand performance 

Table 1. Preference for certain Romanian letters  
 

Romanian letters N 
 

% 

A 10 16,9 

D 1 1,7 

L 8 13,6 

M 1 1,7 

O 1 1,7 

P 7 11,9 

Y 1 1,7 

Total 56 94,9 

 
Preference of brands according to the stops and fricatives. The difference 
between the usage of fricatives and stops in a brand name did not turn out to be 
statistically significant. For designating a hair-loss treatment shampoo, the 
respondents preferred the name which began with a stop consonant, Pimeli (35,5%) as 
compared to a fricative consonant (Simeli, 30,5%). Otherwise, the percentage of the 
subjects saying they preferred the name Pimeli instead of Simeli is not statistically 
significant. This is also showed by the values of ANOVA (F=0.46; p=0.94>0.05) test 
which is higher than 0.05. 

 
Tabel 2. Preference of stop versus fricative brand name 

 
Brand name N % 

 
Simeli 18 30.5 

Pimeli 21 35.6 

I don’t know/don’t 
respond 

17 28.8 

Missing responses 3 5.1 

Total 59 100 

 
The preference of brands according to the front and back vowels 

When the subjects chose between a brand name made up of front vowels (Simeli) and 
one with back vowels (Sameli), they preferred the latter, Sameli. Almost 39% of 

173 

 



Duduciuc, Ivan 

subjects declared that they preferred the name Sameli instead of Simeli for designating 
a hair-loss treatment shampoo. 

 
Tabel 3. Preference of back versus front vowels name 

 
Brand name N % 

 
Sameli 23 39.0 

Simeli 16 27.1 

I don’t know/don’t 
respond 

17 28.8 

Total 56 94.9 

Missing responses 3 5.1 

Total 59 100 

 
 

Sound symbolism and brand performance  
The outcomes of evaluating products according to their characteristics, namely how 
much the subjects believed that either Sameli or  Simeli designated a hair loss 
shampoo, were statistically significant (F=134.083; p=0,001<0.05). On average, 
subjects tend to believe that Sameli is more efficient than Simeli in preventing hair 
loss.  
 

Table 3. ANOVA test 
 

 Sum of squere df Mean 
square 

F Sig 

Between 
Groups 

32.862 2 16.431 134.083 .000 

Within 
Groups 

6.495 53 .123 
  

Total 39.357 55    

 
In this research, the brand names are equally rated, whether they began with fricatives 
or stops. The presence of stops or fricatives had no effect in the evaluation of brand 
characteristics – whether they contained.  Subjects rated brand name Simeli (almost 
29%) as efficient as Pimeli (almost 30%) for shampoos preventing hair loss (Tabel 4). 
 
 
 
 
 

174 

 



Brand naming: sound symbolism, brand preference and brand performance 

Tabel 4. Evaluation of Pimeli and Simeli brand name 
 

Brand name N % 
 

Simeli 17 28.8 

Pimeli 18 30.5 

I don’t know/don’t 
respond 

21 35.6 

Total 56 94.9 

Missing responses 3 5.1 

Total 59 100 

 
Sound symbolism and brand characteristics  

When testing the hypothesis – i.e. brand names with front vowels (Simeli) are 
associated more with feminine brand characteristics than brand names containing 
back vowels (Sameli) – subjects rated Sameli and Simeli as having the same effect. On 
the other hand, Simeli was categorized as a more feminine (62,7%) product than 
Pimeli (16,9%). In other words Pimeli is perceived as being (62,7%) more masculine 
than Simeli (15,3%). 
 
 

Conclusion 
In this article, we provided empirical support for three of our four assumptions. On 
one hand, students evaluate better the performance of brands containing back vowels 
[a] than front vowels [e]. Meanwhile, the presence of stops [p] and fricatives [s] did 
not turn out to have any effect regardless the preference for brand names. The 
prevalence of evaluating brand names with sound vowel [a] as better than others, in 
this case the usage of back vowels (Sameli), could be linked to the mere exposure 
effect (R. Zajonic, 1968). In Romanian language, girls’ first names and feminine 
nouns often end in a. Consequently, most students from our research assigned 
feminine attributes to a brand name that contains an emphasis on the vowel a. The 
subjects might automatically associate the familiarity of the vowel [a] with their 
attractiveness. On the other hand, we can explain these results taking into account that 
in the current sample female outnumber male. The larger presence of women (F=49) 
as compared to men (M=10) could led to the preference for more feminine words (in 
this case, Sameli) and, consequently, to their better evaluation.   
Following the outcomes of present research, no effect of stops and fricatives were 
obtained, whether the subjects chose the brand name for a shampoo or they assess 
their performance. But when it comes to convey meanings and communicating 
characteristics, as the results of the present research revealed, vowels possess an 
inherent meaning of womanliness while consonants, in this case the stop [p] from 
Pimeli, are perceived as more masculine than word with fricative [s, Simeli]. In this 
respect, our research is consistent with previous ones (Klink, 2000; Yorkston & 
Menon, 2004; Lowrey & Shrum, 2007) with respect to the effect of stops on brand 
characteristics. 

175 

 



Duduciuc, Ivan 

To conclude, several limits must be taken into account, such as the short number of 
participants in the study and their similarities in terms of socio-demographics 
characteristics. Further research on phonetic symbolism could investigate how other 
variables, for instance the type of product (food, electronics, automobiles, body care 
products) fit with the sounds as well as with the concept of the brand. Nevertheless, 
our study could be considered a starting point in developing the applied advertising 
research in Romania as long as it provides empirical evidence that the sounds 
generated by vowels and consonants count in the perception of young people as well 
in defining the brand on the market. 
 
 
References  
Alter, A.L., Oppenheimer, D. M. (2006). Predicting short-term stock fluctuations by 

using processing fluency, Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences 
of the USA. Available of the URL 
http://www.pnas.org/content/103/24/9369.full 

Brendl, C. M., Chattopadhyay, A., Pelham, B.W., Carvallo, M. (2005). Name Letter 
Branding: Valence Transfers When Product Specific Needs Are Active, 
Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 405-415. 

Fennis, B.M, Stroebe, W., (2012). The Psychology of Advertising, New York, 
Psychology Press. 

Gunasti, K., Ross, W.T, (2010). How and when alphanumeric brand names affect 
consumer preferences, Journal of Marketing Research, XLVII, 1177-1192. 

Kahneman, D. (2012). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York, Penguin Books.  
Klink, R. R. (2000), Creating Brand Names with Meaning: The Use of Sound 

Symbolism, Marketing Letters, 11 (1), 5–20. 
Klink, R. R. (2001), Creating meaningful new brand names: a study of semantics and 

sound symbolism, Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 9 (2), 27–34. 
Klink, R. R. (2003), Creating meaningful brands: the relationship between brand 

name and brand mark, Marketing Letters, 14 (3), 143–157. 
Lowrey, T. M., Shrum, L.J. (2007). Phonetic Symbolism and Brand Name Preference, 

Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (3), 406–414. 
Nuttin, J. M. (1985). Narcissism beyond Gestalt and Awareness: The Name Letter 

Effect, European Journal of Social Psychology, 15 , 353-61. 
Pelham, B. W.,. Carvallo, M. R,  DeHart, T., Jones, J. T.  (2003). Assessing the 

Validity of Implicit Egotism: A Reply to Gallucci, Journal of Personality 
and Social Psychology, 85 (November), 800-807. 

Vanden, B., Alder, G. B. K., Oliver, L. (1987). Linguistic distinction among top brand 
names, Journal of Advertising Research, 27(4), 39–44. 

Wansink, B., Painter, J., Ittersum, van Koert. (2001). Do Descriptive Menu Labels 
Influence Restaurant Sales and Repatronage? Descriptive Meniu Labels’ 
Effect on Sales,  Cornell hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly,42 
(4), 68-72. 

Wu, L.,. Klink, R. R., Guo, J. (2013). Creating Gender Brand Personality with Brand 
Names: The Effects of Phonetic Symbolism,  Journal of Marketing Theory 
and Practice, 21, (3), 319–329. 

Yorkston, E., and Geeta Menon (2004). A Sound Idea: Phonetic Effects of Brand 
Names on Consumer Judgments, Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (1), 43–
51. 

 
 

176