Theory and Practice of Second Language Acquisition 
vol. 4 (1) 2018, pp. 49–67

Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow
Pedagogical University of Cracow

Social Constraints of Aspirations 
for Second Language Achievement

A b s t r a c t

Educational aspirations are defined as “educational goals students set for themselves” 
(Trebbels, 2015, p. 37). They are widely studied in psychological and sociological research, in 
which it was found that, firstly, aspirations form in late adolescence and early adulthood, and 
secondly, their goal and level (i.e., high vs. low) are determined by the social environment 
they come from, that is, their family background, peer and school inf luence.

The paper presents the results of the qualitative study, in which 56 students of English 
philology in one of the vocational schools in the south of Poland expressed their aspirations 
in reference to their future foreign attainment and associated vocational goals. The results 
showed that in the majority of cases the aspirations are not so high and fully-formed, which, 
it is hypothesized, is rooted in the social background the students come from.

Keywords: foreign language, young adults, educational aspirations, social background

Introduction

In the poststructuralist framework to the study of SLA, it is emphasized 
that success in foreign language (FL) learning does not depend merely on 
the individual psychological traits of the learner, but on the social context in 
which learning takes place, such as the family, the school, the neighborhood, 
and even the socio-political situation of the country one lives in. Each of these 
environments determines educational opportunities and limitations, and thus 
may indirectly impact the learners’ educational decisions. This impact has 
long been studied in educational psychology (e.g., Marjoribanks, 2006; Spera, 
Wentzel, & Mato, 2009). It has been generally found that educational success is 
to a large extent dependant on the family environment in the early years, and 



Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow50

on peer and school influence in the adolescent years. This impact manifests in 
the arousal and directioning of educational aspirations, which, in turn, justify 
the effort undertaken towards the achievement of educational goals.

While many of the studies focus on the educational aspirations of middle-
class youth from big city environments, this one focuses on the aspirations of 
young people from a rather provincial and rural area. More precisely, it aims 
to investigate what goals and aspirations young adult learners of L2 English 
have towards their achievement, how they plan to utilize their L2 knowl-
edge for vocational purposes, and if their aspirations are dependant on social 
variables. 

Educational Aspirations — 
Defining the Concept and Their Determinants

Educational aspirations are defined as “educational goals an individual sets 
for himself/herself” (Fraser & Garg, 2017, p. 807). They can be measured as 
high, low or medium. The level of aspirations an individual possesses is par-
ticularly relevant for educational achievement, as it has been found in research 
(cf. Majoribanks, 2003; Farmer, 1985; Lewowicki, 1987) that individuals of 
high educational aspirations tend to achieve higher success in education, which 
in turn leads to better-paid occupational careers. If that causal relationship is 
true, one should be interested in boosting educational aspirations in adoles-
cents and young adults, as it is at this age that individuals decide about their 
vocations.

While possessing high/low educational aspirations can be related to a vari-
ety of personal characteristics (e.g., Skorny, 1980) such as experience of success 
and failure, level of intelligence, personality, level of neurosis, and perception of 
Self (cf. Galas & Lewowicki, 1991), more recent research focuses on the social/
environmental factors that contribute to the growth of aspirations, particularly 
to the role of family, and later in the adolescent years, of school, peers, and 
even wider educational policy. 

The influence of the family is seen as an interplay of proximal and distal 
characteristics. The former relate to socio-emotional and cognitive interactions 
among the family members. For example, it was found that children of parents 
presenting an authoritative parenting style characterized by support, emotional 
warmth, attention, and clear demands are more likely to achieve better results 
at school (Baumrind, 1989). Additionally, Rosenzweig (1989) identified seven 
parental practices which contribute to high school attainment: parental educa-
tional aspirations and grade expectations, parental engagement, authoritative 



Social Constraints of Aspirations for Second Language Achievement 51

parenting, autonomy support, emotional support, providing resources and learn-
ing experiences, and parental participation in school. These factors stem from 
the general family orientations towards upbringing and education and are less 
dependent or dependent indirectly on distal characteristics. 

The distal characteristics of the family include: socio-economic status (SES), 
ethnicity, availability of material resources, parental level of education, age of 
the parents, child gender, family size, living standards, place of living. The most 
commonly studied of these variables is SES, although its impact on the level of 
aspirations is not direct. On the one hand, higher-SES families can provide their 
children with better access to material resources, and can pay for additional/
private tuition. Higher-SES parents may also have a higher level of education 
and show better educational awareness, thus may hold higher educational as-
pirations. Yet, due to performing more time-consuming jobs, they may be less 
involved, or have less time to engage directly, in their children’s education. 
As a result, these parents may delegate the role of supporter to teachers, often 
in private school and private tutoring. By contrast, lower-SES families, not 
having such capabilities, may aspire for their children’s high educational and 
occupational achievement more directly by providing such cognitive stimula-
tion on their own.

These dependencies show that parents’ own educational aspirations can 
further be transferred onto children and contribute to the growth of the chil-
dren’s educational aspirations which normally manifest in late adolescence or 
early adulthood. It is unclear as to what is first in this cause-effect relation-
ship between aspirations and achievement: is it high educational aspirations 
which boost higher achievement, or is it high achievement caused by parental 
educational aspirations which boost high school achievement, where the feel-
ing of success contributes further to the growth of children’s own educational 
aspirations?

A model of growth of educational aspirations has been proposed by Garg 
et al. (2002), who take into account similar characteristics as above, that is, 
background, environmental and personal factors. The background factors would 
coincide with the distal factors as distinguished by Majoribanks (2003). The 
environmental factors refer to parental involvement, including their support and 
communication with school. These could partly overlap with proximal factors. 
The last element distinguished by Garg et al. (2002) is the personal factor, called 
academic Self-Schema, which is composed of self-perception of competence, 
school achievement, attitudes toward education and work, and extracurricular 
activities such as reading at home. According to the authors, students who 
have a positive Self-Schema lay more trust in their learning abilities and find 
learning more enjoyable. I believe this concept is related to what is known 
in motivational theories as self-efficacy beliefs. Both of these concepts may 
be an outcome of inborn traits as well as parental support and prior learning 



Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow52

experiences, thus there is a great role for schooling institutions to support and 
not discourage learning.

Finally, it has to be noted that research on educational aspirations is mark-
edly boosted in liberal and democratic societies, where there is an opportunity 
to move up the social mobility ladder, and where education is considered to 
be one of the keys to it. High educational aspirations are less likely to arise in 
communities which have a fairly established social structure, and as a result, 
little chance to change social position. For this reason, research on educational 
aspirations has been particularly strong in Australia (Majoribanks, 2003, 2006), 
especially among groups of immigrants, and in the USA (Spera, Wentzel, & 
Mato, 2009), predominatly in the 1970s and 1980s. Research conducted in 
more recent years, that is, from 2000 onwards, has observed that in times of 
neoliberal economy, it is more difficult to guarantee mobility merely through 
education. As the number of university graduates has grown, the number of 
positions for which educated people are required has not. The competition for 
occupational positions has kept the salaries stable, or has even seen salaries 
lowered, thus it is hard to say that high educational achievement leads to 
higher vocational positions and social mobility. As Devine (2004), working in 
the British context, noticed, it was much easier to achieve advancement in the 
1960s when the post-war generation had grown-up as there were more places 
to be filled by educated labor than nowadays. Yet, previous advancement oc-
curred not so much due to removing the elite from the privileged positions, 
but due to a higher demand for skilled professionals and more space at the top. 
This shows that aspirations are more likely to arise in the times of sociological 
and historical change. 

For similar reasons, there is a well-established tradition for psychological 
and sociological research on aspirations in the Polish setting. Already in com-
munist times of the 1970s and 1980s, individuals who were willing to com-
plete a higher education usually obtained jobs of higher prestige, although not 
necessarily much better paid due to the official policy of equality (Janowski, 
1977; Lewowicki, 1987; Domański, 2007). Another historical moment which 
precipitated the growth of aspirations, and particularly the need for foreign 
language competence, was the fall of communism. This was the time when the 
borders opened, thus giving ground for foreign investments. Individuals who 
knew any FLs made astonishing careers in foreign companies, even without 
formal qualifications for which they made up later on. The 1990s was the period 
when a variety of foreign languages was learnt and required in the job market. 
However, with the growth of popularity of English as a global language, this 
yearning for multilinguality diminished. English dominated the educational 
and job market, not least since Poland joined the EU in 2004. A similar shift 
in FL motivation has been noticed in Hungary, which passed through similar 
historical moments (cf. Dörnyei, Csizer, & Nemeth, 2006).



Social Constraints of Aspirations for Second Language Achievement 53

Joining the EU has given many a chance for personal and professional 
mobility for which languages are necessary, both for communication and func-
tioning in the job market. Thus it seems young people should be willing and 
motivated to learn foreign languages, especially lingua franca English. Thanks 
to it they have a chance to move upwards either in the social or occupational 
structure.

Aspirations and SLA

In SLA theory, aspirations have received more attention in theories of lan-
guage learning motivation, originating in psychology. One of the major theories 
within achievement motivation theory is attribution theory. Attribution theory 
deals with individuals’ perceptions of their causality and their understanding 
of cause-effect relationships between various phenomena. It also identifies 
relationships between a person’s perceptions of causality and their behavioral 
tendency in the future, especially in reference to achievement motivation and 
the level of aspirations (Weiner, 1972). So the attribution theory has utilized 
the concept of experience in formulating individuals’ aspirations.

The theory claims that people of higher achievement motivation are more 
likely to attribute their success or failure to their own work and effort. What 
is more, past experience of a similar type positively influences a person’s 
orientation towards goals of similar type. By contrast, people of low achieve-
ment motivation ascribe their success or failure to pure luck and coincidence, 
or lack of ability (in the case of failure). This causative relationship can lie at 
the bottom of the formation of aspirations, as only persons who feel that they 
have influence over their success or failure in the future can formulate stronger 
aspirations. Weiner (1972) introduced the term locus of control to describe an 
individual’s power to direct one’s behavior. The locus is the place where an 
individual places this power in the continuum from objectivity to subjectiv-
ity, and can be dependent on the person’s volition, intelligence, ability, or on 
external influences.

Aspirations are also regarded as an important component of Ideal L2 Self, 
that is, a Motivational L2 Self Theory proposed by Dörnyei (2009), as they set 
goals for learning. In further development of the theory Dörnyei (2014, 2015), 
stated that what motivates learners for the learning activity are their visions 
of future achievement, which, to my belief, coincides with aspirations. Thus 
imagination plays an important role in establishing the vision/goal.

Finally, it must be mentioned that no other studies have dealt with young 
people’s aspirations so far. Some common ground can be found in the studies 



Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow54

conducted by Mystkowska-Wiertelak and Pawlak (2017) on willingness to com-
municate from the macro- (i.e., as a general learner feature) and micro- (i.e., 
as a class factor) perspective. The studies were conducted among students of 
English philology at the Higher Vocational School in Konin with a view to 
verifying the factors that make some students more willing to communicate 
than others. Having conducted a number of studies, the authors have proposed 
a model of interrelated factors influencing WTC. According to them, the 
variables are: communication confidence, ought-to self, classroom environ-
ment, international posture—openness to experience, unplanned in-class WTC, 
international posture—interest in international affairs, practice-seeking WTC, 
and planned in-class WTC. Of these factors, WTC correlated the most with 
the two dimensions of International posture, which indicates that learners who 
are more willing to learn and use the language are those who see its utility in 
the wider world. This stance, I believe, is rooted in prior experience and social 
background, and these learners have been found to seek more practice in out-
of-class situations. The classroom environment was found to be conducive to 
WTC only if foreign-exchange students were present in class, which indicates 
that the classroom environment generates fewer communicative activities.

This study, focusing only on WTC, has omitted social reasons due to which 
learners may be motivated towards learning a language and believe in achiev-
ing the final goal.

While the aforementioned study focused on WTC, the following paper 
aims to verify the general motivation towards L2 achievement, which could 
be called the willingness to study. The common feature of both studies is the 
fact that a learner willing to obtain communicative competence in a FL must 
be motivated and willing to undertake the effort to study the language, and 
to actively seek opportunities to use it both in class and in the outside world. 

The Study

The following study aims to investigate what are the goals and aspirations 
for L2 achievement of young adults. It is assumed that in early adulthood as-
pirations of young people coincide with vocational aspirations, as young adults 
are at the threshold of making serious decisions regarding their future. Their 
vocational goals determine their educational decisions. It is also assumed that 
the vision of their future attainment is what can motivate them to undertake 
a serious effort in pursuing the goal. More precisely, it is investigated why 
young adults choose to study English philology in a vocational school and how 
this relates to their vocational aspirations.



Social Constraints of Aspirations for Second Language Achievement 55

The following research questions have been addressed:
1. What goals and aspirations do young adult learners of L2 English have 

towards their FL achievement?
2. How do they plan to utilize their L2 knowledge for vocational purposes?
3. Are their aspirations dependant on social variables? If so, which ones?
4. What do they ascribe their FLL success to, that is, to what situations, events, 

factors, people?
The social variables taken into account are: socio-economic status (SES)-

measured as a self-perceived feature on a 7-point scale (1–poor, 7–excel-
lent), parental level of education (1–primary, 5–doctorate level), family size, 
parental level of FL knowledge. In reference to the last variable, it is be-
lieved that those parents who have some FL knowledge can support their 
children in FL learning by either directly helping with the retention of 
class material, or by setting positive models and creating opportunities for 
L2 use.

The Instrument

The instrument was a survey which consisted of seven open-ended ques-
tions inquiring about factors, situations and life events which influenced their 
current FL knowledge, beliefs about factors contributing to/inhibiting success, 
visions of Self and their FL ability in five years’ time, a scale about self-efficacy 
beliefs, which was to provide information about their locus of control (in their 
ability of external factors).

Additionally, the demographic information about self-assessed L2 profi-
ciency, length of learning, gender, age of the learner, as well as their parental 
level of education, occupation and level of FL knowledge was collected, and 
the size of family and self-perceived SES measured on the self-assessment scale 
(1–very bad, 7–very good).

The study was qualitative in nature, yet the data will be presented in refer-
ence to the social variables.

Participants

The participants of the study were 63 students in their 2nd year of English 
philology at the Higher Vocational School in Nowy Targ. There were: 15 males 
and 48 females. Their mean age was: 20.6. There were 33 students in the teach-
ing specialization group (coded as S1–S33), and 30 students who had chosen 
the translation specialization (codes S34–63). In the teacher specialization, there 
was only one male student.



Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow56

At this point, it should be mentioned that the region where the school is 
located differs from others in the sense that traditionally, it has been a place 
where families were quite big (had many children) and where it was common 
to emigrate to the USA. Thus, there is a long-standing need and willingness to 
learn English for utilitarian purposes as this language enables communication 
and higher earnings abroad.

Also, what makes this study different is the fact that it is the students in 
big city institutions that are usually investigated/studied, who by getting a place 
at a prestigious university already show a certain degree of success and ability. 
The participants in this study come from lower SES families, a group which 
is quite underrepresented in research.

Results

The following section presents the results of the study, first presenting the 
quantitative data for the synthesis of the findings, and then illustrating the key 
points with qualitative remarks.

As regards their proficiency level, the students assessed their ability at either 
B2 (N = 34) or B1 level (N = 28), most probably referring to their results at 
the Matura exam. Only one person self-reported a C1 level. This level of FL 
knowledge allows communicative use, yet still requires further development. It 
is notable to say that the students have studied L2 English for a relatively long 
time (Mean length of study: 11.2 years), which indicates they have mainly learnt 
the language throughout the schooling years, however, not always successfully, 
as the attainment of B1 level shows. Surprisingly, more students at B1 level 
were in the teaching specialization group, while in the translation section there 
were more students at B2 level.

Table 1 below shows the types of aspirations as identified in the qualitative 
analysis together with the quantity of their appearance.

In regard to the students’ aspirations referring to their FL attainment and 
vocational attainment, it can be seen that the learning goals are quite varied. 
Nearly half of the students are language aficionados, as they either aim for 
achieving a native-like level of English (25%) or wish to learn other foreign 
languages (22%). Thirteen students (21%) would like to be teachers and eight 
students (13%) would like to be translators, while ten students (16%) would 
like to emigrate.

It seems that to a certain extent their vocational plans are connected with 
the choice of specialization. The desire to become teachers has been voiced 
by almost all students in that specialization (88%), whereas in the translation 
section, vocational plans seem to be more ambiguous. This is manifested in the 
vast array of responses showing plans which seem to remain in the sphere of 



Social Constraints of Aspirations for Second Language Achievement 57

dreams rather than being fully materialized plans (positions 8–20). Apparently, 
the choice of this specialization was dictated by the lack of willingness to be-
come a teacher on the one hand, and the desire to obtain additional time before 
making a final decision on the other.

Table 1 
Goals of studying English and future vocational/educational aspirations 
(N = 63)

Aspiration type Count
Percentage 

score
%

1. Achieving native-like level 16 25

2. Learning other languages (Italian, Russian, German, Spanish) 14 22

3. Work as a teacher 13 21

4. Going/working abroad 10 16

5. Work as a translator in a firm/sworn translator 8 13

6. Working with languages 6 10

7. Emigrate to an EU country/Australia/theUSA 5 8

8. Interpersonal communication 6 10

9. Open my own business (language school/kindergarten/tourist 
agency)

6 10

10. Working in tourism (as a guide) 4 6

11. Travelling 4 6

12. Learning L2 culture 2 3

13. Working in a corporation 1 2

14. Giving private tution 1 2

15. Study further and work 1 2

16. Work as a journalist 1 2

17. Combine language with studying another field 1 2

18. Translating books 1 2

19. Helping a family member to learn 1 2

20. Working in a consulate 1 2

*The total number of aspirations exceeds the number of respondents as more than one aspiration was expressed.

This tendency can be further endorsed by explicitly stated student aspira-
tions. A student from the teaching specialization clearly knows what she would 
like to do in the future, saying,



Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow58

(S1, B2 level): I would like to teach young children so as to give them the 
best opportunities and make them speak fluently from the earliest years. 
I would like to speak the language fluently, just like in Polish.

From this statement, it can be seen that the student perceives a teaching pro-
fession as rewarding, and the student’s choice of this career is deliberate with 
high aspirations for her future learners’ achievement.

Whereas the students from the translation specialization group say,

(S 46) Foreign language learning is a pleasure for me; I would like to 
combine this (hobby) with my future profession. I hope that my language 
level in 5 years will be advanced. 
(S 59) I would like to master English to the degree that would enable me 
free conversation, however, I am trying not to look too much into the future, 
and so I have no plans. 
(S 45) It is difficult to say for the time being as I am not planning my future 
yet. However, I would like to work, maybe as a translator. In that situation 
I would use the language every day during my work. 
(S 56) I am going to continue learning L2 in my own way so that it is 
pleasant and motivating towards the goal. Nothing enforced. 

These students supposedly enjoy learning English, but as they reported 
elsewhere in the questionnaire, they learnt it to a large extent effortlessly, often 
by playing online games or using the Internet. It might be hypothesized that if 
students acquired language to a large extent implicitly, they are less familiar-
ized with effective studying techniques, but may have willingness or interest 
to learn. Those who acquired it through conscientious study might have taken 
liking to it, and consequently, would like to pass their interest and competence 
to others via the teaching profession. 

Additionally, due to the fact that the students have unclearly specified vo-
cational goals, the students may be less knowledgeable or aware of the ways 
which help to pursue them. Table 2 shows a summary of the students’ beliefs 
as to what leads to success in a FL. 

As can be seen, about one third of the students (33%) believe that sys-
tematic learning/working hard is sufficient to realize their learning and voca-
tional goals. What is more, nine students (14%) believe that graduation from 
English studies will help to attain these goals. Another five students believe 
that the key to success is enrolling in additional FL courses. These choices 
were supported by 53% of the students, and indicated their belief in external 
factors aiding the process of a FL. The opinions relating to students’ autono-
mous learning were solitary, and denoted such aspects of learning as: going 
abroad temporarily (14%), looking for an opportunity to communicate (8%), 



Social Constraints of Aspirations for Second Language Achievement 59

reading in English (6%), and watching films (6%). Furthermore, there were 
only singular innovative ideas about ways of improving their FL command, 
such as learning words and structures daily (3 persons) or even finding a job 
where English is required (2 persons) as a means of improving their com-
mand. Clearly, these students felt that the best way to improve language skills 
is to use it frequently (e.g., as when abroad) and for communicative purposes 
(e.g., at work). It is quite surprising that the learning opportunities provided 
through various tools on the Internet remained unnoticed. Only one person 
pointed to the possibility of using various social networking sites as a means of 
using a FL.

Table 2 
Ways of pursuing the aspirations (N = 63)

Ways of pursuing the aspirations Count
Percentage 

score
%

1. Systematic learning / working hard 21 33

2. Graduate from English studies 9 14

3. Going abroad for a while 9 14

4. Looking for an opportunity to communicate 5 8

5. Enroll on FL courses 5 8

6. Reading in English 4 6

7. Watching films 4 6

8. Learning words and structures daily 3 5

9. Study more on my own 2 3

10. Find a job where English is required 2 3

11. Revising L2 material 2 3

12. Study more on my own 2 3

13. Reward myself for achievements 1 2

14. Increase L2 contact 1 2

15. Pursue interests 1 2

Most of the ways of pursuing aspirations relate to formal instruction which 
may also indicate the locus of control is placed outside the learner. This further 
indicates the learners feel less responsible for their own achievements, do not 
believe in their self-efficacy, and delegate the responsibility for achievement to 
their teachers. Thus it may be speculated the learners are not autonomous and 
cannot pose clear goals themselves. Consequently, learning a foreign language 
seems for them to be an infinite process.



Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow60

One could wonder what the reasons for this situation might be: the long-
lasting experience of authoritarian education which does not foster autonomy, 
or lack of out-of-class experiences for learning the language. Indeed, in the 
times of the Internet and availability of online resources, the lack of its use is 
surprising. One would expect greater autonomy and a clear pursuit of the goals 
by university-level students. 

The use of metacognitive or affective strategies which would indicate 
one’s autonomy is also scarce. Single metacognitive and affective strategies 
are mentioned as indicated in statements number 9, 11, 12, 13; however, they 
were reported only by a few students. This finding may indicate that despite 
many years of school education, the students still do not know how to learn 
a language.

Additionally, it was observed that learners who have already achieved 
a higher level of L2 proficiency (B2) had more concrete visions of what they 
would like to achieve, and had more goals and more ideas of how to pursue 
these goals. Similarly, respondents of the teacher specialization group had 
more accurate plans (students 1–33) than students of the translation group. 
It may be thus concluded that those who chose a teaching specialization had 
clearer language learning goals at the start of the study in a higher education 
institution while those who chose the translation specialization were looking 
to gain additional time before making a final vocational decision. This stand-
point can be illustrated by statements of selected students of the translation 
group: 

(S 39) I am planning to complete BA and MA studies. If this does not teach 
me the language, nothing will, not even working abroad. 

(S 61) First I would like to learn English perfectly and then go abroad, so 
as to use it skillfully. 

From such statements it is evident that the learners have little knowledge 
of language learning processes and that they allocate teaching-learning effec-
tiveness to educational institutions rather than holding it in their own hands. 
This finding is quite surprising, as it is already known from research in 
language pedagogy that what is most conducive to language learning success 
is going abroad and intensive communicative language use (Muñoz, 2012), 
and not school instruction. The role of implicit language acquisition remains 
unnoticed. This may be due to the fact that contrary to the initial assumption, 
not many students in the cohort studied have the experience of living/travel-
ling abroad (only one person was born in the US). As it was found from other 
responses, in this environment, the goal of many of those young people is to 
emigrate and stay abroad for good. Therefore, language learning is for these 



Social Constraints of Aspirations for Second Language Achievement 61

young people a tool which will help them realize an important life goal. They 
learn the language in order to go abroad, and not use an opportunity to stay 
abroad to improve language skills. Language learning has a purely instrumental 
function. It is also possible that the perception of available life choices is limited. 
The students may observe their own cognitive and social constraints which do 
not let them to either dedicate a sufficient amount of time to the study, or use 
extracurricular resources. As the following student says, 

(S 41) When I entered the university I thought I would become a world-
famous translator, but now when I understood how difficult it is, I decided 
to learn the language in order to go abroad. With FL knowledge, it will be 
easier for me to get a well-paid job, and not e.g. of a cleaner. 

A student from a low socio-economic background says:

(S 37) I spend too much time helping my parents on a farm, and when 
I have time, I often do everything to put it off. I have poor concentration, 
I forget about motives that direct me towards FLL.

This finding leads us to answering the third research question, which aimed 
to identify social variables which might determine these rather low educational 
and vocational aspirations.

In a vast number of sociological studies, SES is measured as an important 
social variable influencing L2 outcomes as it enables access to a better quality 
of education, learning materials, cultural resources, etc. In this study, however, 
the SES has not turned out to be a significant variable. The mean score was 
5.6, which denotes a quite high level of satisfaction with one’s socio-economic 
status (on a 7-point scale) and indicates that none of them felt any financial 
inadequacies. For this reason, on the basis of this scale, it was difficult to sin-
gle out higher or lower SES students. The reason for that might be that SES 
was measured as a self-assessed feature, and therefore the respondents may not 
have been willing to reveal their true SES as it is a delicate issue. Secondly, 
it is possible that the students’ perception of their status is quite positive as 
there are no huge disparities in the level of income among the students, and 
the sense of well-being is always measured against others. Therefore, SES de-
fined as a self-perceived feature has not turned out to be a sufficient/objective 
descriptor of the ability.

However, when we look at other distal variables, such as parental level 
of education and parental FL knowledge, they can tell us more about the 
SES of the learners. In the cohort studied, practically no parent knew a FL 
at a high level (B2). Only in one case both parents were reported to speak 
English at an intermediate level (B1). In all other cases the parents were re-



Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow62

ported to have no knowledge or very little (only rudimentary) knowledge of 
a FL (Mean: 0.8 for mothers and 0.4 for fathers). The same can be observed 
in reference to parental education (Mean 2.7 on a 5 point scale). The major-
ity of parents had a vocational or intermediate level of education. There were 
only a few parents, and these were usually mothers, who had a higher level 
of education.

This data indicates that having taken into account the distal factors of the 
family environments from which the students came, the group studied was rath-
er homogenous, that is, they came from low socio-economic families in which 
parents had a lower level of education and no, or only a rudimentary level of, 
FL knowledge. Yet, it is known from other research in educational psychology 
that in postmodern society, it is particularly the parental level of education and 
the type of occupation that define the SES of the family. Erikson, Goldthorpe, 
and Portocarero (1979) developed scales which describe social structure in post-
modern society according to occupations. According to them, the occupations 
which guarantee the highest SES are: intelligentsia, top corporate management, 
and large-scale businessman, referred to as service class, while the occupations 
which denote lower SES are: skilled workers, unqualified manual workers, rural 
workers, and farm owners. Taking this classification into account, it can be 
judged that the participants come from lower SES backgrounds. Furthermore, 
parents of the students studied had little knowledge of a FL themselves, thus 
were unable to help their children in language learning at school, and/or inspire 
them for autonomous language development. It may be, therefore, concluded 
that the impact of low linguistic/cultural capital is evident in the fact that the 
majority of students ascribe their language success to the teacher and school, 
or to staying abroad, which, however, was undertaken for work, and not leisure 
purposes. This view is further illustrated by factors to which the students at-
tributed their current FLL level (Table 3).

As can be seen in Table 3, approximately one third of the students ascribe 
their current FL success to visits/staying abroad. Yet, as is known from previous 
analysis, this experience of living/working abroad has not turned out profitable 
to all of the students. Despite this opportunity, many still believed that attend-
ing formal instruction is necessary (cf. Table 1). The reason for this may be 
twofold: the amount of contact with a FL may have been varied and therefore 
not everyone profited with high proficiency. Secondly, it may have been the 
case that some of the students first had a FL experience, and having realized 
their deficiencies, opted for more systematic formal instruction. As one of the 
students confessed,

(S 21) In order to improve my language skills I enrolled to the language 
studies. In my opinion this has increased my liberty at using English. It’s 
a key element that has influenced my knowledge of English. 



Social Constraints of Aspirations for Second Language Achievement 63

Table 3.
Student attributions to FL competence

Sources of language learning success Count Percent

1. Visits abroad; opportunity to practice L2 18 29

2. Watching cartoons/films/TV series 10 16

3. Willingness to communicate with foreigners/foreign friends 6 10

4. Choice of the subject of study/profile class 6 10

5. Work (e.g., restaurants) with an opportunity to speak L2/work abroad 6 10

6. Total time of learning spent at school 6 10

7. Interest in the humanities/languages 5 8

8. School success (in comparison to other school subjects) 5 8

9. Listening to L2 songs 4 6

10. Playing computer games in L2 4 6

11. Private tuition 4 6

12. Reading in English 3 5

13. Early start in a FL 3 5

14. The teacher (bringing additional materials) 3 5

15. Willingness to get to know other cultures 3 5

16. Communication in the Internet 2 3

17. Very good teachers in primary school (raising motivation) 2 3

18. Extended program of FL at school (additional classes) 2 3

19. Living in the English-speaking country 2 3

20. Practice at speaking with foreigners 2 3

21. Exchange programmes with schools from abroad 1 2

22. Requirement to take a FL at school exams 1 2

23. Participation in EU progams (free courses) 1 2

24. Help of a family member 1 2

25. Self-study 1 2

26. Attending language courses 1 2

27. Parents’ motivation 1 2

28. *Lack of opportunity to speak L2 with foreigners 1 2

29. Hard work in preparing for exams/tests 1 2

30. Internship abroad 1 2

31. Ambition to improve (due to poor school results) 1 2



Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow64

Another group of students are those who may not have had a chance of 
going abroad but found opportunities of learning the language in the home 
country, arousing their own interest in languages and looking for contacts with 
foreigners (points 2, 3, 5). However, such self-motivated students constitute only 
about 10% of the whole cohort. Student 29 illustrates this type of experience 
best:

(S 29) I have been always interested in the English language (mainly its 
melody). I began learning subconsciously with listening to English songs 
and printing out the lyrics so as to know precisely what they mean. The 
other factors that influenced my FLL are the willingness to communicate 
with foreign friends; I have always admired people who could speak this 
language fluently (teachers, translators), and looked up to them. 

Yet another group of students are those who were successful at language learn-
ing at school and therefore chose it as a subject of university study. They at-
tended a special profile class with an extended program of English (position 4), 
or they ascribe it to their overall time spent at school and relative success in 
language learning as compared to learning other subjects. All other events took 
place sporadically as they were mentioned by individual students.

Discussion

The above study showed that not all young adults, despite studying in 
a higher education institution and having chosen a prestigious subject to study 
(English philology), have high educational and vocational aspirations. Only 25% 
point out to clear language learning goals, like achieving native-like competence 
or learning other FLs (24%). As regards the choice of career, only about one 
third of the students are positive about their future occupational careers, such 
as the teaching (21%) or the translation profession (13%).

These findings are quite surprising as in socio-pedagogical literature, young 
adults, when choosing higher educational institutions, have already well crystal-
lized goals. Yet, the above findings negate this fact and show rather low, or no, 
educational and vocational aspirations. This finds reflection in a rather relaxed 
approach to studying in general, and a lack of study skills.

One reason for that fact may be that the students studied have low aca-
demic self-schema/self-efficacy beliefs due to low parental involvement at ear-
lier stages of education, and often low language achievement (B1 level). This 
may denote also that they come from families of low cultural and linguistic 



Social Constraints of Aspirations for Second Language Achievement 65

capital. Thus the FL level they have achieved so far can, to a large extent, be 
attributed to effortless acquisition rather than to a deliberate plan and invest-
ment or conscientious study. The reason they chose to study a FL was probably 
caused by the fact that they found it easy and enjoyable to learn the language 
in school times.

Another reason, which may overlap with the former, is the fact that in times 
of crisis of neoliberal economy, young people may be less positive about their 
future and hesitant about what profession to choose. There are few stable jobs 
available and a higher level of education no longer guarantees access to secure 
and better-paid jobs. Consequently, young people additionally opt for ‘wait 
time.’ The availability of a free higher education institution in their neighbor-
hood allows them to obtain a diploma, and possibly to gain some academic and/
or professional skills which may turn out useful in the job market. But first and 
foremost, it allows them to delay making decisions about their vocational future. 
This often results, in Brzezińska’s words, “delayed adolescence” (Brzezińska, 
Ziółkowska, & Appelt, 2016). Young people have a chance to prolong their 
carefree years and postpone the time of making crucial life decisions, such as 
starting up a family or setting on a career path, while having a good excuse of 
study. It can be further speculated that had a higher education institution not 
been freely available in their environment, these young people would not have 
chosen to study and would have started to work earlier, possibly in some manual 
jobs. This provides the justification for the existence of such higher vocational 
schools in rural areas. The role of educational institutions is to make up for 
the deficiencies in cultural capital that a family has not been able to cater to, 
and provide learning opportunities for ambitious individuals. 

On the other hand, it is speculated that those young adults who have higher 
educational and vocational aspirations, if only they can afford it, decide to study 
in bigger cities, where more educational and vocational opportunities can be 
found. It is also possible that higher educational aspirations are characteristic of 
gifted learners who have internally placed locus of control and who thus feel 
more responsible for their learning, and set clearer goals. This issue, however, 
would require further investigation.

Conclusion

The above study was carried out in a specific social environment, char-
acterized by low socio-economic status, low level of parental education, 
including FL knowledge. These facts denote that learners can rely on the 
family environment for educational support to a lesser extent than learners 



Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow66

from big city schools, where they receive more support from both parents 
and peers. Ambitious adolescents coming from less favorable environments 
can always look up to their privileged friends, aspire for similar status and 
seek ways for its achievement.

The homogeneity of the group studied is counterproductive as the young 
adults cannot learn about other career paths or ways to achieve them as they 
are all in the same boat. Emigration seems to be the only available choice in 
improving one’s socio-economic status. For this reason, the role of school and 
teachers in awakening educational aspirations, as well as showing means of their 
realization is even more prominent in rural areas, in a considerable distance 
from larger educational centers. In a wider socio-political scale, there should 
be supportive education policies, for example via grants, which enable young 
people to pursue their aspirations.

References

Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today 
and tomorrow (pp. 349–378). San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass.

Brzezińska, A., Ziółkowska, B., & Appelt, K. (2016). Psychologia rozwoju człowieka. 
[Psychology of human development]. Gdańsk: GWP.

Devine, F. (2004). Class practices. How parents help their children get good jobs. Cambridge: 
Cambridge University Press.

Domański, H. (2007). Struktura społeczna [Social structure]. Warszawa: Scholar.
Domański, H., Sawiński, Z., & Słomczyński K. (2007). Nowa klasyfikacja i skale zawodów. 

Socjologiczne wskaźniki pozycji społecznej w Polsce [A new classification and scales of 
professions. Sociological indicators of social position in Poland]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo 
Instytutu Filozofii i Socjologii Polskiej Akademii Nauk.

Dörnyei, Z., & Ryan, S. (2015). The psychology of the language learner revisited. New York: 
Routledge.

Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The L2 Motivational Self System. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.), 
Motivation, language identity and the L2 self (pp. 9–42). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

Dörnyei, Z., & Kubanyiova, M. (2014). Motivating learners, motivating teachers: Building vision 
in language education. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dörnyei, Z., Csizér, K., & Németh, N. (2006). Motivation, language attitudes and globalisation: 
A Hungarian perspective. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.

Farmer, H. S. (1985). Model of career and achievement motivation for women and men. Journal 
of Counseling Psychology, 32, 363–390.

Fraser, M., & Garg, R. (2014). Educational aspirations. In R. J. R. Levesque (Ed.), Encyclopaedia 
of adolescence (pp. 807–812). New York: Springer. DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4419-1695-2_147

Galas, B., & Lewowicki, T. (1991). Osobowość a aspiracje [Personality and aspirations]. 
Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego.

Garg, R., Kauppi, C., Lewko, J., & Urajnik, D. (2002). A structural model of educational aspira-
tions. Journal of Career Development, 29(2), 87–108.



Social Constraints of Aspirations for Second Language Achievement 67

Janowski, A. (1977). Aspiracje młodzieży szkół średnich [Aspirations of secondary school youth]. 
Warszawa: PWN.

Lewowicki, T. (1987). Aspiracje dzieci i młodzieży [Aspirations of children and youth]. 
Warszawa: PWN.

Marjoribanks, K. (2006). Family environments and children’s outcomes. In K. Wheldall (Ed.), 
Developments in educational psychology. How far have we come in 25 years? (pp. 75–85). 
London: Routledge.

Marjoribanks, K. (2003). Family background, individual and environmental inf luences, aspira-
tions and young adult’s educational attainment: A follow–up study. Educational Studies, 
29, 233–242.

Muñoz, C. (Ed). Intensive exposure experiences in second language learning. Bristol: 
Multilingual Matters.

Mystkowska-Wiertelak, A., & Pawlak, M. (2017). Willingness to communicate in instructed 
second language acquisition. Combining a macro- and micro- perspective. Bristol: 
Multilingual Matters.

Rosenzweig, C. J. (2000). A meta-analysis of parenting and school success: The role of parents 
in promoting students’ academic performance. Dissertation Abstract International Section 
A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 61(4A), 1636.

Skorny, Z. (Ed.) (1980). Mechanizmy funkcjonowania aspiracji [Mechanisms of aspirations]. 
Wrocław: Ossolineum.

Spera, Ch., Wentzel, K. R., & Matto, H. C. (2009). Parental aspirations for their children’s 
educational attainment: Relations to ethnicity, parental education, children’s academic 
performance, and parental perceptions of school climate. Journal of Youth Adolescence, 
38, 1140–1152.

Weiner, B. (1972). Attribution theory, achievement motivation and the educational process. 
Review of Educational Research, 42, 2-3-215.

Joanna Rokita-Jaśkow

Soziale Begrenzungen der Bildungsaspirationen beim Zweitspracherwerb

Z u s a m m e n f a s s u n g

Die Bildungsaspirationen werden meistens definiert als „Bildungsziele, die sich der 
Lernende setzt“ (Trebbels, 2015:37). Diese werden häufig zum Gegenstand der psychologischen 
und soziologischen Forschungen, in Folge deren es u.a. festgestellt wurde, dass Aspirationen 
auf Bildung in späten Jugendjahren und /oder im frühen Erwachsenenalter entstehen und deren 
Ausrichtung und Niveau (hohes oder niedriges) durch soziale Umgebung des Lernenden (d.i. 
seine Familie, Altersgenossen u. Schule) gestaltet werden. 

Der vorliegende Beitrag präsentiert die Ergebnisse der Qualitätsforschung, die unter 56 
Anglistikstudenten einer der Berufsfachschulen in Südpolen durchgeführt wurde. Sie bezweck-
te, die Bildungsaspirationen der Studenten auf Englischerwerbung und die damit verbundenen 
Berufsaspirationen zu untersuchen. Die Ergebnisse zeugen davon, dass diese Aspirationen in 
den meisten Fällen nicht zu hoch sind, was mit sozialer Herkunft der Studenten begründet 
werden kann. 

Schlüsselwörter: Fremdsprache, Jugend, Bildungsaspirationen, soziale Herkunft