Department of Ear, Nose & Throat, University of Siena, Siena, Italy
*Corresponding Author e-mail: ishajacopo@hotmail.it

توطني املصدر الصويت حتت املاء بني الغواصني املكفوفني واملبصرين
دراسة مقارنة

جاكوبو كامبي، لودوفيكا ليفي، والرت ليفي

abstract: Objectives: Many blind individuals demonstrate enhanced auditory spatial discrimination or locali-
sation of sound sources in comparison to sighted subjects. However, this hypothesis has not yet been confirmed 
with regards to underwater spatial localisation. This study therefore aimed to investigate underwater acoustic 
source localisation among blind and sighted scuba divers. Methods: This study took place between February 
and June 2015 in Elba, Italy, and involved two experimental groups of divers with either acquired (n = 20) or 
congenital (n = 10) blindness and a control group of 30 sighted divers. Each subject took part in five attempts 
at an under-water acoustic source localisation task, in which the divers were requested to swim to the source 
of a sound originating from one of 24 potential locations. The control group had their sight obscured during the task. 
Results: The congenitally blind divers demonstrated significantly better underwater sound localisation compared 
to the control group or those with acquired blindness (P = 0.0007). In addition, there was a significant correlation 
between years of blindness and underwater sound localisation (P <0.0001). Conclusion: Congenital blindness 
was found to positively affect the ability of a diver to recognise the source of a sound in an underwater environment. 
As the correct localisation of sounds underwater may help individuals to avoid imminent danger, divers should 
perform sound localisation tests during training sessions.

Keywords: Blindness; Auditory Perception; Sound Localization; Spatial Processing; Diving.

املب�رشين.  من  بنظرائهم  مقارنة  ال�سوت  م�سادر  لتحديد  �سمعيا  و  مكانيا  متيزا  املكفوفني  االأفراد  من  العديد  يظهر  الهدف:  امللخ�ص: 
امل�سادر  توطني  يف  البحث  اإىل  الدرا�سة  هذه  هدفت  لذا،  املاء.  حتت  املكاين  بالتوطني  يتعلق  فيما  الفر�سية  هذه  تاأكيد  يتم  مل  ذلك،  ومع 
2015 وحزيران/يونيه  �سباط/فرباير  بني  الدرا�سة  هذه  اأجريت  وقد  الطريقة:  واملب�رشين.  املكفوفني  الغوا�سني  بني  املاء  حتت   ال�سوتية 
يف اإلبا باإيطاليا، و�سملت جمموعتني جتريبيتني من الغوا�سني امل�سابني اإما بعمى مكت�سب )n = 20( اأو عمى خلقي )n = 10( وجمموعة 
�سابطة من 30 من الغوا�سني املب�رشين. وقام كل م�سارك بخم�س حماوالت لتوطني م�سدر �سوتي حتت املاء، طلب فيها من الغوا�سني 
ال�سباحة اإىل م�سدر �سوتي واحد من 24 موقعا حمتمال. وقد حجبت املجموعة ال�سابطة الب�رش خالل املهمة. النتائج: اأظهرت النتائج اأن 
الغوا�سني امل�سابني بالعمى اخللقي اأف�سل بكثري يف توطني ال�سوت حتت املاء مقارنة مع املجموعة ال�سابطة اأو تلك امل�سابة بالعمى 
املكت�سب )P = 0.0007(. وباالإ�سافة اإىل ذلك، كان هناك ارتباط كبري بني عدد �سنوات االأ�سابة بالعمى وقدرة توطني ال�سوت حتت املاء 
اأن  املاء.  حتت  ال�سوت  م�سدر  على  التعرف  على  الغوا�س  قدرة  على  اإيجابي  ب�سكل  يوؤثر  اخللقي  العمى  اأن  وجد  P(. اخلال�صة:   <0.0001(
اختبارات  باإجراء  للغوا�سني  ين�سح  فاأنه  ولذلك  املخاطرالو�سيكة،  جتنب  على  االأفراد  ي�ساعد  قد  املاء  حتت  لالأ�سوات  ال�سحيح  التوطني 

توطني ال�سوت خالل الدورات التدريبية.
الكلمات املفتاحية: العمى؛ االإدراك ال�سمعي؛ توطني ال�سوت؛ املعاجلة املكانية؛ الغو�س.

Underwater Acoustic Source Localisation Among 
Blind and Sighted Scuba Divers

Comparative study
*Jacopo Cambi, Ludovica Livi, Walter Livi

clinical & basic research

Advances in Knowledge 
- To the best of the authors’ knowledge, this study is the first to investigate the sound recognition capabilities of both blind and sighted 

subjects in an underwater environment.
- The findings of this study indicated that subjects who were congenitally blind demonstrated superior spatial discrimination of an 

underwater sound source.
- Among individuals who are blind, sensory substitution in the cortex may explain enhanced localisation of an underwater sound source, 

which is an extremely difficult task for a person who is sighted.

Application to Patient Care
- The findings of this study indicated that congenitally blind subjects were significantly more able to locate underwater sound sources. As 

such, healthcare practitioners should encourage both sighted divers and those with acquired blindness to undertake additional sound 
localisation tests during training sessions. 

Sultan Qaboos University Med J, May 2017, Vol. 17, Iss. 2, pp. e168–173, Epub. 20 Jun 17
Submitted 27 Dec 16
Revision Req. 15 Feb 17; Revision Recd. 26 Feb 17
Accepted 9 Mar 17 doi: 10.18295/squmj.2016.17.02.006



Jacopo Cambi, Ludovica Livi and Walter Livi

Clinical and Basic Research | e169

Sound waves travel through the air at a speed of approximately 300 m/second; however, since water is denser than air, sound waves 
travel at 1,471 m/second underwater.1 In addition, 
variations in salinity, temperature and pressure 
(i.e. depth) also influence the propagation of sound 
underwater.1 The human ear functions relatively poorly 
underwater because the external ear canal fills with 
water, the eardrum moves less and the resonant 
frequency shifts to the bass tones. In such circum-
stances, most individuals hear via bone conduction and 
scuba divers consequently are more able to perceive 
low-frequency sounds than high-frequency sounds; 
however, the water has an overall sound damping 
effect.2 Another peculiarity of underwater hearing is 
the difficulty in establishing the precise location of a 
source of sound; this therefore has important safety 
implications for scuba divers in terms of hazard 
perception.

The ability of the brain to learn or relearn to 
localise sounds does not necessitate visuomotor feed-
back as the neural paths assigned to spatial hearing can 
be set up in a vision-independent manner.3 Previous 
research on sound localisation among people who 
are blind has been based on the theory of cross-
modal compensatory plasticity in which increased 
cortical activity has been hypothesised to compensate 
or hypercompensate for the loss of visual calibration 
in spatial hearing by employing non-visual sensory 
signals.4 Using neuroimaging techniques, several studies 
have suggested that posterior visual areas in subjects 
who are blind may be active during the performance of 
non-visual tasks such as auditory localisation.5,6

Nevertheless, while some studies have reported 
that blind individuals show better auditory spatial 
discrimination or localisation of sound sources than 
sighted subjects, other studies have failed to demonstrate 
this gain.7–10 Recently, Finocchietti et al. reported that 
localisation of sound in the mid-sagittal plane was 
poorer among blind individuals than sighted controls, 
indicating a clear deficit in encoding the sound motion 
in the lower hemisphere of the spherical coordinate 
system; however, the researchers confirmed that no 
such deficit was observed among congenitally blind 
subjects.11 Voss et al. confirmed that individuals who 
are blind demonstrated supernormal abilities in the 
recognition of sound sources both nearby and at a 
distance.12 However, several other researchers have 
reported inferior performances during experimental 
tasks of spatial sound recognition among subjects who 
were blind compared to those who were sighted.13,14 

To the best of the authors’ knowledge, the 
localisation of spatial sounds underwater has never 
before been evaluated among both blind and sighted 

subjects. This is significant as, in water, sounds can 
arise from any point in a three-dimensional setting, 
including the vertical as well as horizontal planes. This 
study therefore aimed to evaluate underwater sound 
recognition among both blind and sighted scuba divers 
as well as to evaluate differences in sound recognition 
between subjects with acquired blindness and those 
with congenital blindness.

Methods

This study took place between February and June 
2015 in Elba, Italy. A total of 60 subjects were 
recruited to participate in the study, of which 30 
participants had either acquired (n = 20) or congenital 
(n = 10) visual impairment and 30 had no visual 
impairment. Subjects were considered congenitally 
blind if their visual acuity was below 1/300 within 
the first year of life, indicating that they were unable 
to detect hand movements at a distance of 1 m from 
their better eye. All participants had been scuba 
diving for at least a year before the beginning of the 
study; the sighted divers had a minimum of an 
advanced open water diver qualification and at least 
20 underwater dives in the preceding two years. 
The blind subjects were recruited with the aid of 
blind scuba diver partnerships. An audiological and 
motor function examination was performed on all 
subjects, including otoscopy, pure tone audiometry, 
tympanometry and tubaric function tests. All of the 
subjects had normal hearing levels at ≤20 decibels (dB) 
and no history of hearing disorders. 

Sound localisation tasks were performed in the 
Secca della Torre, a bay with a rocky seabed located 
in Marciana Marina, Elba. The tasks took place in 
an underwater test area of 10 m x 10 m in which the 
average depth was 22 m and the maximum depth 
was 38 m. Each subject entered the water and moved 
to the centre of the test area at a depth of approxi-
mately 5 m. Subsequently, a sound was produced 
approximately 5 m away from the diver by rhythmi-
cally hitting a metal screwdriver against the side 
of a diving tank at a frequency of 1 Hz and a rate of 
60 times per minute. The estimated sound pressure level 
was 90 dB with a frequency band range of 2–8 KHz. 

The sound originated from one of 24 possible 
locations with each location varying by 45° on three 
horizontal planes (either on the surface, at a depth 
of 5 m or at a depth of 10 m). Each plane had eight 
possible origin points of the sound. The sound source 
was determined randomly by throwing a 24-sided 
polyhedral dice [Figure 1]. Each subject repeated the 
task five times. Some of the individuals in the congenital 
blindness group had residual light perception and were 



Underwater Acoustic Source Localisation Among Blind and Sighted Scuba Divers 
Comparative study

e170 | SQU Medical Journal, May 2017, Volume 17, Issue 2

therefore required to perform the test with their eyes 
closed. Subjects in the control group were requested 
to perform the tests with closed eyes while wearing a 
diving mask which obscured their vision.

Completion of the sound localisation task was 
scored as follows. Subjects were assigned one point if 
they reached the sound source in less than a minute 
without changing course. Half a point was awarded 
if they reached a position within 45° of the sound source 
on the horizontal plane and then corrected their route 
(e.g. if the sound source was at position nine and the 

diver initially swam towards positions 16 or 10) or if 
they reached a position deeper or shallower than the 
sound source and then corrected their route (e.g. if 
the sound source was at position nine and the diver 
initially swam towards position one). The participants 
were assigned zero points if they did not reach the 
sound source within one minute or swam more than 
45° away from the sound source. The total number of 
points for the five task attempts was then summed up 
for each diver to constitute the total task score.

Data were analysed using the Statistical Package 
for the Social Sciences (SPSS), Version 16.0 (IBM 
Corp., Armonk, New York, USA). The participants 
were divided into three groups for the purposes of 
the statistical analysis: the acquired blindness group, 
the congenital blindness group and the control 
group of sighted subjects. Percentages of exact locali- 
sation of the sound source were computed indepen-
dently for each subject and each group. Comparisons 
between groups were assessed using either a t-test, 
analysis of variance or Spearman’s rank-correlation 
coefficient analysis, as appropriate. A P value of 
<0.0500 was considered statistically significant.

This study was performed in accordance with 
the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Informed written consent was obtained from all of 
the participants prior to their inclusion in the study.

Results

Among the 60 divers included in the study, 20 
were female and 40 were male. The mean age was 
36.9 ± 10.1 years. The mean number of years of 
diving experience was 5.0 ± 1.4, while the mean 

Figure 1: Diagram showing the experimental set-up of 
an underwater sound localisation task for blind and sighted 
scuba divers. Each task took place in an underwater test 
area of 10 m x 10 m with the diver located in the middle 
at a depth of 5 m. Subsequently, sounds originated from 
one of 24 potential sources on three horizontal planes, 
with the diver required to reach the sound source within 
one minute in as direct a route as possible.

Table 1: Demographic and clinical characteristics and test scores of blind and sighted scuba divers participating in an 
underwater sound localisation task (N = 60)

Characteristic Mean ± SD

Acquired 
blindness group 

(n = 20)

Congenital 
blindness group 

(n = 10)

Control group 
(n = 30)

Total 
(N = 60)

Age in years 37.2 ± 9.1 37.1 ± 10.0 36.7 ± 10.4 36.9 ± 10.1

Male-to-female ratio 15:5 7:3 18:12 40:20

Years of blindness 14.6 ± 8.0 37.1 ± 10.0 - -

Years of diving experience 4.9 ± 8.0 5.5 ± 0.9 5.0 ± 1.4 5.0 ± 1.4

Total task score* 2.6 ± 1.8 4.2 ± 0.8 1.8 ± 1.5 2.5 ± 1.5

Time taken to perform task in seconds 46.0 ± 8.8 39.0 ± 6.5 46.6 ± 7.8 45.1 ± 7.8

Number of perfect task scores† out of the total 
number of task attempts

33/100 36/50 36/150 105/300

SD = standard deviation.
*Total score after five attempts at the task.
†Perfect scores were awarded if subjects reached the sound source in less than a minute without changing course.



Jacopo Cambi, Ludovica Livi and Walter Livi

Clinical and Basic Research | e171

number of years of blindness was 14.6 ± 8.0 and 
37.1 ± 10.0 for the acquired blindness and congenital 
blindness groups, respectively [Table 1]. Among the 
acquired blindness group, 14 patients had retinitis 
pigmentosa, while three had glaucoma and one patient 
each had macular degeneration, a bilateral ocular 
injury or optic nerve atrophy. There were no significant 
differences in age, gender distribution or number of 
years of diving experience between the three groups. 

There was a statistically significant difference 
between the total task scores of the congenital 
blindness and acquired blindness groups as well as 
between the congenital blindness and control groups 
(P = 0.0007) [Figure 2]. However, no significant difference 
was observed between the total scores of control 
subjects and those with acquired blindness. There 
was a significant positive association between the 

total sound localisation score and number of 
years of blindness (P <0.0001) and a significant 
negative correlation between the time taken to 
execute the task and number of years of blindness 
(P = 0.0452). However, no correlations were noted 
between age and years of diving experience or 
between time taken to execute the task and years of 
diving experience [Table 2]. In an independent analysis 
of the acquired blindness group, there was a positive 
correlation between the number of years of blindness 
and the total task score (P = 0.0231). 

Discussion

While numerous studies have investigated sound 
localisation among sighted and blind subjects, most 
have focused on comparing sound localisation in two 
and three dimensions or on identifying mechanisms 
and conditions which favour individuals who are 
congenitally blind.3,15,16 To the best of the authors’ 
knowledge, the current study is the first to investigate 
the sound recognition capabilities of blind and sighted 
subjects in an underwater environment, comparing 
divers who were congenitally blind, those with 
acquired visual loss and a sighted control group whose 
vision was temporarily obscured. Moreover, previous 
research has limited sound localisation experiments 
among blind individuals to one horizontal plane 
or to a small number of potential sound source 
positions.17 In the current study, a high number of 
potential sound source locations were included on 
three different horizontal planes; these experimental 
conditions therefore more accurately reflected the 
real-life experience of scuba diving in an open 
water environment. 

During auditory processing, a lag period of 
about 0.6 milliseconds—known as the interaural time 
difference (ITD)—permits location of a sound source 

Figure 2: Bar chart showing the mean total task scores* 
of blind and sighted scuba divers participating in an 
underwater sound localisation task (N = 60). There was a 
significant difference between the scores of divers who 
were congenitally blind in comparison to sighted subjects 
and those with acquired blindness (P = 0.0007).†

*Total score after five attempts at the task.
†Using Dunn’s multiple comparison test.

Table 2: Correlation matrix* between demographic and clinical characteristics and test scores among blind and 
sighted scuba divers participating in an underwater sound localisation task (N = 60)

Correlation coefficient (P value)

Years of 
blindness

Years of diving 
experience

Time taken to 
perform task

Total task score† Age

Years of blindness - 0.1298 (0.3225) -0.2594 (0.0452)‡ 0.5145 (0.0001)‡ 0.0670 (0.6108)

Years of diving experience 0.1298 (0.3225) - -0.0336 (0.7987) 0.1646 (0.2086) 0.1052 (0.4232)

Time taken to perform task -0.2594 (0.0452)‡ -0.0336 (0.7987) - -0.1465 (0.2639) 0.0364 (0.7821)

Total task score† 0.5145 (0.0001)‡ 0.1646 (0.2086) -0.1465 (0.2639) - 0.1585 (0.2261)

Age 0.0670 (0.6108) 0.1052 (0.4232) 0.0364 (0.7821) 0.1585 (0.2261) -

*Using Spearman’s rank-correlation coefficient.
†Total score after five attempts at the task.
‡Statistically significant at P <0.0500.



Underwater Acoustic Source Localisation Among Blind and Sighted Scuba Divers 
Comparative study

e172 | SQU Medical Journal, May 2017, Volume 17, Issue 2

in air, but does not occur under water due to the 
rapid propagation of sound.18 According to functional 
magnetic resonance imaging, four anatomical 
structures are involved during ITD: the superior olivary 
complex in the pons, in which loudness is analysed 
by the medial nuclei and spatial laterality by the 
lateral nuclei; the midbrain inferior colliculus, which 
senses duration, loudness, frequency, spatial domain, 
amplitude modulation and bi-aural interactions; the 
medial geniculate body, which determines spatial 
localisation and differences in interaural intensity; and 
the posterior parietal and frontal cortexes.19,20 

Usually, a spherical coordinate scheme is used to 
express the origin of a sound relative to the position 
or orientation of the cranium, with sounds detectable 
on the frontal, horizontal and median planes. When 
a sound arrives at the ear, the auditory signal is 
successively transmitted by the neuronal nuclei in 
the auditory pathway in the brainstem until it reaches 
the auditory cortex. However, underwater sounds are 
perceived as if they originate symmetrically in front, 
behind and above the subject.21 In addition, the head 
shadow effect (i.e. a reduction in sound amplitude due 
to the volume of head) is missing underwater, so the 
discrimination of high-frequency (>3,000 Hz) sound 
sources is difficult.22 Recently, Shupak et al. tested the 
use of masks with an air pocket around the ears as a 
method of improving underwater auditory perception; 
however, the authors concluded that the masks did not 
significantly improve underwater hearing.23

Among scuba divers, poor sound localisation is 
a source of concern as certain sounds may indicate 
imminent danger, such as an approaching motorboat. 
In the current study, the results of the spatial 
localisation tasks among the sighted controls indicated 
that they could only immediately identify the source of 
sound at a rate of 24%; therefore, the task was feasible 
in only one out of four attempts. Hypothetically, simply 
guessing the source of the sound using mathematical 
probability would result in seven correct answers, 
which is much lower than the actual rate of correct 
localisation. Additionally, the average task execution 
time and rate of correction was relatively higher 
in the control group, with subjects making many 
changes in course. Divers should therefore practice 
sound localisation during their training so as to 
more accurately perceive sources of potential hazards 
while underwater.

As confirmed by Wightman et al., it is easier to 
identify the source of a sound which originates closer 
to the subject than those which are more remote; 
therefore, sound localisation tasks become much 
easier during execution as the subjects move closer 
to the source.24 In accordance with existing scientific 

literature, the current researchers observed that many 
divers started their route more medially than ideal, 
as if exposing one ear towards the sound so as to aid 
understanding of where the sound was generated.25 
In addition, interaural temporal and intensity differ- 
ences are reduced in water; this explains why divers 
have difficulty in determining from which side a 
sound originates.2

The results of the current study agree with those 
of the existing literature in demonstrating enhanced 
spatial sound recognition among blind subjects in 
comparison to sighted controls.3,26 In particular, 
subjects who were blind from birth spent less time 
reaching the sound source and more often obtained 
perfect results; they also changed or adjusted their 
direction less frequently when moving towards the 
source of the sound. One theory to explain these 
findings is that certain auditory brain areas implicated 
in auditory motion perception may show greater 
activation among people who are blind; for example, 
neuroimaging has revealed that the posterior parietal 
cortex in blind subjects is more intensely activated 
during tasks of immobile sound localisation and 
motion discrimination than in sighted controls.6,27–29 
Another hypothesis focuses on compensatory plasticity 
in blindness, which may result in the recruitment of 
deafferented occipital cortical areas for the processing 
of auditory data; this theory is based on the strong 
extension and improvement of pre-existing neuronal 
pathways in the occipital cortex for processing 
auditory information.29,30 In the current study, a 
positive correlation was noted between number of 
years of blindness and task scores among subjects 
with acquired blindness; this finding indicates that, for 
two blind individuals of the same age, the individual 
who lost their sight earlier may have developed more 
effective spatial sound localisation abilities.

A major limitation of the present study was that 
the sounds used in the sound localisation task were 
not studied with a hydrophone system to determine 
the exact frequency and intensity of the sound upon 
reaching the diver. However, all subjects had normal 
hearing levels and they all reported hearing the 
rhythmic sounds very clearly. Another limitation is 
that, in seawater, different temperature levels can act 
as acoustic insulators;1 however, to counteract this, all 
tasks were conducted in the morning, when the sea 
was calm and at an average temperature of 20 ± 2 °C.

Conclusion

Subjects who were congenitally blind demonstrated 
superior spatial discrimination abilities in terms of 
locating an underwater sound source. These results 



Jacopo Cambi, Ludovica Livi and Walter Livi

Clinical and Basic Research | e173

therefore add to the growing body of evidence 
supporting sensory substitution or hypercompensation 
among individuals who are blind. 

c o n f l i c t o f i n t e r e s t
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

f u n d i n g

No funding was received for this study.

References
1. Lurton X. An Introduction to Underwater Acoustics: Principles 

and applications, 2nd ed. London, UK: Springer, 2010. Pp. 13–20. 

2. Hollien H. Underwater sound localization in humans. J Acoust 
Soc Am 1973; 53:1288–95. doi: 10.1121/1.1913466.

3. King AJ. Visual influences on auditory spatial learning. Philos 
Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2009; 364:331–9. doi: 10.1098/
rstb.2008.0230.

4. Kujala T, Alho K, Huotilainen M, Ilmoniemi RJ, Lehtokoski A, 
Leinonen A, et al. Electrophysiological evidence for cross-
modal plasticity in humans with early- and late-onset blindness. 
Psychophysiology 1997; 34:213–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986. 
1997.tb02134.x.

5. Leclerc C, Saint-Amour D, Lavoie ME, Lassonde M, Lepore F. 
Brain functional reorganization in early blind humans revealed 
by auditory event-related potentials. Neuroreport 2000; 11:545–50.

6. Weeks R, Horwitz B, Aziz-Sultan A, Tian B, Wessinger CM, 
Cohen LG, et al. A positron emission tomographic study of 
auditory localization in the congenitally blind. J Neurosci 2000; 
20:2664–72.

7. Ashmead DH, Wall RS, Ebinger KA, Eaton SB, Snook-Hill MM, 
Yang X. Spatial hearing in children with visual disabilities. 
Perception 1998; 27:105–22. doi: 10.1068/p270105. 

8. Lessard N, Paré M, Lepore F, Lassonde M. Early-blind human 
subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects. 
Nature 1998; 395:278–80. doi: 10.1038/26228.

9. Muchnik C, Efrati M, Nemeth E, Malin M, Hildesheimer M. 
Central auditory skills in blind and sighted subjects. Scand 
Audiol 1991; 20:19–23. doi: 10.3109/01050399109070785.

10. Wanet MC, Veraart C. Processing of auditory information by 
the blind in spatial localization tasks. Percept Psychophys 1985; 
38:91–6. doi: 10.3758/BF03202929.

11. Finocchietti S, Cappagli G, Gori M. Encoding audio motion: 
Spatial impairment in early blind individuals. Front Psychol 
2015; 6:1357. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01357.

12. Voss P, Lassonde M, Gougoux F, Fortin M, Guillemot JP, 
Lepore F. Early- and late-onset blind individuals show supra-
normal auditory abilities in far-space. Curr Biol 2004; 14:1734–8. 
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.051. 

13. Gori M, Sandini G, Martinoli C, Burr DC. Impairment of 
auditory spatial localization in congenitally blind human 
subjects. Brain 2014; 137:288–93. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt311. 

14. Thinus-Blanc C, Gaunet F. Representation of space in blind 
persons: Vision as a spatial sense? Psychol Bull 1997; 121:20–42. 
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.20.

15. Lewald J. Vertical sound localization in blind humans. 
Neuropsychologia 2002; 40:1868–72. doi: 10.1016/S0028-
3932(02)00071-4.

16. Voss P, Tabry V, Zatorre RJ. Trade-off in the sound localization 
abilities of early blind individuals between the horizontal and 
vertical planes. J Neurosci 2015; 35:6051–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEU 
ROSCI.4544-14.2015. 

17. Zwiers MP, Van Opstal AJ, Cruysberg JR. Two-dimensional 
sound-localization behavior of early-blind humans. Exp Brain 
Res 2001; 140:206–22. doi: 10.1007/s002210100800. 

18. Bernstein LR. Auditory processing of interaural timing 
information: New insights. J Neurosci Res 2001; 66:1035–46. 
doi: 10.1002/jnr.10103.

19. Seifritz E, Di Salle F, Esposito F, Herdener M, Neuhoff JG, 
Scheffler K. Enhancing BOLD response in the auditory system 
by neurophysiologically tuned fMRI sequence. Neuroimage 
2006; 29:1013–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.029. 

20. Engel SA, Rumelhart DE, Wandell BA, Lee AT, Glover GH, 
Chichilnisky EJ, et al. fMRI of human visual cortex. Nature 
1994; 369:525. doi: 10.1038/369525a0.

21. Hernández-Zamora E, Poblano A. [The auditory pathway: Levels 
of integration of information and principal neurotransmitters]. 
Gac Med Mex 2014; 150:450–60.

22. Rayleigh L. Our perception of the direction of a source of 
sound. Nature 1876; 14:32–3. doi: 10.1038/014032a0.

23. Shupak A, Sharoni Z, Yanir Y, Keynan Y, Alfie Y, Halpern P. 
Underwater hearing and sound localization with and without 
an air interface. Otol Neurotol 2005; 26:127–30.

24. Wightman FL, Kistler DJ. Factors affecting the relative salience 
of sound localization cues. In: Gilkey RH, Anderson TR, Eds. 
Binaural and Spatial Hearing in Real and Virtual Environments. 
New York, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc., 1997. 
Pp. 1–24.

25. Wells MJ, Ross HE. Distortion and adaptation in underwater 
sound localization. Aviat Space Environ Med 1980; 51:767–74.

26. Röder B, Teder-Sälejärvi W, Sterr A, Rösler F, Hillyard SA, 
Neville HJ. Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans. 
Nature 1999; 400:162–6. doi: 10.1038/22106.

27. Gougoux F, Zatorre RJ, Lassonde M, Voss P, Lepore F. A 
functional neuroimaging study of sound localization: Visual 
cortex activity predicts performance in early-blind individuals. 
PLoS Biol 2005; 3:e27. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030027.

28. Poirier C, Collignon O, Scheiber C, Renier L, Vanlierde A, Tranduy 
D, et al. Auditory motion perception activates visual motion 
areas in early blind subjects. Neuroimage 2006; 31:279–85. 
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.036. 

29. Lewald J. Exceptional ability of blind humans to hear sound 
motion: Implications for the emergence of auditory space. 
Neuropsychologia 2013; 51:181–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsycho 
logia.2012.11.017.

30. Lewald J, Meister IG, Weidemann J, Töpper R. Involvement 
of the superior temporal cortex and the occipital cortex 
in spatial hearing: Evidence from repetitive transcranial 
magnetic stimulation. J Cogn Neurosci 2004; 16:828–38. 
doi: 10.1162/089892904970834.

https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1913466
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0230
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0230
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02134.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02134.x
https://doi.org/10.1068/p270105
https://doi.org/10.1038/26228
https://doi.org/10.3109/01050399109070785
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202929
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01357
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.051
https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt311
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.20
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932%2802%2900071-4
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932%2802%2900071-4
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4544-14.2015
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4544-14.2015
https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210100800
https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.10103
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.029
https://doi.org/10.1038/369525a0
https://doi.org/10.1038/014032a0
https://doi.org/10.1038/22106
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030027
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.036
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.017
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.017
https://doi.org/10.1162/089892904970834